Justin Kruger’s research while affiliated with University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (49)


How unaware are the unskilled? Empirical tests of the “signal extraction” counterexplanation for the Dunning–Kruger effect in self-evaluation of performance
  • Article

December 2013

·

1,178 Reads

·

198 Citations

Journal of Economic Psychology

·

·

Kerri L. Johnson

·

Justin Kruger

Previous work on the Dunning–Kruger effect has shown that poor performers often show little insight into the shortcomings in their performance, presumably because they suffer a double curse. Deficits in their knowledge prevent them from both producing correct responses and recognizing that the responses they produce are inferior to those produced by others. Krajč and Ortmann (2008) offered a different account, suggesting instead that poor performers make performance estimates with no more error than top performers. Floor effects, coupled with the assumption of a backwards-J performance distribution, force their self-evaluations errors to be frequently positive in nature. Krajč and Ortmann, however, offered no empirical data to test their “signal extraction” account. In three studies, we assessed their theoretical model by examining whether (1) the data producing the Dunning–Kruger effect fit the statistical assumptions considered by Krajč and Ortmann necessary to produce it, and (2) to see if their framework reproduced Dunning–Kruger errors in a data set that fit their statistical assumptions. We found that the Krajč–Ortmann framework failed to anticipate self-evaluative misperceptions on the part of poor performers, but that it does much better at accounting for misperceptions among top performers. Paradoxically, the model suggests that Kruger and Dunning (1999) may have underestimated the accuracy of top performers, even though their account asserts such accuracy.


Figure 1. Relationship between actual and perceived performance in percentile terms for the Wason task (Studies 1, 4, 5, and 6). Aggregate refers to an unweighted average of predicted values across the four studies.
Figure 2. Relationship between actual and perceived performance on absolute measures for the Wason task (Studies 1, 4, 5, and 6). Aggregate refers to an unweighted average of predicted values across the three or four studies. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Table 7 Consistency and Self-Evaluations of Participants Who Stated They Did or Did Not Follow an Explicit Rule in Study 4 Measure Followed explicit rule t Yes No
Figure 11. Mediational analysis of the link between decision consistency and favorability of self-evaluation of performance, with neglect of alternatives as the mediator. A. R 2 Used as measure of consistency. B. Self-reported endorsement of rule used as measure of consistency (Study 6).  
Table 11 Correlations of Consideration of Alternatives (Study 6) With Consistency, Self-Evaluation Measures, and Objective Performance Measure Consideration of alternatives 

+2

The Hobgoblin of Consistency: Algorithmic Judgment Strategies Underlie Inflated Self-Assessments of Performance
  • Article
  • Full-text available

April 2013

·

1,337 Reads

·

39 Citations

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

People often hold inflated views of their performance on intellectual tasks, with poor performers exhibiting the most inflation. What leads to such excessive confidence? We suggest that the more people approach such tasks in a "rational" (i.e., consistent, algorithmic) manner, relative to those who use more variable or ad hoc approaches, the more confident they become, irrespective of whether they are reaching correct judgments. In 6 studies, participants completed tests involving logical reasoning, intuitive physics, or financial investment. Those more consistent in their approach to the task rated their performances more positively, including those consistently pursuing the wrong rule. Indeed, completely consistent but wrong participants thought almost as highly of their performance as did completely consistent and correct participants. Participants were largely aware of the rules they followed and became more confident in their performance when induced to be more systematic in their approach, no matter how misguided that approach was. In part, the link between decision consistency and (over)confidence was mediated by a neglect of alternative solutions as participants followed a more uniform approach to a task. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved).

Download

Slow Down! Insensitivity to Rate of Consumption Leads to Avoidable Satiation

May 2012

·

480 Reads

·

101 Citations

Journal of Consumer Research

Consumers often choose how quickly to consume things they enjoy. The research presented here demonstrates that they tend to consume too rapidly, growing tired of initially well-liked stimuli such as a favorite snack (experiments 1 and 4) or an enjoyable video game (experiments 2 and 3) more quickly than they would if they slowed consumption. The results also demonstrate that such overly-rapid consumption results from a failure to appreciate that longer breaks between consumption episodes slow satiation. The results present a paradox: Participants who choose their own rate of consumption experience less pleasure than those who have a slower rate of consumption chosen for them.


The feature-positive effect in allocations of responsibility for collaborative tasks

May 2012

·

160 Reads

·

1 Citation

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

People commonly believe they have contributed more to collaborative tasks than others give them credit for. We distinguish between two types of contributions – additions (such as adding words to a co-authored paper) and deletions (such as removing extraneous words) – and show that individuals are especially prone to receive less credit from others than they believe they deserve when their contributions consist of taking something away rather than adding something. Participants who shortened some writing believed they improved it just as much as did participants who lengthened some, but were seen by others as having contributed less. Although one can hardly fail to notice one's own deletions, these contributions – like any contributions that, by their very nature, leave little trace of themselves – are easy for others to overlook.


Is Variety the Spice of Life? It All Depends on the Rate of Consumption

March 2011

·

477 Reads

·

59 Citations

Judgment and Decision Making

Is variety of the spice of life? The present research suggests that the answer depends on the rate of consumption. In three experiments, we find that, whereas a variety of stimuli is preferred to repetition of even a better-liked single stimulus when consumption is continuous, this preference reverses when the satiation associated with repetition is reduced by slowing down the rate of consumption. Decision makers, however, seem to under-appreciate the influence of consumption rate on preference for (and satisfaction with) variety. At high rates of consumption, they correctly anticipate their own, high, desire for variety, but at low rates of consumption people tend to overestimate their own desire for variety. These results complicate the picture presented by prior research on the ``diversification bias'', suggesting that people overestimate their own desire for variety only when consumption is spaced out over time.


Variety Amnesia: Recalling Past Variety Can Accelerate Recovery from Satiation

December 2009

·

168 Reads

·

66 Citations

Journal of Consumer Research

Consumers frequently consume items to the point where they no longer enjoy them. In a pilot study and two experiments spanning three distinct classes of stimuli, we find that people can recover from this satiation by simply recalling the variety of alternative items they have consumed in the past. And yet, people seem to exhibit "variety amnesia" in that they do not spontaneously recall this past variety despite the fact that it would result in a desirable decrease in satiation. Thus, rather than satiation being a fixed physiological process, it appears that it is at least partially constructed in the moment. We discuss some of the theoretical implications of these findings and provide some prescriptive measures for both marketers and consumers. (c) 2009 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH, Inc..


(Not so) positive illusions

December 2009

·

113 Reads

·

8 Citations

Behavioral and Brain Sciences

We question a central premise upon which the target article is based. Namely, we point out that the evidence for "positive illusions" is in fact quite mixed. As such, the question of whether positive illusions are adaptive from an evolutionary standpoint may be premature in light of the fact that their very existence may be an illusion.


Between a rock and a hard place: Damned if you do, damned if you don’t

November 2009

·

180 Reads

·

9 Citations

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Individuals are frequently forced to make decisions from among undesirable choice-sets. Raise taxes or cut social services? Lay off workers or go bankrupt? Go deep in debt or forgo a college education? The research presented here suggests that in such situations, decision-makers are often evaluated negatively regardless of the choice they make. In Experiment 1, participants read about a judge deciding which of two seemingly unfit parents to award sole custody in a real-life divorce case. In Experiment 2, participants were led to believe that their partner in the experiment was forced to pick one of two unpleasant tasks for the participant to perform. In both cases, the decision and decision-maker were evaluated negatively regardless of the alternative chosen–and regardless of the fact that they were the only options in the choice-set. Discussion focuses on the source, scope, and consequences of this phenomenon.


On the genesis of inflated (and deflated) judgments of responsibility

January 2009

·

100 Reads

·

36 Citations

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

Prior research has found that people tend to overestimate their relative contribution to joint tasks [e.g., Ross, M., & Sicoly, F. (1979). Egocentric biases in availability and attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 322-336]. The present research investigates one source of this bias, and in doing so, identifies an important moderator of the effect. Three studies demonstrate that when people estimate their relative contribution to collective endeavors they focus on their own contribution and give less consideration to the contribution of their collaborators. This can cause overestimation for tasks in which total contributions are plentiful, but underestimation for tasks in which total contributions are few--despite the fact that both tasks reflect positively on the person who performs them. These results extend Ross and Sicoly's (1979) original analysis of bias in responsibility judgments, but also suggest that the tendency to overestimate one's relative contribution to collaborations is not as ubiquitous as once thought.


Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments.

January 2009

·

2,618 Reads

·

3,499 Citations

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

People tend to hold overly favorable views of their abilities in many social and intellectual domains. The authors suggest that this overestimation occurs, in part, because people who are unskilled in these domains suffer a dual burden: Not only do these people reach erroneous conclusions and make unfortunate choices, but their incompetence robs them of the metacognitive ability to realize it. Across 4 studies, the authors found that participants scoring in the bottom quartile on tests of humor, grammar, and logic grossly overestimated their test performance and ability. Although their test scores put them in the 12th percentile, they estimated themselves to be in the 62nd. Several analyses linked this miscalibration to deficits in metacognitive skill, or the capacity to distinguish accuracy from error. Paradoxically, improving the skills of the participants, and thus increasing their metacognitive competence, helped them recognize the limitations of their abilities. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)


Citations (46)


... They argue that extreme scores (both high and low) naturally move closer to the average upon repeated testing, which could explain the DKE without invoking any cognitive biases. Dunning (2022), however, has argued against this statistical interpretation, asserting that the DKE represents a genuine cognitive phenomenon where individuals with lower ability levels fail to recognize their incompetence, leading to inflated self-assessments (see also Kruger & Dunning, 2002). He emphasizes that the DKE is not merely a statistical artifact but a consistent finding across various domains of knowledge and skill, from academic performance to social skills. ...

Reference:

We all fall for it: Influence of driving experience, level of cognitive control engaged and actual exposure to the driving situations on the Dunning-Kruger effect
Unskilled and Unaware—But Why? A Reply to Krueger and Mueller (2002)

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... According to this effect, students tend to overestimate their academic performance because they do not rely on their previous achievement when predicting their future school success (Geraci et al., 2023). This over-confidence seems to be stronger for students with lower competences, while those with higher competence even tend to underestimate their abilities ("Dunning-Kruger effect"; Kruger & Dunning, 1999). This effect has been found consistently across multiple countries (Yang Hansen et al., 2024). ...

Unskilled and Unaware of It: How Difficulties in Recognizing One's Own Incompetence Lead to Inflated Self-Assessments

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... The self is a dominant theme in consumer behaviour studies. Scholars have advocated the role of selfimage motives in predicting behavioural tendencies (Kruger et al., 2007;Sedikides et al., 2007). Understanding the influence of these underlying motives that cater to the "self" can be a crucial goal for the marketers. ...

When Consumers’ Self‐Image Motives Fail
  • Citing Article
  • October 2007

Journal of Consumer Psychology

... In a famous paper, Kruger and Dunning (1999) attribute differences in monitoring effectiveness to differences in competence (see also, Lichtenstein & Fischhoff, 1977). Weaker performers tend to overestimate their capabilities on a number of tasks from emotional intelligence (Sheldon et al., 2014) to skeet shooting (Ehrlinger et al., 2008), whereas stronger performers correctly assess (or even underestimate) their capabilities (see Dunning, 2011 for a full review). These researchers argue that weaker performers are doubly disadvantaged: not only do they lack the skills necessary to perform the task, but these same skills are crucial to monitoring performance. ...

Why the Unskilled Are Unaware: Further Explorations of (Absent) Self-Insight Among the Incompetent

SSRN Electronic Journal

·

Kerri Johnson

·

·

[...]

·

Matthew Banner

... " People overestimate their likelihood of beating a competitor when the contest is simple (such as a trivia contest involving easy categories), but underestimate those odds when it is difficult (such as a trivia contest involving difficult categories;Moore & Kim 2003;Windschitl et al. 2003). Roommates overestimate their relative contribution to tasks involving frequent contributions like cleaning the dishes, but underestimate their relative contribution to tasks involving infrequent contributions like cleaning the oven (Kruger & Savitsky 2009). And preliminary work suggests that although people overestimate their degree of control over that which can be controlled easily, they underestimate their degree of control of what cannot (Kruger, unpublished data). ...

On the Genesis of Inflated (and Deflated) Judgments of Responsibility: Egocentrism Revisited
  • Citing Article
  • January 2006

SSRN Electronic Journal

... The self is a habitual starting point in many of life's important judgments (Chambers et al., 2003; Kruger, 1999; Ross & Sicoly, 1979). Whether it is because people simply know more about themselves than others (Moore & Cain, 2007; Moore & Small, 2007) and are more confident of what they know about themselves (Kruger, Windschitl, Burrus, Fessel, & Chambers, 2008), or because self-knowledge comes to mind more easily, rapidly, and efficiently than other knowledge (Chambers & Windschitl, 2004; Chan, Chambers, & Kruger, 2013; Radzevick & Moore, 2013), the self looms large in judgments that require people to take others into consideration, resulting in predictable judgment errors. People are thus " egocentric " thinkers, having considerable difficulty casting aside their own unique perspective when attempting to take the perspective of another. ...

The Rational Side of Egocentrism in Social Comparisons
  • Citing Article
  • January 2006

SSRN Electronic Journal

... Similarly, behavioral pricing research indicates that for an item initially priced at (MXN) $480, consumers prefer a $120 discount over an economically equivalent 25% discount (González et al. 2016), likely because the number "120" is greater than the number "25". Similar findings emerge when examining between-product comparisons (in attribute domains like warranty length, processing speed, memory, screen size and price comparisons; Kruger and Vargas 2008) and in work that compares price promotions versus bonus packs (Chen et al. 2012;Mohan, Chandon and Riis 2015). These findings originate in different literatures, yet the effects described are consistent and convergent. ...

Consumer Confusion of Percent Differences
  • Citing Article
  • January 2006

SSRN Electronic Journal

... Over time, the impact of curiosity on decision-making evolves, as prolonged information gaps can diminish users' anticipation and reduce their exploratory drive [30]. Despite its potential to introduce discomfort in uncertain situations, individuals are naturally drawn to the unknown, as highlighted by [31] and [30]. ...

The Paradox of Alypius and the Pursuit of Unwanted Information
  • Citing Article
  • January 2006

SSRN Electronic Journal

... The medical staff in particular perceived effective learning progress with VPs. However, self-assessed knowledge and skills can differ from external assessment, e.g., inexperience can lead to an overestimation in selfassessment, which must therefore be considered carefully [51,52]. ...

Unskilled and unaware--but why? A reply to Krueger and Mueller (2002).
  • Citing Article
  • January 2002

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... -William Shakespeare (King Lear, act 1, scene 2) As the name "regression fallacy" implies, people have a hard time recognizing regression effects when they see them, opening the door to a host of superstitious beliefs (Nisbett and Ross 1980;Kruger, Savitsky, and Gilovich 1999). Those that constitute admonitions about not tempting fate result from those occasions when regression effects lead (predictably) to a preponderance of negative outcomes after a run of unusual success. ...

Superstition and the regression effect
  • Citing Article
  • January 1999