Jon Hurwitz’s research while affiliated with University of Pittsburgh and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (32)


Playing the Race Card in the Post-Willie Horton Era: The Impact of Racialized Code Words on Support for Punitive Crime Policy
  • Article

March 2005

·

166 Reads

·

286 Citations

Public Opinion Quarterly

JON HURWITZ

To date, little is known about the precise impact of racially coded words and phrases. Instead, most of what we know about racialized messages comes from studies that focus on pictorial racial cues (for example, the infamous “Willie Horton” ad) or on messages with an extensive textual narrative that is laced with implicit racial cues. Because in a “post-Horton” era strategic use of racially coded words will often be far more subtle than those explored in past studies, we investigate the power of a single phrase believed by many to carry strong racial connotations: “inner city.” We do so by embedding an experiment in a national survey of whites, where a random half of respondents was asked whether they support spending money for prisons (versus antipoverty programs) to lock up “violent criminals,” while the other half was asked about “violent inner city criminals.” Consistent with the literature on issue framing, we find that whites’ racial attitudes (for example, racial stereotypes) were much more important in shaping preferences for punitive policies when they receive the racially coded, “inner city” question. Our results demonstrate how easy it is to continue “playing the race card” in the post–Willie Horton era, as well as some of the limits of such framing effects among whites with more positive racial attitudes.


The Racial Component of ‘Race-Neutral’ Crime Policy Attitudes
  • Article
  • Full-text available

March 2002

·

1,392 Reads

·

137 Citations

Political Psychology

Past studies have found evidence of a connection between race and crime in the minds of many white Americans, but several gaps remain in our knowledge of this association. Here, a multimethod approach was used to examine more closely the racial component of whites’ support for ostensibly race-neutral crime policies. Conventional correlational analysis showed that negative stereotypes of African Americans—specifically, the belief that blacks are violent and lazy—are an important source of support for punitive policies such as the death penalty and longer prison terms. A survey experiment further showed that negative evaluations of black prisoners are much more strongly tied to support for punitive policies than are negative evaluations of white prisoners. These findings suggest that when many whites think of punitive crime policies to deal with violent offenders, they are thinking of black offenders.

Download

Democratic Principles, Discrimination and Political Intolerance

January 2002

·

92 Reads

·

70 Citations

British Journal of Political Science

Political intolerance has typically been conceptualized as an unwillingness to extend expressive rights to disliked groups or individuals. One problem with this conceptualization is that, when a given percentage of individuals in a polity is found to be intolerant, it is not known if these respondents are intolerant because of the act or because of the actor. We conceptualize intolerance multidimensionally, making a distinction between generic and discriminatory intolerance; while the former stems from an unwillingness to permit the expressive act (such as holding a rally) regardless of the actor, the latter is reserved for an unwillingness to permit the act only when performed by a noxious group. Using data from the Multi-Investigator II Study (a national telephone survey of adults in the United States), we employed a split ballot technique to decompose the total proportion of intolerant respondents into groups whose intolerance stems from an aversion to the actor (discriminatory) versus those whose intolerance stems from an aversion to the act (generic). We further explored the genesis of intolerance, finding that the two types we identified stem from different antecedents.


A Reply to Miller et al.: Replication Made Simple

June 2001

·

15 Reads

·

3 Citations

Political Research Quarterly

In the preceding argument, Miller and his colleagues leveled strong charges against our multiple values approach to the study of political intolerance. Distilled to its essence, their critique focused on three elements our study: (1) they claimed an inability to replicate our results; (2) they challenged the appropriateness of our measurement protocol; and (3) they raised more general problems with research on political tolerance. In this our rejoinder, we show, first, that any problems Miller et al. experienced in replicating our results are of a trivial nature: generally speaking, our findings are robust and may be replicated freely by interested readers by visiting our web-site where both the data for the analysis and our statistical program are posted. Second, Miller et al.'s evidence concerning the weakness of our value measures is based on a fundamentally flawed analysis. And third, the authors' more sweeping criticisms of political tolerance research, as well as their specific criticisms of our study, suffer from a limited understanding of existing theory and research in the area. We take up each of Miller et al.'s criticisms in turn.



Figure 1.C. Value Priorities, Hold Rally Question 
Table 1 . Predicting Political Tolerance from Value Priorities and Value Conflict 
Table 2 .Predicting Pliability of Political Tolerance from Value Priorities and Value Conflict 
A Multiple Values Model of Political Tolerance

June 2001

·

318 Reads

·

102 Citations

Political Research Quarterly

While students of political tolerance often view tolerance decisions as a trade-off between opposing values (civil liberties versus other values), there have been few explicit attempts to formulate and test such a multiple-values model. With rare exception, researchers typically examine linkages between tolerance judgments and a single value constellation (civil liberties or general norms of democracy) without examining directly the way people rank competing values. In this essay, we use data from a national telephone survey to test a model of how various value trade-off measures (e.g., value conflict) influence citizens' initial tolerance decisions, as well as their willingness to stick to that judgment in the face of counter-arguments (i.e., the pliability of the initial baseline judgment). We find that while value conflict is often associated with greater political forbearance of disliked groups (e.g., the Klan, flag burners), greater conflict also makes individuals more susceptible to counter-arguments. We also find that when people are presented with roughly equal counterarguments, the tolerant are much more willing to abandon their initial judgment than the intolerant. We conclude with a discussion of the broader implications of our findings for the study of political tolerance and political values.



Racial Polarization on Criminal Justice Issues: Sources and Political Consequences of Fairness Judgments

January 2001

·

957 Reads

·

9 Citations

Our investigation of the antecedents and consequences of citizens' beliefs about the fairness of the criminal justice system among blacks and whites is motivated by three related concerns: 1) research showing that beliefs about procedural fairness (even more than assessments of outcomes) play a crucial role in shaping political attitudes and behavior, 2) the apparent widespread lack of confidence in the justice system expressed by both races, and 3) the historically high levels of perceived injustice and unfairness among African Americans. We report the results of a specially-commissioned national survey of whites and blacks that focuses on the chasm that exists between the races in their assessments of the criminal justice system. Three principal findings emerge from our study of the perceived fairness of the CJS: 1) race is the dominant fault line in evaluations of the CJS, with African Americans being much less likely to view the justice system (i.e., the police, the courts and the system in general) as fair than whites; 2) blacks who report being treated unfairly by the police in traffic incidents are substantially more likely to rate the justice system as unfair; and 3) based on our analysis of survey experiments where we manipulated the race of the victim in a police brutality case and the race of the suspects in a police drug search, fairness judgments play a powerful role in shaping interpretations of police behavior, particularly when blacks are the possible target of discrimination.



Values, Acts, and Actors Distinguishing Generic and Discriminatory Intolerance

January 1998

·

8 Reads

·

18 Citations

Political Behavior

Where tolerance is defined as a person's willingness to put up with political expression that the person finds objectionable, we see three prerequisites for tolerance. The person must support the general right of political expression, the general right of people to engage in the particular acts under consideration, and finally the right of members of even objectionable groups to engage in those specific acts. Many past studies of tolerance proceed directly from the first of these prerequisites to the third, and, in doing so, fail to distinguish between general attitudes regarding particular acts of expression (i.e., does the survey respondent support the right of people in general to hold public rallies) and attitudes regarding particular groups engaged in those same acts (i.e., does the respondent support the right of Communists or militia groups to hold public rallies). The consequence is ambiguity in interpretation of the meaning and etiology of tolerance, and in cross-national comparison. We demonstrate our concerns using data from a split-ballot survey conducted in Romania. Results reveal that accurate interpretation of Romanians' tolerance of the right of ethnic Hungarians to engage in various acts of political expression requires attention to respondents' general attitudes regarding those same acts.


Citations (29)


... (604). Conversely, some argue that whites' responses and evaluations of Black leadership are rooted in racial prejudice and are unlikely to change (Allport 1954;Hurwitz and Peffley 1998;Macrae et al. 1993). Another perspective suggests that there is something about Blacks in power that triggers negative reactions from whites (Blumer 1958;Bobo 1983;Giles and Evans 1986;Peterson and Riley 2022;Maxwell et al. 2013). ...

Reference:

A Democratic Dilemma: Racial Attitudes, Authoritarianism, and Whites’ Evaluation of Minority Legislators DeSante
Perception and Prejudice: Race and Politics in the United States
  • Citing Article
  • January 2001

Contemporary Sociology A Journal of Reviews

... The denial of institutional discrimination of Black individuals within society works to justify rejecting efforts to promote their inclusion in society. Peffley et al. (2017) further explored the intersection of racial attribution, punitiveness, and capital punishment opinion. Data were sourced from the 2012 Justice in Washington State survey, with a sample consisting of 611 Whites, 305 Latinos, and 288 Blacks. ...

Racial Attributions in the Justice System and Support for Punitive Crime Policies

American Politics Research

... However, consistent reevaluation and reinvention of the past through the abovementioned mechanisms (material incentives, intergenerational transmission, and historical revisionism) can lead to authoritarian nostalgia among young voters who did not experience the past. Material benefits from the authoritarian past facilitate positive memories to persist, which can be transmitted across generations through parental socialization that provides vicarious experiences of the nation's past (Mondak et al. 2017). The authoritarian past is often subject to historical revisionism by intellectuals as part of national narrative construction, via academic and popular texts, including memoirs, biographic novels, as well as comic strips for children, many of which targeting younger generations (Yang, 2021;Moon, 2009). ...

The Vicarious Bases of Perceived Injustice: BASES OF PERCEIVED INJUSTICE

American Journal of Political Science

... Burge and colleagues (2020) point to the invocation of stereotypes as a primary mechanism that results in perceptions of pandering. This makes sense, as stereotypes homogenize an entire group and assign particular characteristics to that group (Bouchard 2022;Hainmueller and Hopkins, 2014;Peffley and Hurwitz, 2009;Pérez 2010;Stangor et al., 2014). Unfortunately, White candidates have a long history of deploying racial stereotypes. ...

Racial Stereotyping and Political Attitudes: The View From Political Science

... I also include a measure of respondents' sense of linked fate with women (1 = low linked fate, 7 = high linked fate). Research has found that linked fate shapes attitudes toward the criminal justice system (Hurwitz, Peffley, and Mondak 2015). Gender, age, and region of residency are also considered relevant characteristics and are included as controls in all models. ...

Linked Fate and Outgroup Perceptions: Blacks, Latinos, and the U.S. Criminal Justice System

Political Research Quarterly

... 2 The impartial spectator or impartial bystander is the concept initially discussed in Adam Smith's Theory of Moral Sentiments (Smith, 2010). develop their foreign country images or foreign policy beliefs based on "dramatic events," such as the high-profile diplomatic discussions between the U.S. and USSR in the late 1980s (Jervis, 1976;Peffley and Hurwitz, 1992). In domestic politics, when people assess electoral candidates, voters could use the candidates' partisanship as the only relevant attribute and ignore all other attributes, including the candidates' policy positions or other individualspecific credentials. ...

International Events and Foreign Policy Beliefs: Public Response to Changing Soviet-U.S. Relations

American Journal of Political Science

... Indeed, these studies have shown that resentment against racial/ethnic minorities is independently associated with support for increased criminal justice spending (Barkan & Cohn, 2005), harsher criminal sentences (Baker et al., 2018;D. Johnson, 2001), punitive juvenile justice measures (Pickett & Chiricos, 2012), and other such policies (Peffley & Hurwitz, 1998. Most of this research has been conducted using survey data from the United States, though studies of Great Britain (Stansfield & Stone, 2018), France (Dambrun, 2007), Germany (Cochran & Piquero, 2011), Russia (Wheelock et al., 2011), other European countries (Ousey & Unnever, 2012;Unnever & Cullen, 2010a), and Israel (Pickett et al., 2014) have revealed similar patterns of findings. ...

Perception and Prejudice: Race and Politics in the United States.
  • Citing Article
  • March 2000

American Political Science Association

... How publics' perceptions of a foreign country will affect their attitudes toward external/foreign policies has long attracted scholarly attention [38,40]. In their seminal work on Americans' foreign policy beliefs, Hurwitz and Peffley [39] detected a significant effect of Americans' image of the Soviet Union on their attitudes toward defense spending, nuclear, military, and Contras policies. ...

Foreign Policy Belief Systems in Comparative Perspective: The United States and Costa Rica

International Studies Quarterly

... The authors of this model condense the existing academic literature on attitudes towards foreign and security policy (e.g. Holsti and Rosenau, 1990;Hurwitz and Peffley, 1987;Wittkopf, 1986) and supplement the traditional areas of "international cooperation" and "military might" with isolationist attitudes and questions of global justice, solidarity and redistribution. Specifically, the model consists of the four dimensions "cooperative internationalism", "isolationism", "militant internationalism" and "global justice". ...

How Are Foreign Policy Attitudes Structured? A Hierarchical Model

American Political Science Association

... Across different conceptualizations and measures, scholars consistently identify race as a key factor in individual punitive attitude formation, with White people differentially supporting punitive policies (more so when they exhibit racial animus), and holding beliefs consistent with Black people being those to whom punitive policies will be applied (Bobo and Johnson 2004;Hurwitz and Peffley 1997;Pollak and Kubrin 2007;Ramirez 2021;Cullen 2012, 2010). This difference especially pronounced when considering the death penalty as a specific case of state punishment (Unnever and Cullen 2007;Unnever, Cullen and Jonson 2008). 2 By contrast, scholars argue that Black attitudes are formed distinct from those of White people as a consequence of anxiety about crime, perceptions of criminal justice system bias, and fear that state repression will be directed toward them (Johnson 2006). ...

Public Perceptions of Race and Crime: The Role of Racial Stereotypes

American Journal of Political Science