John Sweller’s research while affiliated with UNSW Sydney and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (253)


The effect of element interactivity and mental rehearsal on working memory resource depletion and the spacing effect
  • Article
  • Full-text available

May 2024

·

161 Reads

·

1 Citation

Contemporary Educational Psychology

·

·

Ellie Anderson

·

[...]

·

John Sweller

The spacing effect occurs when learning with rest periods is superior to learning without rest periods. Cognitive load theory has explained this superiority by working memory resource depletion, under which resources are depleted during cognitive activity but restored with rest. In a series of four experiments involving 341 participants, we explored the relationships between the spacing effect, depletion of working memory resources, and mental rehearsal, particularly focusing on how these dynamics are influenced by task complexity as defined by element interactivity. Experiment 1 showed that materials with higher element interactivity had a greater impact on working memory resource depletion. In Experiment 2, using materials low in element interactivity, a spacing effect was obtained with no evidence of working memory resource depletion. Instead, results suggested that the effect might be due to mental rehearsal occurring during rest periods. Experiment 3, using more complex information, obtained both the spacing and working memory resource depletion effects for less knowledgeable learners for whom the information was high in element interactivity. In Experiment 4, testing more knowledgeable learners for whom the same information was lower in element interactivity, both effects disappeared. The results indicated that working memory resource depletion and recovery may be more sensitive to materials high in element interactivity and suggest that it is only one of multiple causes of the spacing effect.

Download


Instructional sequences in science teaching: considering element interactivity when sequencing inquiry-based investigation activities and explicit instruction

January 2024

·

205 Reads

·

2 Citations

European Journal of Psychology of Education

The use of investigations in science teaching is both common and commonly advocated for in science education literature. We suggest that the use of investigations should differ depending on the complexity of the subject matter. That complexity can vary depending on both the nature of the information and students’ expertise levels. The present study used Cognitive Load Theory and tested a hypothesis that the sequential order of having investigations first followed by explicit instruction only is more effective than that of having explicit instruction first when students have acquired sufficiently high levels of knowledge. This hypothesis was tested with 85 middle school students. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two sequences. The results indicated that for sufficiently knowledgeable students, a dis-ordinal knowledge level by sequence interaction was obtained. There was an advantage to the explicit instruction first approach for the less knowledgeable students for whom element interactivity was high but an advantage to the investigation first approach for the more knowledgeable students for whom element interactivity was lower. The results were discussed in light of a recent debate between the authors and De Jong et al. (Educational Research Review, 39, 1-14, 2023) on the role of investigations and explicit instruction in science education.


The bacterium (large oval) is challenged with a new antibiotic (ab), for which it has no defense genes to nullify its killing action. However, by accumulation of point mutations in gene sequences (arrow heads pointing to rectangles), the bacterium might develop a new gene (light rectangle on the left) coding for a function that will neutralize the harmful action of the antibiotic.
(A) In prokaryotes (bacteria), the genetic information can be organized as either single genes (light arrow) or as clusters of genes, operons (light and dark arrows), which are expressed together. This organization as operons is part of the economy of prokaryotes, as their genomes are smaller than those of eukaryotes. The organization in operons allows a prompt response when various protein products are needed in a changing environment. (B) In eukaryotes, with larger genomic information than prokaryotes, there are both exons (dark) and interspaced introns (white). Only the information in the exons constitute the mRNA and final proteins.
Natural information processing system principles.
Biological evolution and human cognition are analogous information processing systems

January 2024

·

207 Reads

·

4 Citations

The mechanisms that govern biological evolution and human cognition are analogous, as both follow the same principles of natural information processing systems. In this article, we describe the following five principles that provide an analogy between biological evolution and human cognition: (a) Randomness as Genesis Principle and (b) Borrowing and Reorganizing Principle, which indicate how natural information processing systems obtain information; (c) Narrow Limits of Change Principle and (d) Information Store Principle, which indicate how information is processed and stored; and (e) Environmental Organizing and Linking Principle, which indicate how stored information is used to generate actions appropriate to an environment. In human cognition, these analogs only apply to cognitive processes associated with biologically secondary knowledge, the knowledge typically taught in educational institutions. Based on these five principles, cognitive load theory researchers have provided diverse prescriptions to optimize instructional activities and materials. We conclude by discussing general instructional implications and future research directions based on this analogy.




Experiment 1: procedure.
Experiment 1: means (and standard deviations) of mathematics and language working memory scores.
Experiment 2: procedure.
Experiment 2: means (and standard deviations) of working memory scores.
The relationship between interleaving and variability effects: A cognitive load theory perspective

November 2023

·

318 Reads

·

1 Citation

Education Sciences

The interleaving effect indicates that students learn better from multiple areas that are interleaved rather than blocked. Two experiments tested the hypothesis that the effect is because interleaving facilitates comparisons between areas and is a variation of the variability effect that increases intrinsic cognitive load. Experiment 1 used an interleaved design with two obviously different topics and found no interleaving effect. Experiment 2 used a similar design but used topics that were more difficult to discriminate between, resulting in a clear advantage for the interleaved group associated with an increase in cognitive load. These results support the hypothesis that the interleaving and variability effects are closely related.



The Development of Cognitive Load Theory: Replication Crises and Incorporation of Other Theories Can Lead to Theory Expansion

September 2023

·

843 Reads

·

45 Citations

Educational Psychology Review

Cognitive load theory has been in development since the 1980s. Much of the impetus for that development has come from firstly, replication failures using randomised controlled trials and secondly, from the incorporation of other theories into cognitive load theory. Both have led to theory expansion. The immediate cause of the so-called “replication crisis” in psychology and other disciplines is a failure to replicate previous empirical findings. Using cognitive load theory as an example, I argue that the appearance of contradictory evidence does not necessarily derive from a failure to properly collect data. Rather, it can be caused by initially insufficiently detailed theories, with increasing detail often revealing the reason for a failure to replicate. For cognitive load theory, each failure to replicate, rather than being a negative, contributed to the further development of the theory. In addition, the theory has developed over many years by closely incorporating other theories associated with human cognitive architecture and evolutionary psychology. In this paper, I discuss some of the developmental milestones associated with cognitive load theory and how they were informed by replication failures and theory integration.


Example of material used in Chen et al.’s study (2015)
Example of material used in Leahy et al.’s study (2019)
a Example of material used for split-attention effect study (split-attention format). b Example of material used for split-attention effect study (integrated format)
A Cognitive Load Theory Approach to Defining and Measuring Task Complexity Through Element Interactivity

June 2023

·

1,156 Reads

·

73 Citations

Educational Psychology Review

Educational researchers have been confronted with a multitude of definitions of task complexity and a lack of consensus on how to measure it. Using a cognitive load theory-based perspective, we argue that the task complexity that learners experience is based on element interactivity. Element interactivity can be determined by simultaneously considering the structure of the information being processed and the knowledge held in long-term memory of the person processing the information. Although the structure of information in a learning task can easily be quantified by counting the number of interacting information elements, knowledge held in long-term memory can only be estimated using teacher judgment or knowledge tests. In this paper, we describe the different perspectives on task complexity and present some concrete examples from cognitive load research on how to estimate the levels of element interactivity determining intrinsic and extraneous cognitive load. The theoretical and practical implications of the cognitive load perspective of task complexity for instructional design are discussed.


Citations (89)


... Relevant examples of the influence of cultural tradition on schooling come from Botswana (Tabulawa 2004), South Africa (Kubow 2017), and Zimbabwe (Madzima 2010). Consistent with these traditions, the cognitive architecture field in cognitive psychology has provided neurobiological evidence about how the brain processes memory, which generates dramatically different understandings of cognition and long-term memory (Abadzi 2006;Sweller 2021). Long-term memory is not seen negatively as mere rote learning, but as the central, dominant structure of human cognition: 'if nothing has changed in long-term memory, nothing has been learned' (Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark 2006, 77). ...

Reference:

Attainable pedagogical practices for contexts of poverty: a South African case study
Instructional Design
  • Citing Chapter
  • April 2021

... Therefore, improper design of multimodal elements in videos may lead to a higher cognitive load compared to paper-based materials, thereby affecting learning outcomes (Mayer, 2020). Within a group of students exposed to identical instructional designs, differences in prior knowledge represent the primary source of cognitive diversity among individuals (Sweller, 2024). Thus, while various cognitive elements contribute to individual variations, disparities in acquired knowledge significantly influence how individuals process information. ...

Cognitive load theory and individual differences
  • Citing Article
  • February 2024

Learning and Individual Differences

... Moreover, modelling the initial state (prior knowledge, K 0 ) allows accounting for the impact of individual differences and thereby possibly explaining aptitude-treatment-interactions (Cronbach & Snow, 1977;Preacher and Sterba, 2019), that is, interactions between students' prior knowledge (aptitude) and specific instructional designs (treatment). For instance, the different impact of sequences of instructional phases on students with low or high prior knowledge (Zhang & Sweller, 2024). Such expertisereversal effects (Kalyuga, 2007) are explained at the level of the learning process by a decrease of element interactivity (several elements of the concept stored as one higher-level element) with an increase in prior knowledge (Sweller, 2010). ...

Instructional sequences in science teaching: considering element interactivity when sequencing inquiry-based investigation activities and explicit instruction
  • Citing Article
  • January 2024

European Journal of Psychology of Education

... Another method, is transduction, where a virus infecting bacteria serves as a carrier for genetic materials (Molan & Zgur, 2021). These information gives researchers insight on how organisms process information and respond to environmental cues, thereby informing strategies for optimizing genetic modification techniques (Castro-Alonso et al., 2024). Moreover, the developments in computational modeling, such as neural networks inspired by brain function, could enhance stimulations used in genetic engineering to predict the behavior of modified organisms more accurately. ...

Biological evolution and human cognition are analogous information processing systems

... This is a truly bewildering set of claims that, given a moment's thought, are educationally, philosophically, and neurobiologically questionable. For example, does the insistence on direct instruction apply to all learners, in all subject areas, at all times, in accordance with Geary's evolutionary theory, or is it only essential for novice learners (Key claim 5) to be superseded by 'problem solving practice as the knowledge of the learner increases' (Sweller et al., 2024, p. 2)? If it's only essential for novices, how does that square with Sweller's emphatic claim that biologically secondary knowledge, the kind of knowledge student are said to acquire in school, requires explicit instruction and motivational encouragement? ...

Response to De Jong et al.’s (2023) paper “Let's talk evidence – The case for combining inquiry-based and direct instruction”

Educational Research Review

... Learners often struggle when instructional materials contain redundant information (see Redundancy Principle, Kalyuga & Sweller, 2014) or when they have to divide their attention between multiple visual stimuli (see Split Attention Effect, Sweller et al., 2011). In this context, MLLMs have the potential to assist on the level of the learner by extracting key information or focusing their attention on essential aspects. ...

The Redundancy Principle in Multimedia Learning
  • Citing Chapter
  • November 2021

... Extraneous cognitive load involves unnecessary cognitive processing demands imposed by instructional design or the learning environment, potentially arising from poorly designed materials. Germane cognitive load, whose independent status in Cognitive Load Theory has been debated (Sweller, 2023), concerns mental effort directed at organizing and integrating information into longterm memory, beneficial for learning and problem-solving. As Sweller et al. (2019) summarized the evolution of Cognitive Load Theory from 1998 to 2018 and offered insights into potential avenues for future research, they advocated for a nuanced approach that recognizes the role of emotions, stress, and uncertainty in learning. ...

The Development of Cognitive Load Theory: Replication Crises and Incorporation of Other Theories Can Lead to Theory Expansion

Educational Psychology Review

... In other words, information relevance is an everchanging product of content features, individual-level processes (cognitive, affective, motivational), and the context (see Figure 1). The content features involve (1) surface features, such as color or shape, (2) visual/auditory/tactile attractiveness, (3) information source, (4) intrinsic and extraneous cognitive load (i.e., the load induced by the complexity of the learning materials and the instruction-induced load, respectively; Chen et al., 2023), and (5) the relationships between concurrently available components of information (e.g., the foreground to the background, the colorful to the black-and-white). The individual-level processes rely on (1) individual goals and meaning ascribed to given information, (2) cognitive processes such as stimulus-driven (bottom-up) and goaldirected (top-down) attention, working memory, and long-term memory, (3) germane cognitive load (the amount of cognitive resources, e.g., working memory, devoted to the task at hand; Korbach et al., 2018), (4) metacognitive processes, (5) previous experience, (6) affect, (7) motivation, and (8) attitudes. ...

A Cognitive Load Theory Approach to Defining and Measuring Task Complexity Through Element Interactivity

Educational Psychology Review

... The effect of playback speed on comprehension may depend on if information is presented in audio-only or audio-visual format. The multimedia principle posits that audio-visual information is superior for working memory and learning as it can prevent visual and auditory channels from being overloaded with processing demands (Castro-Alonso & Sweller, 2022;Leahy & Sweller, 2016;Mayer & Moreno, 2003). Since increasing playback speed primarily overwhelms the auditory channel, it is possible that visual information can provide additional support for encoding relevant verbal information, explaining why we see little to no costs of increased speed on learning of audio-visual information (Ritzhaupt et al., 2011; see the multimedia principle in Mayer, 2022). ...

The Modality Principle in Multimedia Learning

... In this model, cognitive load is synonymous with WM load, and instruction is designed to make sure cognitive resources between selective attention, distractor information, and WM to retention are adequate In general, our results fit within the context of cognitive load theory (O. Chen et al., 2023;Sweller, 2023). ...

Discussion of the special issue on cognitive load theory
  • Citing Article
  • May 2023

British Journal of Educational Psychology