John Ruscio’s research while affiliated with College of New Jersey and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (82)


Performing Small-Telescopes Analysis by Resampling: Empirically Constructing Confidence Intervals and Estimating Statistical Power for Measures of Effect Size
  • Article

March 2024

·

14 Reads

·

2 Citations

Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science

Samantha Costigan

·

John Ruscio

·

Jarret T. Crawford

When new data are collected to check the findings of an original study, it can be challenging to evaluate replication results. The small-telescopes method is designed to assess not only whether the effect observed in the replication study is statistically significant but also whether this effect is large enough to have been detected in the original study. Unless both criteria are met, the replication either fails to support the original findings or the results are mixed. When implemented in the conventional manner, this small-telescopes method can be impractical or impossible to conduct, and doing so often requires parametric assumptions that may not be satisfied. We present an empirical approach that can be used for a variety of study designs and data-analytic techniques. The empirical approach to the small-telescopes method is intended to extend its reach as a tool for addressing the replication crisis by evaluating findings in psychological science and beyond. In the present tutorial, we demonstrate this approach using a Shiny app and R code and included an analysis of most studies (95%) replicated as part of the Open Science Collaboration’s Reproducibility Project in Psychology. In addition to its versatility, simulations demonstrate the accuracy and precision of the empirical approach to implementing small-telescopes analysis.


Fig. 1 Accuracy of CD for different sample sizes, α-levels, numbers of factors (k), variables per factor (vpf) and between-factor correlations (ρ)
Fig. 2 Accuracy of CDF for different sample sizes, N rep values, number of factors (k), variables per factor (vpf) and between-factor correlations (ρ)
Fig. 3 Comparison of CD and CDF with selected hyperparameters (and EKC as a baseline) across conditions with different between-factor correlations (ρ), variables per factor (vp f ) and numbers of factors (k)
Fig. 4 Accuracy of CD and CDF with selected hyperparameters depending on the level of their agreement ( ˆ k C D − ˆ k C DF ) [relative frequency of the scenario in brackets]
The comparison data forest: A new comparison data approach to determine the number of factors in exploratory factor analysis
  • Article
  • Full-text available

June 2023

·

155 Reads

·

12 Citations

Behavior Research Methods

Developing psychological assessment instruments often involves exploratory factor analyses, during which one must determine the number of factors to retain. Several factor-retention criteria have emerged that can infer this number from empirical data. Most recently, simulation-based procedures like the comparison data approach have shown the most accurate estimation of dimensionality. The factor forest, an approach combining extensive data simulation and machine learning modeling, showed even higher accuracy across various common data conditions. Because this approach is very computationally costly, we combine the factor forest and the comparison data approach to present the comparison data forest. In an evaluation study, we compared this new method with the common comparison data approach and identified optimal parameter settings for both methods given various data conditions. The new comparison data forest approach achieved slightly higher overall accuracy, though there were some important differences under certain data conditions. The CD approach tended to underfactor and the CDF tended to overfactor, and their results were also complementary in that for the 81.7% of instances when they identified the same number of factors, these results were correct 96.6% of the time.

Download

Re-examining the latent structure of suicidal thoughts using taxometric analysis: Implications for testing ideation to action theoretical models of suicidal thoughts and behavior

March 2021

·

15 Reads

A central question in psychological science concerns whether psychological constructs are best conceptualized as dimensional or consist of one or more categories. The present study uses contemporary taxometric procedures to examine the latent structure of suicidal thoughts, with implications for how suicidal thoughts and behavior (STB) ought to be conceptualized, assessed, measured, and managed. Three nonredundant taxometric procedures (MAMBAC, MAXEIG, and L-Mode) were performed on various sets of indicators, and analyses were replicated across two large samples that included large numbers of individuals reporting current and recent STB. Results provide further evidence that the latent structure of suicidal thoughts is best understood as dimensional. However, inconsistent findings across studies and the relatively small number of taxometric studies conducted to date both suggest that it is premature to draw clear or definitive conclusions about the latent structure of STB being dimensional or categorical based on taxometric evidence. We report a meta-analysis of the current literature which evidences this ambiguity. We provide a detailed, critical discussion of the STB taxometric literature and outline key directions for future taxometric studies in this area, particularly how taxometric analysis relates to testing ‘ideation to action’ theoretical models, which hypothesize that the development of suicidal ideation and the progression from suicide desire to attempting suicide are distinct processes with distinct explanations/mechanisms. It remains entirely possible that qualitatively distinct types of STB (e.g., representing ideation vs. action) or populations have different latent structures indicating different levels of risk.


Asking People to Explain Complex Policies Does Not Increase Political Moderation: Three Preregistered Failures to Closely Replicate Fernbach, Rogers, Fox, and Sloman’s (2013) Findings

March 2021

·

27 Reads

·

18 Citations

Psychological Science

Fernbach et al. (2013) found that political extremism and partisan in-group favoritism can be reduced by asking people to provide mechanistic explanations for complex policies, thus making their lack of procedural-policy knowledge salient. Given the practical importance of these findings, we conducted two preregistered close replications of Fernbach et al.’s Experiment 2 (Replication 1a: N = 306; Replication 1b: N = 405) and preregistered close and conceptual replications of Fernbach et al.’s Experiment 3 (Replication 2: N = 343). None of the key effects were statistically significant, and only one survived a small-telescopes analysis. Although participants reported less policy understanding after providing mechanistic policy explanations, policy-position extremity and in-group favoritism were unaffected. That said, well-established findings that providing justifications for prior beliefs strengthens those beliefs, and well-established findings of in-group favoritism, were replicated. These findings suggest that providing mechanistic explanations increases people’s recognition of their ignorance but is unlikely to increase their political moderation, at least under these conditions.


Suicidal Thoughts and Behaviour and Demographic Characteristics in Each Sample
Reexamining the Latent Structure of Suicidal Thoughts Using Taxometric Analysis: Implications for Testing Ideation to Action Theoretical Models of Suicidal Thoughts and Behavior

January 2021

·

127 Reads

·

10 Citations

Psychological Assessment

A central question in psychological science concerns whether psychological constructs are best conceptualized as dimensional or consist of one or more categories. The present study uses contemporary taxometric procedures to examine the latent structure of suicidal thoughts, with implications for how suicidal thoughts and behavior (STB) ought to be conceptualized, assessed, measured, and managed. Three nonredundant taxometric procedures (MAMBAC, MAXEIG, and L-Mode) were performed on various sets of indicators, and analyses were replicated across two large samples that included large numbers of individuals reporting current and recent STB. Results provide further evidence that the latent structure of suicidal thoughts is best understood as dimensional. However, inconsistent findings across studies and the relatively small number of taxometric studies conducted to date both suggest that it is premature to draw clear or definitive conclusions about the latent structure of STB being dimensional or categorical based on taxometric evidence. We report a meta-analysis of the current literature which evidences this ambiguity. We provide a detailed, critical discussion of the STB taxometric literature and outline key directions for future taxometric studies in this area, particularly how taxometric analysis relates to testing "ideation to action" theoretical models, which hypothesize that the development of suicidal ideation and the progression from suicide desire to attempting suicide are distinct processes with distinct explanations/mechanisms. It remains entirely possible that qualitatively distinct types of STB (e.g., representing ideation vs. action) or populations have different latent structures indicating different levels of risk. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Taxometric Analysis

January 2021

·

21 Reads

·

4 Citations

Taxometric analysis is often used in clinical psychological research to determine whether a construct of interest is categorical or dimensional in nature. This article reviews the method and provides empirical guidelines for performing and interpreting results of taxometric analysis. Doing so can be quite subjective, and we describe recent advances for reducing this subjectivity. We describe a software package (RTaxometrics) for taxometric analysis and demonstrate its use with illustrative categorical and dimensional data sets. These analyses show how to determine whether data sets are appropriate for taxometric analysis, how to perform various taxometric procedures, and how to interpret the results.


Number of Qualified Studies in Each Topic Category
Coding Agreement and Findings for External Validity Criteria
Weighing People Rather Than Food: A Framework for Examining External Validity

November 2019

·

61 Reads

·

12 Citations

Perspectives on Psychological Science

Caitlin M. Loyka

·

John Ruscio

·

·

[...]

·

Amanda Zabel

Research training in psychological science emphasizes common threats to internal validity, with no comparably systematic or rigorous treatment of external validity. Trade-offs between internal and external validity are well known in some areas (e.g., efficacy vs. effectiveness studies in clinical psychology), less so in others (e.g., forensic research on eyewitness identification, false memories, or confessions). We present a framework for examining external validity grounded in four domains—populations, settings, outcomes, and timeframes—that can be used to enhance the generalizability of findings. We discuss this framework and then illustrate its use by reviewing mindless eating interventions intended to help people lose weight. Research in this published literature seldom samples from appropriate populations (e.g., overweight or obese individuals) or measures appropriate outcomes (e.g., weight change) in appropriate settings (e.g., the home) over appropriate timeframes (e.g., sustained interventions with follow-up) to determine whether practical advice is empirically supported. In their applied work, we encourage psychological scientists to design studies, analyze data, and report findings with greater attention to external validity to demonstrate, rather than assume, the generalizability of findings to the intended populations, settings, outcomes, and timeframes. Editors and reviewers can hold investigators accountable for doing so.


Do Status-Legitimizing Beliefs Moderate Effects of Racial Progress on Perceptions of Anti-White Bias? A Replication of Wilkins and Kaiser (2014)

August 2018

·

17 Reads

·

5 Citations

Social Psychological and Personality Science

In three studies, Wilkins and Kaiser found that both chronic and experimental salience of racial progress in the United States increased the perceptions of anti-White bias only among people high in status-legitimizing beliefs (SLBs). We conducted four preregistered high-powered replications of this research. Studies 1, 2, and 3a were close replications of studies 1–3, respectively. Study 3b was a close replication that included an additional experimental condition. Contrary to the original findings, none of the four expected interaction effects tested were statistically significant in the predicted direction, and only one of the four survived a “small telescopes” analysis. We provide additional tests addressing whether changing social contexts explain our failures to replicate, with mixed conclusions. Whereas it is possible that changing social contexts may have eliminated a once true effect, it is also possible that the original results were false positives.



Does Subjective SES Moderate the Effect of Money Priming on Socioeconomic System Support? A Replication of Schuler and Wänke (2016)

November 2017

·

21 Reads

·

4 Citations

Social Psychological and Personality Science

Findings that money priming increases socioeconomic system support have proven difficult to replicate. Schuler and Wänke found that subjective socioeconomic status (SES) moderates money priming effects on system justification and belief in a just world. We conducted three preregistered replications of this research, with sample sizes 3 times those of the original studies. Replication 1 was a conceptual replication that combined elements from the original two studies, and Replications 2 and 3 were close replications of Studies 1 and 2, respectively. None of the four subjective SES × Money Prime interaction effects tested were statistically significant, and only one of the four survived a “small telescopes” analysis. We discuss reasons for our general failure to replicate the original findings and implications for money priming effects.


Citations (66)


... Generalised Anxiety Disorder (GAD) is characterised by uncontrollable worry and somatic symptoms (e.g., muscle tension [4]). It affects 1-3% of [11][12][13][14][15][16][17][18][19] year-olds [5], commonly co-occurs with other anxiety disorders [6], and is one of the most frequent anxiety diagnoses in treatment-seeking adolescent populations [7]. There is an increase in the prevalence [5] and clinician-rated symptom severity [7] of GAD from childhood into adolescence, perhaps reflecting the growth of meta-cognitive skills [8], and the heightened social-evaluative and academic concerns [9,10] associated with this developmental stage. ...

Reference:

Cognitive Aspects of Generalised Anxiety Disorder in Adolescents: Exploring Intolerance of Uncertainty, Cognitive Avoidance, and Positive Beliefs About Worry
A Taxometric Investigation of the Latent Structure of Worry

Journal of Abnormal Psychology

... Summary scores ranged from 0 to 44, with 25% of the sample scoring at or above the cut-off (≥ 21) for moderate depressive symptoms among college aged samples [75]. Research consistently supports that depression presents along a continuum rather than as discrete categories [76,77]. Therefore, depressive symptoms was entered as a continuous variable in all analyses in the current study. ...

Informing the Continuity Controversy: A Taxometric Analysis of Depression

Journal of Abnormal Psychology

... As psychological data (e.g., questionnaire data) may not meet these assumptions and the exact-fit hypothesis of the χ 2 test may be too strict, fit indices are often used instead for model fit evaluation (e.g., Bentler, 2007;Goretzko et al., 2024;Preacher et al., 2013). Numerous fit indices have been developed to assess the goodness or badness of fit of a latent variable model-with the CFI (Bentler, 1990), the goodness-of-fit index (e.g., Mulaik et al., 1989), the normed fit index (Bentler & Bonett, 1980), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI, Tucker & Lewis, 1973), the standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR, e.g., Hu & Bentler, 1998), and the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA, e.g., Steiger, 1998) being the most popular ones (Finch, 2020;Goretzko et al., 2024;Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003). ...

The comparison data forest: A new comparison data approach to determine the number of factors in exploratory factor analysis

Behavior Research Methods

... Importantly, this applies to the group of subjects who were not asked about beliefs in conspiracy theories, and as such, beliefs in conspiracy theories might be just as amenable to change upon reflection as beliefs in other ideas. These results contribute to a body of research into reflective thinking, that, at times, has presented contradictory findings (Crawford & Ruscio, 2021;Fernbach et al., 2013;Hirt & Markman, 1995;Sloman & Vives, 2022;Tesser, 1978;Vitriol & Marsh, 2018;Yelbuz et al., 2022). Thus, our study contributes more evidence in favor of reflection potentially weakening beliefs, suggesting that people are open to changing their minds about some propositions. ...

Asking People to Explain Complex Policies Does Not Increase Political Moderation: Three Preregistered Failures to Closely Replicate Fernbach, Rogers, Fox, and Sloman’s (2013) Findings
  • Citing Article
  • March 2021

Psychological Science

... [16][17][18] Many devices have been developed for the assessment of tokophobia [19][20][21] and fetal bonding (also known as maternal prenatal attachment) [22][23][24][25] as well as suicidal ideation. [26][27][28] Nevertheless, these devices are not without drawbacks. Such devices were developed as a specific assessment or screening tool for single mental health domains (e.g., the EPDS for depression, the Wijma Delivery Expectancy/Experience Questionnaire[W-DEQ] 21 for tokophobia). ...

Reexamining the Latent Structure of Suicidal Thoughts Using Taxometric Analysis: Implications for Testing Ideation to Action Theoretical Models of Suicidal Thoughts and Behavior

Psychological Assessment

... Only through additional research will we be able to conclusively demonstrate which of the various common traits identified in this study are and are not part of what has come to be called psychopathic personality disorder. Ruscio et al. (2006) have argued that scales designed to measure a dimension should attempt to establish adequate discrimination along the entire length of the construct rather than just focusing on discrimination at the putative taxonic boundary. The Farrington et al. results for the present study identified the levels in the PCL-R facets that likely require enhanced attention to improve discrimination across the continua. ...

Introduction To The Taxometric Method: A Practical Guide
  • Citing Book
  • January 2013

... The presence of one or more depressive symptoms among the core symptoms of depression seems to identify a group of individuals who are distinct from those with no core depressive symptoms suggesting this to be a more appropriate threshold for identifying clinically depressed individuals within a classificatory system such as the ICD, and questions the complete dimensional character of depression from 'normal' depressive symptoms through severe forms of depression widely reported in previous literature. 6,28 Although the decrement associated with the symptoms of the depressive episode group appears to be greater than for the other subtypes of depression, all subtypes have significant and large effects on health status. ...

Applications of the Taxometric Method
  • Citing Chapter
  • January 2013

... Finally, the adoption of CS elements is anticipated to strengthen the generalizability of the study findings, given their influence on the determinants affecting external validity. These determinants include the inclusion of the general public, the pragmatic and tailored dietary recommendations, the real-life setting of the study, and the inclusion of a follow-up measurement concerned with behavior change [ 89 ]. To make this more concrete, we included a diverse group (aged 18-70 years) of healthy Dutch individuals, likely representative of the healthy Dutch population. ...

Weighing People Rather Than Food: A Framework for Examining External Validity

Perspectives on Psychological Science

... However, predictors of perceived discrimination might change depending on the socio-political context (Crawford et al., 2019). Therefore, a replication of the present findings is necessary. ...

Do Status-Legitimizing Beliefs Moderate Effects of Racial Progress on Perceptions of Anti-White Bias? A Replication of Wilkins and Kaiser (2014)
  • Citing Article
  • August 2018

Social Psychological and Personality Science

... Edens and colleagues uncovered evidence of dimensional latent structure in their study on psychopathy in a sample of 876 prison inmates and court-ordered substance abusers. Research published within a year of Edens' investigation (Guay et al., 2007;Murrie et al., 2007;Walters et al., 2007) corroborated that psychopathy, as assessed with the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 2003) and Psychopathy Checklist-Screening Version (PCL:SV; Hart et al., 1995) was more appropriately modeled as a dimension than as a category, a finding later replicated on samples of female (Guay et al., 2018) and Asian (Ren et al., 2020) prison inmates, and also found when biomarkers were analyzed alongside PCL-R scores (Walters et al., 2015). ...

A taxometric investigation of psychopathy in women
  • Citing Article
  • January 2018