John F. Dovidio’s research while affiliated with Yale University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (531)


Weight Bias and Preparedness to Treat Higher-Weight Patients: Understanding the Role of Motivation to Respond Without Weight-Based Prejudice Among Resident Physicians
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

October 2024

·

10 Reads

Stigma and Health

·

·

John F. Dovidio

·

[...]

·

Sean M. Phelan

Weight stigma is widely recognized as a significant concern in health care. Studies indicate that as medical trainees advance through their training, their levels of weight bias may intensify. Such prejudices can lead to inferior care for higher-weight patients, underscoring the urgency for research that identifies factors that are protective against the development of weight bias. Prior findings have shown that the motivation to respond without prejudice is a key determinant of explicit bias across various contexts, yet its role concerning weight bias remains underexplored. In a 1-year longitudinal study of U.S. resident physicians (N = 3,507), we examined the associations between participants’ internal motivation (i.e., based on personal values) and external motivation (i.e., based on social pressures) to respond without weight-based prejudice on self-reported explicit weight bias, self-rated preparedness to treat higher-weight patients, and participation in bias-related training. We found that internal motivation predicted lower weight bias (across three measures) and greater self-rated preparedness to treat higher-weight patients (ps ≤ .001), whereas external motivation predicted higher-weight bias (ps < .001). Participants’ internal motivation also predicted their participation in bias-related training during the following year (ps ≤ .01). Each of these findings held when controlling for demographic variables and body mass index. Participants were young and predominantly White, so further research will be needed to generalize these findings to the broader population of health care providers. Taken together, these findings suggest that internal motivation to respond without weight-based prejudice may be a useful target for future bias-reduction intervention efforts.

View access options

Cultural Tightness-Looseness and Individual Differences in Non-Normativeness Predict Stigmatization of Out-Groups: A Multilevel Cross-Cultural Study

September 2024

·

38 Reads

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

Although a large body of research has focused on the determinants of stigma, multilevel approaches that can identify both micro- and macro-level influences are rarely employed. We adopted a multilevel perspective with data from 174,325 participants from 80 countries in two waves—Wave 5 (Study 1) and Wave 6 (Study 2) of the World Values Survey. We examined how country-level normative tightness-looseness and individual-level non-normativeness relate to stigma toward racial and ethnic out-groups and groups deviating from social standards. Preregistered analyses showed that for both waves individuals in normatively tighter (vs. looser) societies exhibited more stigma generally. Also, for both waves, individuals higher in non-normativeness demonstrated a greater level of stigma toward members of immigrant, racial, or ethnic out-groups while exhibiting a lower level of stigma toward groups deviating from social standards. The current work thus reveals how characteristics of both individuals and culture jointly affect stigma.


Selective cultural adoption: The roles of warmth, competence, morality and perceived indispensability in majority-group acculturation

September 2024

·

46 Reads

·

2 Citations

British Journal of Social Psychology

Psychological research has begun considering the dynamics involved in majority-group acculturation, which is the extent to which cultural majority groups adopt the culture of immigrants and minority groups. However, previous research has predominantly concentrated on reactions to 'immigrants' or 'minority groups' as a homogenous entity, overlooking the nuanced perceptions and varied valuations attributed to different groups. Recognizing the heterogeneity among immigrant and minority groups, the present work investigated the influence of several perceived characteristics of immigrant and minority groups on majority-group members' adoption of their cultures. Specifically, in three pre-registered studies-one correlational (Nparticipants = 201, Ntrials = 2814) and two within-subjects experimental (Nparticipants = 144 and 146, Ntrials = 720 and 730) designs with close to politically representative samples from the U.K. and U.S. -majority-group members were more willing to adopt immigrant and minority-group cultures that they perceived as warm, competent and moral because these perceptions made immigrants and minority groups seem indispensable to the identity and economy of the mainstream society. Our studies highlight the importance of considering the differentiated acculturation that majority-group members have to various groups within the same national context. We discuss the societal and cultural repercussions of this selective uptake of other cultures.



Figure 1
Dissecting the Onion: Identifying and Remediating Issues Surrounding Data Integrity in Online Survey Research

July 2024

·

936 Reads

The Quantitative Methods for Psychology

In this non-empirical article, which is intended as a decision-making resource for researchers , we identify issues surrounding data integrity that commonly arise in online survey research and we propose remediation strategies based on challenges we encountered during a particular pilot study as well as our collective experience with conducting online survey research. Using the metaphor of an onion, we peel off the layers of this complex problem, synthesize the various available strategies used across disciplines, and propose some novel ones based on our perspective as psychologists. Corresponding to this multi-layered problem, we propose multi-layered solutions to prevent illegitimate responding-by both humans and non-humans (robots or "bots" for short)-from compromising the quality of data collected via online survey research. The first layer entails strategic item selection and protective programming in survey development. The second layer involves astute advertising and recruitment tactics to minimize illegitimate responses during survey dissemination. The third layer includes algorithms and other mechanisms to identify suspicious responses for possible exclusion during data verification. When we peel off the layers and reach the core problem of illegitimate responses to online surveys-financial incentives-we will propose ways of navigating respondent reimbursement to mitigate their inadvertent harmful impacts on the research process. By proposing these solutions, we aim to protect the integrity of scientific inquiry in psychology, especially given how often this method is used in the discipline.


Experimental differences in gender keyword(s) listed in Experiments 1-3
All-Male Research Samples Discourage Accurate Science Communication

June 2024

·

74 Reads

Research samples have historically overrepresented men, resulting in worse outcomes for women. Even when women are represented, inequity might persist due to underlying differences in how people reason about gender. Theories of androcentrism argue that, in general, people emphasize gender more about women than men. Based on this theory and building on prior archival work, we conducted three preregistered experiments (N = 1,315). Participants read scientific findings based either on all-male or all-female samples. For both, the gender homogeneity of the sample is a key limitation that should be noted when describing results. However, participants were more likely to pick women as a keyword to represent findings based on all-female samples (compared to leaving gender unlisted or listing both women and men) than to pick men to describe identical findings based on all-male samples. Grounded in androcentrism theory, this work has practical implications for accurate science communication and gender equity.


Messaging About HIV Transmission Risk When Viral Load Is Undetectable: Reactions and Perceived Accuracy Among US Sexual Minority Men

June 2024

·

24 Reads

·

1 Citation

JAIDS Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes

Background Scientific evidence indicates that HIV viral suppression to an undetectable level eliminates sexual transmission risk ("Undetectable=Untransmittable” or “U=U”). However, U=U messaging has been met with skepticism among sexual minority men (SMM) and others. In this survey-based experiment, we manipulated messaging about HIV risk and examined reactions and perceived message accuracy among US SMM. Methods SMM living with HIV ( n =106) and HIV-negative/status-unknown SMM ( n =351) participated in an online survey (2019-2020). Participants were randomly assigned to one of three messaging conditions, which varied by level of HIV sexual transmission risk associated with an undetectable viral load (No Risk [U=U]/Low Risk/Control). Participants reported reactions, message accuracy, and reasons for perceiving inaccuracy. We coded open-response data (reactions and reasons) into conceptual categories (e.g., “Enthusiasm”). We compared reactions, accuracy ratings, and reasons by condition and serostatus. Results In the No Risk Condition, common reactions were Enthusiasm (40.0%), Skepticism/Disagreement (20.0%), and Agreement (19.4%), reactions common to comparison conditions. A higher percentage of HIV-negative/status-unknown participants (24.1%) expressed Skepticism/Disagreement in the No Risk Condition compared with other conditions (3.2%-9.7%). Participants living with HIV were more likely than HIV-negative/status-unknown participants to perceive all messages as accurate. In the No Risk Condition, common reasons for perceiving inaccuracy were Risk Misstated (46.1%), Oversimplified/Caveats Needed (17.1%), and Personal Unfamiliarity/Uncertainty (14.5%), reasons common to comparison conditions. Across conditions, 10.3% of participants attributed message inaccuracy to misdefinition of undetectable. Conclusion Most SMM reacted favorably to U=U messaging. However, many—especially HIV-negative/status-unknown SMM—expressed skepticism. Interventions are needed to enhance U=U understanding and acceptance.


Fig. 1. Study 1. increasing diversity experimental manipulation (dichotomous variable) and actual state immigrant integration policies (continuous variable) predicting negative emotions. less inclusive policies defined as −1 Sd from the mean and more inclusive policies defined as +1 Sd from the mean.***P < 0.001 (two-tailed).
Fig. 3. Study 2. Actual county diversity (continuous variable) and policy manipulation (dichotomous variable) predicting positive emotions. low diversity defined as −1 Sd from the mean and high diversity defined as +1 Sd from the mean.**P < 0.01 (two-tailed).
Fig. 5. Study 4. change in diversity (continuous variable) and change in policy (continuous variable) predicting positive immigration attitudes (low increase in diversity and less inclusive policy change defined as −1 Sd from the mean, high increase in diversity, and more inclusive policy change defined as +1 Sd from the mean).***P < 0.001 (two-tailed).
Overview of the four studies.
Integration policies shape ethnic-racial majorities' threat reactions to increasing diversity

May 2024

·

187 Reads

·

2 Citations

Science Advances

Increasing ethnic and racial diversity often fuels feelings of threat among ethnic-racial majorities (e.g., self-identifiedwhite Americans and European nationals). We contend that these threat perceptions depend on the policy context.Across four studies, we test whether more inclusive immigrant integration policies attenuate ethnic-racial majorities’ threat reactions. Studies 1 to 3 (n = 469, 733, and 1745, respectively) used experimental methods with whiteAmerican participants in the United States. Study 4 (n = 499,075) used secondary analysis of survey data compar-ing attitudes of nationals in 30 European countries and measured the impact of actual changes in diversity andpolicies over 10 years. Our results show that integration policies shape threat reactions even in those situationswhen increasing diversity could be seen as the most threatening: when narratives highlight the majority’s impend-ing minority position or when diversity suddenly increases. When policies are more inclusive toward immigrants,ethnic-racial majority participants report less threat (or no threat) in response to increasing diversity


Affirmative Action

February 2024

·

46 Reads

Affirmative action refers to laws or government-mandated or voluntary policies or procedures designed to promote the equitable inclusion of members of certain historically excluded groups by granting members of these groups additional consideration for educational, economic, and employment opportunities. Affirmative action has traditionally been one of the structural ways to promote diversity in society. While much of the research on affirmative action has focused on the United States, affirmative action programs exist in many other countries in Europe, Asia, Africa, and South America. The approaches used internationally vary in many ways, most commonly with respect to the groups that the programs are designed to benefit and the degree to which group-based characteristics are considered in relation to other, merit-based credentials. For example, whereas the use of quotas in affirmative action is prohibited in the United States, affirmative action programs for college admission and federal employment in Brazil and India employ quotas for people of color. In 2023, India passed a law to reserve 33 percent of state and national legislative positions for women. India’s affirmative action policy, in which a person’s nationality, sex, religion, and caste are taken into account in education and employment by public and private organizations, is grounded in the country’s constitutional principles. Also, different countries justify having affirmative action policies to varying degrees by the goals of remediating past injustice, improving economics, promoting diversity, and achieving social justice. Who affirmative action benefits, how the policy is justified, how affirmative action programs are implemented, and the attitudes and ideologies of people viewing the program all contribute to the extent to which affirmative action is supported or opposed by people. While considering affirmative action internationally, this examination focuses on affirmative action in the United States, which has stimulated significant behavioral science research and generated considerable public controversy, in order to illuminate basic processes that shape the content and consequences of affirmative action. Support and opposition to affirmative action may both be grounded in principles of justice, but these are sometimes competing values. Specifically, this entry considers (a) a range of resources examining multiple facets of the topic, (b) assessments of the effectiveness of affirmative action, (c) reactions based on self- or group-interest, (d) individual differences in responses to affirmative action, (e) impact of the way the program is justified on support, (f) psychological consequences, (g) dynamics of help in relation to affirmative action, and (h) recent developments for promoting diversity.


Partner viral load x condom use interaction effect on openness among Never PrEP Users.Note. There was a significant partner viral load status x condom use interaction effect among never PrEP users, indicating that the combination of using (vs. not using) condoms and a partner having an undetectable (vs. detectable) viral load was associated with the greatest level of openness to serodifferent partnering. No significant interaction effects were found among current and former PrEP users
The Implications of PrEP Use, Condom Use, and Partner Viral Load Status for Openness to Serodifferent Partnering Among US Sexual Minority Men (SMM)

February 2024

·

37 Reads

·

2 Citations

AIDS and Behavior

PrEP has been reported to facilitate openness to serodifferent sexual partnerships among sexual minority men (SMM). However, other aspects of a sexual scenario likely come into play, including whether or not condoms are used and whether or not the partner living with HIV has an undetectable viral load. This online survey study evaluated the association between PrEP status and openness to serodifferent partnering, as well as the effects of various sexual scenario variables (condom use and partner’s viral load) among 268 HIV-negative SMM in the US. Each participant reported their PrEP status and rated their openness to serodifferent partnering in four sexual scenarios, which varied by condom use (with/without) and partner viral load status (detectable/undetectable). Analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were conducted to assess differences in openness to serodifferent partnering by PrEP status in each scenario, adjusting for background characteristics. A two-way repeated measures ANCOVA and a three-way mixed factorial ANCOVA, including PrEP status as a between-subjects variable, were also performed to assess the effects of condom use and partner viral load status on openness. Across all scenarios, current PrEP users expressed significantly greater openness to serodifferent partnering compared to participants who had never used PrEP. Current PrEP users were also more likely than former PrEP users to consider partnering with someone with an undetectable viral load without using condoms. Both PrEP users and PrEP-inexperienced individuals had greater openness to serodifferent partnering with a partner having an undetectable (vs. detectable) viral load, which was magnified by condom use among the latter.


Citations (67)


... Circumstantial evidence at the group level seems to back this proposition. In a set of studies by Kunst, Dovidio, et al. (2023), the more competent, warm, and moral immigrants were perceived to be, the greater the motivation was for majority-group members to adopt their culture. Although these stereotypes can be considered only a proxy of prestige, it is probable that these mechanisms operate in the context of inter-individual cultural learning as well. ...

Reference:

Selective Cultural Adoption: The Roles of Warmth, Competence, Morality, and Perceived Indispensability in Majority-Group Acculturation
Selective cultural adoption: The roles of warmth, competence, morality and perceived indispensability in majority-group acculturation
  • Citing Article
  • September 2024

British Journal of Social Psychology

... Some of the methods described here have been reported in other articles for which data were collected as part of the same study [44,52]. ...

Messaging About HIV Transmission Risk When Viral Load Is Undetectable: Reactions and Perceived Accuracy Among US Sexual Minority Men
  • Citing Article
  • June 2024

JAIDS Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes

... As such, inclusive institutional policies constitute an alternate normative source which may direct majority youths' individual intergroup attitudes and behavior -over and above an inclusive social climate. In support of the normative impact of inclusive policies, in more welcoming national policy environments native majority members reported more positive individual attitudes towards immigrants (Green et al., 2020;Kende et al., 2024). Turning to schools as institutional settings, when school policies were more inclusive, majority students reported less prejudice or bias against ethnic minorities in more diverse schools (Kende et al., 2022). ...

Integration policies shape ethnic-racial majorities' threat reactions to increasing diversity

Science Advances

... Accordingly, a detectable status has been experienced as feeling "dirty" or "dangerous" and has served as the basis for social rejection [26,[41][42][43]. For example, HIV-negative sexual minority men, including those on PrEP, have previously expressed less openness to "bridging the serodivide" (having sex with PLWH) when their prospective sex partners who are living with HIV have a detectable rather than undetectable viral load [39,44]. The idealization of "achieving" an undetectable viral load and the common framing of "being undetectable" as an expected and desired identity symbolic of social responsibility simultaneously casts PLWH who are not virally suppressed as deviant, infectious, and immoral [37,38]. ...

The Implications of PrEP Use, Condom Use, and Partner Viral Load Status for Openness to Serodifferent Partnering Among US Sexual Minority Men (SMM)

AIDS and Behavior

... When a group's basic needs are frustrated or denied it leads to intergroup con ict and even aggressive behavior (Tajfel & Turner, 1979,1986. There is evidence that perceived threat is heightened when group identity is salient (Stephan, Ybarra, & Morrison, 2009;Weisel, & Zultan, 2016;Dovidio et al., 2020) and is more likely to lead to aggressive and retaliatory responses (Fischer, Haslam, & Smith, 2010), including "vicarious retaliation " against outgroup members (i.e., ingroup members retaliate for attacks or provocations that have no personal consequences for them but harm ingroup members) (Lickel, Miller, Stenstrom, Denson, & Schmader, 2006). And gang members tend to dehumanize or denigrate outgroup members in order to protect ingroup identity and rationalize outgroup attacks (Alleyne, Fernandes, & Pritchard, 2014;Enock, Tipper, & Over,2021).Compared to outgroup members, especially those who are highly identi ed group members have a more relaxed psychological perception threshold for ingroups (Hackel, Looser, & Van Bavel, 2014). ...

Common Identity and Humanity
  • Citing Chapter
  • January 2020

... [6][7][8] The Prevention Access Campaign 9 developed the term 'Undetectable equals Untransmittable' (U=U), which led to an awareness-raising campaign, launched in 2016, to promote the wellbeing of PWHIV and reduce sexual stigma and discrimination. [10][11][12] In 2018, the Australasian Society for HIV, Viral Hepatitis and Sexual Health Medicine (ASHM) released guidance to clinicians to promote conversations with patients about U=U. 13 While U=U has supported many couples with mixed HIV status to experience a relationship not threatened or disrupted by HIV, experiences or anticipation of stigma remain a challenge. [14][15][16] The 'NSW HIV strategy 2021-2025' has set a target of a 75% reduction in reported experience of stigma or discrimination by people living with HIV in NSW healthcare settings. ...

The Potential Role of Undetectable = Untransmittable (U = U) in Reducing HIV Stigma among Sexual Minority Men in the US

AIDS and Behavior

... We recruited 900 American participants on MTurk. We excluded 46 participants for failing attention checks or indicating that some of their responses were random/jokes (Vial et al., 2022(Vial et al., , 2024, leaving a final sample of 854 (47% female; 52% male and 1% other). A post hoc sensitivity power analysis using G*Power (Faul et al., 2007) revealed that we had 80% power to detect effect size f = 0.13 at p < .05 ...

People Who Accommodate Others’ Sexist Views Are Themselves Perceived to Be Sexist
  • Citing Article
  • January 2024

Psychology of Women Quarterly

... For women, looking feminine and pursuing beauty are central aspects of their gender identity (Chrisler, 2013). Highlighting this, Wittlin et al. (2024) found that threats to U.S. women's facial femininity increased anxiety and lowered self-esteem. We built on and expanded these pioneering findings with a German sample by using three different methods to threaten women's physical femininity and measuring the psychological impact. ...

US cisgender women's psychological responses to physical femininity threats: Increased anxiety, reduced self-esteem
  • Citing Article
  • January 2024

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

... Although this idea has been empirically tested predominantly for disadvantaged group members (e.g., Ufkes et al., 2016) and similar results have been obtained, relatively recent studies suggest that advantaged group members may also experience a similar cognitive process due to intergroup contact (Cocco, Vezzali, Stathi et al., 2024). For instance, Vezzali and colleagues (2017) showed a similar pattern regarding the relationship between CII and collective action among advantaged group members. ...

Mobilizing or Sedative Effects? A Narrative Review of the Association Between Intergroup Contact and Collective Action Among Advantaged and Disadvantaged Groups

Personality and Social Psychology Review

... This conflict encompasses a multitude of Sunni groups and religious practices (Tahiiev, 2023). Such disputes have the potential to give rise to tension, discrimination, and even violence (Obaidi et al., 2024). Furthermore, religious controversies also arise between Islamic groups with different styles, such as Nadhatul Ulama (NU) and Muhammadiyah in Indonesia (Hasan, 2023). ...

The personality of violent Jihadists: Examining violent and nonviolent defense of Muslims

Journal of Personality