John C. Turner’s research while affiliated with Macquarie University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (73)


Self-categorization theory and social influence
  • Article

June 2015

·

1,572 Reads

·

301 Citations

John C. Turner

·

Penelope J. Oakes

presents a new theory of group processes and illustrates its application to the related problems of social influence and group polarization self-categorization theory and social identity: social change, social categorization and the interpersonal-intergroup continuum / self-categorization theory: the relationship between personal and social identity / self-categorization and social influence / an explanation of group polarization / some distinctive implications and directions for research (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)


Social identity and personality processes: Non-Aboriginal Australian identity and Neuroticism

March 2012

·

234 Reads

·

15 Citations

European Journal of Social Psychology

·

·

·

[...]

·

There are ongoing debates both in personality psychology and social psychology on the causes and consequences of personality stability and change. Recent work on social roles suggests that as people change roles (e.g. employee to manager), different experiences and demands are internalised into one's self-concept shaping identity and personality. In this paper, the emphasis moves beyond 'roles' to other group memberships (e.g. ethnicity) in shaping one's self-view and self-rated personality (e.g. Neuroticism). The results of two experiments demonstrated that the salience of a particular group membership (as a Non-Aboriginal Australian) did significantly impact on Neuroticism. Such findings suggest that social identity processes may offer a hitherto neglected avenue for helping to explain personality (dis)continuity. Implications of these findings for both fields are discussed.


Prejudice, social identity and social change: resolving the Allportian problematic

January 2012

·

31 Reads

·

20 Citations

The concept of prejudice has profoundly influenced how we have investigated, explained and tried to change intergroup relations of discrimination and inequality. But what has this concept contributed to our knowledge of relations between groups and what has it obscured or misrepresented? How has it expanded or narrowed the horizons of psychological inquiry? How effective or ineffective has it been in guiding our attempts to transform social relations and institutions? In this book, a team of internationally renowned psychologists re-evaluate the concept of prejudice, in an attempt to move beyond conventional approaches to the subject and to help the reader gain a clearer understanding of relations within and between groups. This fresh look at prejudice will appeal to scholars and students of social psychology, sociology, political science and peace studies.


When liking begets solidarity: An experiment on the role of interpersonal attraction in psychological group formation

June 2011

·

137 Reads

·

28 Citations

British Journal of Social Psychology

The role of interpersonal attraction in psychological group formation is discussed. For the social cohesion model attraction and group formation are isomorphic, while the social identity approach identifies self-categorization as the mechanism of group formation. This paper examines the ‘social attraction hypothesis' (Hogg & Turner, 1985) that interpersonal attraction only leads to group formation if it generates an assumption of common category membership that engages a cognitive process of identification. This hypothesis is experimentally tested in a 2(categorization/no categorization) x 2(idiosyncratically likable-dislikable others) x 2(positively/negatively distinctive in-group) factorial design employing the ‘minimal group’ paradigm. As predicted, group formation was found to be a function of the social attractiveness (positive distinctiveness) rather than the personal attractiveness (idiosyncratic likableness) of the in-group members, and categorization (irrespective of affective relations) also generated group behaviour. Complex interactions revealed the unpredicted finding that individuals spontaneously form a group when aggregated with positively distinctive likable others in the absence of an explicit categorization. Social identity readily provides an explanation in terms of perceptions of interpersonal similarities which are transformed into social similarity and thus cause identification. The results of the study strongly favour the social identity approach to group formation.


The significance of the social identity concept for social psychology with reference to individualism, interactionism and social influence

June 2011

·

2,995 Reads

·

755 Citations

British Journal of Social Psychology

The distinctive metatheoretical and empirical significance of the social identity concept for social psychology is outlined with special reference to social influence and group polarization. ‘Individualism’ and ‘interactionism’ are summarized and contrasted as alternative metatheories of social psychology and it is argued that the social identity concept embodies the latter. A social identity theory of group behaviour (and the individual-group relationship) is introduced and from it are derived further theories of social influence and group polarization. The latter is explained as a special case of normal intragroup influence. It is shown how the social identity theory (theories) of group behaviour, social influence and group polarization explicitly assumes a functional interaction between psychological and social processes, in contrast to individualism, and at the same time generates distinctive, testable, empirical predictions. It is concluded that the social identity concept represents a mechanism of social-psychological interaction and as such demonstrates that social psychology need not be an individualistic science.


Stereotyping and social influence: The mediation of stereotype applicability and sharedness by the views of in‐group and out‐group members

June 2011

·

1,958 Reads

·

96 Citations

British Journal of Social Psychology

Since the advent of the ‘cognitive revolution’ in stereotyping research, interest in the consensual nature of stereotypes and its social psychological basis has declined dramatically. Reversing this trend, this paper examines the manner in which the strength of shared stereotypes and the links between particular content and particular groups are mediated by processes of social influence. Two experiments (Ns = 300, 230) are reported in which participants' pre-existing stereotypes about Australians and Americans were confirmed or contradicted by either an in-group or an out-group source. As predicted, this social validation had powerful effects on the perceived applicability of content to groups (Expt 1) as well as stereotype consensus and favourableness (Expt 2). Stereotypes were typically bolstered when they were confirmed by an in-group or contradicted by an out-group, relative to conditions in which they were contradicted by an in-group or confirmed by an out-group. It is argued that the theoretical integration of principles from stereotyping and social influence research offers the potential for a fuller understanding of the inter-subjective, group based properties of stereotypes and stereotype change.


Studies in self-categorization and minority conversion: Is being a member of the out-group an advantage?

June 2011

·

521 Reads

·

101 Citations

British Journal of Social Psychology

The finding that minorities may tend to produce conversion whereas majorities may tend to produce compliance is an extremely important one for the study of social influence. Most research into minority conversion has been based on conversion theory (Moscovici, 1980) which, we would claim, rests on the notion that the ‘true’ influence exerted by minorities is possible only because they are essentially different from self. This paper reports two studies in a programme of research testing an alternative explanation, based on self-categorization theory, which assumes that perceived similarity to a potential source of influence will be the key to its deep and lasting success. Both studies employ a full majority/minority X in-group/out-group design, socially significant real-life in-group–out-group memberships and measure attitudes directly relevant to these social identities. Study 1 uses immediate and delayed and Study 2, public and private, responses as the measures of, respectively, compliance and conversion. The results support self-categorization theory in that, when exposed to both majority and minority out-group sources, subjects exhibited an immediate/public polarization away from the sources, towards a more extreme in-group position, and there was no diminution of the extremity of their position on delayed/private measures. The classic pattern of majorities bringing about greater compliance than conversion and minorities greater conversion than compliance was evinced in the in-group conditions of both studies, although this tended to be comparative rather than absolute. The implications of the results for the conflict between self-categorization theory and conversion theory are discussed and the future direction of our research indicated.


The Group as a Basis for Emergent Stereotype Consensus
  • Article
  • Full-text available

April 2011

·

1,328 Reads

·

150 Citations

European Review of Social Psychology

The fact that stereotypes are shared within groups is essential to stereotype definition and operationalization. Nonetheless, stereotype consensus remains under-researched and under-explained. To address this problem we present a theoretical analysis of the process through which stereotype consensus develops. Derived from self-categorization theory, this argues that consensus is produced by shared social identification and the collective co-ordination of perception and behaviour that flows from it. This analysis is examined in a review of relevant research and in studies where dynamic processes of category representation and social influence are shown to contribute to consensual stereotypes of both out-groups and ingroups.

Download



Citations (72)


... There is reason to believe that such ingroup perceptions may also drive outparty animosity. People's peer groups are integral to how they see themselves (Smith & Henry, 1996), and tend to shape their attitudes, especially when group identification is strong (Haslam et al., 1996;Terry & Hogg, 1996). Research on social norms also indicates that people's behavior is influenced by their perceptions of their ingroup (Cialdini, 2007). ...

Reference:

Perceiving Affective Polarization in the United States: How Social Media Shape Meta-Perceptions and Affective Polarization
Salient Group Memberships and Persuasion: The Role of Social Identity in the Validation of Beliefs
  • Citing Chapter
  • January 1996

... Oftest i en indbyrdes vekselvirkning gør faktorerne individets gruppetilhørsforhold til et holdningsfaenomen -gruppen (af etniske danskere) findes tiltraekkende, fordi den symboliserer det "rigtige". 1 Binding til gruppen udgør også et følelsesengagement, da selvagtelse motiverer mennesker til at gruppere sig (Reynolds & Turner, 2001;Tajfel, 1974). Det skyldes isaer, at tilknytning til en gruppe og dens vaerdigrundlag også øger individets selvagtelse, herunder sympatien mellem gruppens medlemmer. ...

Prejudice as a Group Process: The Role of Social Identity
  • Citing Chapter
  • January 2001

... Psychologists often study how people evaluate and treat different ethnic and cultural groups (and their members) in isolation from one another, across a series of sequential judgments. In the real world, however, people commonly make judgments and decisions over sets of people (and groups, e.g., Biernat & Manis, 1994;Oakes, Haslam, & Turner, 1998;Judd & Park, 1993;Trope & Mackie, 1987;Wyer, Sadler, & Judd, 2002). This process of joint evaluation characterizes many consequential decision-making contexts including hiring, housing, and voting decisions. ...

The Role of Prototypicality in Group Influence and Cohesion: Contextual Variation in the Graded Structure of Social Categories
  • Citing Chapter
  • January 1998

... In contrast to more explicitly negative "traditional" or hostile forms of prejudice, modern/symbolic/ benevolent prejudiced attitudes are often congruent with other broadly accepted group norms and values (e.g., hard work, religious values). Unfortunately, this variability in understanding means that psychologists often end up examining not a single concept, but multiple ones, often with different underlying assumptions about the social and psychological processes underlying them [23]. ...

Prejudice, social identity and social change: resolving the Allportian problematic
  • Citing Chapter
  • January 2012

... These social categories provide individuals with social identity [86,87]. Social identity is what makes people (group members) prone to social influence because group members share the same values and norms ( [103]; see also [104]). Identity leadership refers to a leader as someone who exemplifies, advocates, embraces and promotes a rationality and thinking pattern of the ingroup ( [20,102], see also [22,23]). ...

Identity Confers Power: The New View of Leadership in Social Psychology
  • Citing Chapter
  • November 2008

... Self-categorization theory 62 sees polarization as increasing adherence to group norms, a process accentuated through interaction with outgroups. Within groups, collective norms are transformed through a process of consensual validation 68,69 , but at the same time are also influenced by the broader intergroup context via interaction with relevant outgroups 70 . This means that stronger group identification, often in combination with other factors such as perceived outgroup threat, for example, tends to be a strong predictor of polarization 70,71 , including in contexts of partisan polarization 72,73 . ...

When do stereotypes become really consensual? Investigating the group-based dynamics of the consensualization process
  • Citing Article
  • September 1998

European Journal of Social Psychology

... Identification, in turn, increases commitment to and cooperation within groups (Ellemers, 2001), even in troubled times (Haslam et al., 2006). A wealth of research testifies that a salient social identity encourages people from the same group to seek agreement (Haslam, Oakes, Reynolds, & Turner, 1999), coordinate their behavior (Turner & Oakes, 1989), and collaborate with each other (van Knippenberg & Ellemers, 2003). The mere idea of group membership is enough to elicit a sense of duty to help the group and fellow group members (Baron, Ritov, & Greene, 2013). ...

PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOG Y BULLETIN Haslam et al. / SOCIAL IDENTITY AND STEREOTYPE CONSENSUS Social Identity Salience and the Emergence of Stereotype Consensus

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

... Online communities are fundamentally enabled by the human need for social relatedness 16,155 . Social psychological evidence has shown that group formation takes place easily in any contextalso online 2,26,156,157 . This has been shown in both the SIDE and IBRM 4,37 . ...

Social categorization, interpersonal attraction and group formation
  • Citing Article
  • September 1983

British Journal of Social Psychology

... In this sense, much of the research on intergroup communication is based on the broader study of intergroup relations from the field of social psychology (see Reid, 2012) and, more specifically, social identity theory (Tajfel, 1974(Tajfel, , 1981Tajfel and Turner, 1979) and self-categorization theory (Turner et al., 1987(Turner et al., , 1994 Social identity theory (SIT) is based on the premise that people identify to a greater extent with ingroup members who are similar to them and to a lesser extent with outgroup members who are dissimilar from them. Tajfel (1974) argued that in order to act "in terms of group rather than in terms of self," people must first have the ability to differentiate between "us" and "them" (p. ...

Self and Collective: Cognition and Social Context

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

... Thus, they think and behave in terms of their group membership rather than according to their individual identities (K. J. Reynolds et al., 2012;; they are thus more concerned with improving their group as a whole than with personal improvement. ...

Social identity and personality processes: Non-Aboriginal Australian identity and Neuroticism
  • Citing Article
  • March 2012

European Journal of Social Psychology