Jennifer J. Richler’s research while affiliated with Vanderbilt University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (50)


Individual Differences in Object Recognition
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

March 2019

·

282 Reads

·

60 Citations

Psychological Review

Jennifer J. Richler

·

Andrew J. Tomarken

·

·

[...]

·

There is substantial evidence for individual differences in personality and cognitive abilities, but we lack clear intuitions about individual differences in visual abilities. Previous work on this topic has typically compared performance with only 2 categories, each measured with only 1 task. This approach is insufficient for demonstration of domain-general effects. Most previous work has used familiar object categories, for which experience may vary between participants and categories, thereby reducing correlations that would stem from a common factor. In Study 1, we adopted a latent variable approach to test for the first time whether there is a domain-general object recognition ability, o. We assessed whether shared variance between latent factors representing performance for each of 5 novel object categories could be accounted for by a single higher-order factor. On average, 89% of the variance of lower-order factors denoting performance on novel object categories could be accounted for by a higher-order factor, providing strong evidence for o. Moreover, o also accounted for a moderate proportion of variance in tests of familiar object recognition. In Study 2, we assessed whether the strong association across categories in object recognition is due to third-variable influences. We find that o has weak to moderate associations with a host of cognitive, perceptual, and personality constructs and that a clear majority of the variance in and covariance between performance on different categories is independent of fluid intelligence. This work provides the first demonstration of a reliable, specific, and domain-general object recognition ability, and suggest a rich framework for future work in this area.

View access options


How holistic processing of faces relates to cognitive control and intelligence

April 2018

·

190 Reads

·

10 Citations

Attention Perception & Psychophysics

The Vanderbilt Holistic Processing Test for faces (VHPT-F) is the first standard test designed to measure individual differences in holistic processing. The test measures failures of selective attention to face parts through congruency effects, an operational definition of holistic processing. However, this conception of holistic processing has been challenged by the suggestion that it may tap into the same selective attention or cognitive control mechanisms that yield congruency effects in Stroop and Flanker paradigms. Here, we report data from 130 subjects on the VHPT-F, several versions of Stroop and Flanker tasks, as well as fluid IQ. Results suggested a small degree of shared variance in Stroop and Flanker congruency effects, which did not relate to congruency effects on the VHPT-F. Variability on the VHPT-F was also not correlated with Fluid IQ. In sum, we find no evidence that holistic face processing as measured by congruency in the VHPT-F is accounted for by domain-general control mechanisms.


Figure 1. Top panel: Targets for each of the three NOMTs (Greebles, Ziggerins, and Sheinbugs). Bottom panel: Illustration of NOMT test format (learning and test phase) with Ziggerins. 
Table 1 . Descriptive statistics and reliability for the NOMTs and CFMT. For all tests, chance is 33% (24 items correct) and perfect performance is 100% (72 items correct). NOMTs CFMT
Figure 2. Example Vocabulary and Matrices items from the Puzzles and Words test battery. In Vocabulary (top panel) participants are instructed to choose the word that best matches the definition of the word in capitals. In Matrices (bottom panel) participants are instructed to choose the item that best completes the pattern. 
Table 8 shows two ways of calculating a grand average representing correlations across different familiar categories. The first and most intuitive is the
Table 9 . Descriptive statistics and reliability for the NOMTs and CCMT. For all tests, chance is 24 and perfect performance is 72. NOMTs CCMT

+1

General object recognition is specific: Evidence from novel and familiar objects

May 2017

·

851 Reads

·

87 Citations

Cognition

In tests of object recognition, individual differences typically correlate modestly but nontrivially across familiar categories (e.g. cars, faces, shoes, birds, mushrooms). In theory, these correlations could reflect either global, non-specific mechanisms, such as general intelligence (IQ), or more specific mechanisms. Here, we introduce two separate methods for effectively capturing category-general performance variation, one that uses novel objects and one that uses familiar objects. In each case, we show that category-general performance variance is unrelated to IQ, thereby implicating more specific mechanisms. The first approach examines three newly developed novel object memory tests (NOMTs). We predicted that NOMTs would exhibit more shared, category-general variance than familiar object memory tests (FOMTs) because novel objects, unlike familiar objects, lack category-specific environmental influences (e.g. exposure to car magazines or botany classes). This prediction held, and remarkably, virtually none of the substantial shared variance among NOMTs was explained by IQ. Also, while NOMTs correlated nontrivially with two FOMTs (faces, cars), these correlations were smaller than among NOMTs and no larger than between the face and car tests themselves, suggesting that the category-general variance captured by NOMTs is specific not only relative to IQ, but also, to some degree, relative to both face and car recognition. The second approach averaged performance across multiple FOMTs, which we predicted would increase category-general variance by averaging out category-specific factors. This prediction held, and as with NOMTs, virtually none of the shared variance among FOMTs was explained by IQ. Overall, these results support the existence of object recognition mechanisms that, though category-general, are specific relative to IQ and substantially separable from face and car recognition. They also add sensitive, well-normed NOMTs to the tools available to study object recognition.


Figure 1. Example target parts used in the VPWT. When the target part included hair, it was cropped in the study face. The top row shows top two-thirds, half and third from left to right; middle row shows bottom two-thirds, half and third from left to right; bottom row shows eyes, nose and mouth from left to right. 
Table 2 . Summary statistics for the tests used in Study 4 (N = 104). Mean SD Reliability
Limited evidence of individual differences in holistic processing in different versions of the part-whole paradigm

March 2017

·

291 Reads

·

26 Citations

Attention Perception & Psychophysics

The part-whole paradigm was one of the first measures of holistic processing and it has been used to address several topics in face recognition, including its development, other-race effects, and more recently, whether holistic processing is correlated with face recognition ability. However the task was not designed to measure individual differences and it has produced measurements with low reliability. We created a new holistic processing test designed to measure individual differences based on the part-whole paradigm, the Vanderbilt Part Whole Test (VPWT). Measurements in the part and whole conditions were reliable, but, surprisingly, there was no evidence for reliable individual differences in the part-whole index (how well a person can take advantage of a face part presented within a whole face context compared to the part presented without a whole face) because part and whole conditions were strongly correlated. The same result was obtained in a version of the original part-whole task that was modified to increase its reliability. Controlling for object recognition ability, we found that variance in the whole condition does not predict any additional variance in face recognition over what is already predicted by performance in the part condition.



(A) Example VHPT-F trials where the top face half is the target. The correct response is the face on the left. On congruent trials, the target part is paired with the distractor as during study. On incongruent trials, the target part is paired with a new distractor part, and the distractor part from the study face is paired with a foil (the task-irrelevant part of the study face is outlined in blue here for illustrative purposes only). (B) Example composite task trials. In these examples, the bottom is the target part and the correct response is “same.” On congruent trials, the target and distractor face halves are associated with the same response (“same” in this example). On incongruent trials, the face halves are associated with different responses (in this example, the bottom half is “same” but the top half is “different”).
Mean performance for congruent and incongruent trials in the (A) VHPT-F and (B) composite task. Error bars show 95% confidence intervals for within-subject effects (Loftus and Masson, 1994).
Mean performance for all holistic processing measures. (A) Accuracy on congruent and incongruent trials in the VHPT-F. (B) Sensitivity (d’) as a function of congruency and alignment in the composite task. (C) Accuracy for same-incongruent aligned and misaligned trials in the composite task. Error bars show 95% confidence intervals for within-subject effects (Loftus and Masson, 1994).
Correlation between age (years) and accuracy on congruent (Left) and incongruent (Right) VHPT-F trials.
Validation of the Vanderbilt Holistic Face Processing Test

November 2016

·

122 Reads

·

10 Citations

The Vanderbilt Holistic Face Processing Test (VHPT-F) is a new measure of holistic face processing with better psychometric properties relative to prior measures developed for group studies (Richler et al., 2014). In fields where psychologists study individual differences, validation studies are commonplace and the concurrent validity of a new measure is established by comparing it to an older measure with established validity. We follow this approach and test whether the VHPT-F measures the same construct as the composite task, which is group-based measure at the center of the large literature on holistic face processing. In Experiment 1, we found a significant correlation between holistic processing measured in the VHPT-F and the composite task. Although this correlation was small, it was comparable to the correlation between holistic processing measured in the composite task with the same faces, but different target parts (top or bottom), which represents a reasonable upper limit for correlations between the composite task and another measure of holistic processing. These results confirm the validity of the VHPT-F by demonstrating shared variance with another measure of holistic processing based on the same operational definition. These results were replicated in Experiment 2, but only when the demographic profile of our sample matched that of Experiment 1.


The Vanderbilt Car Memory Test (VCMT)

September 2016

·

77 Reads

Journal of Vision

Several studies using reliable measures of individual differences in object recognition with several object categories find that car recognition ability shares little to no variance with other object recognition abilities. In fact, car recognition is at least as independent from the recognition of other non-face categories as face recognition. Consequently, using cars to represent non-face objects may be problematic, especially when only one non-face object category is used. While the relative independence of car recognition ability has been replicated in several datasets, this result has only been obtained using learning measures that repeat stimuli across trials. This learning component common to every test in these datasets may moderate correlations between tests. For example, it may be that greater experience with cars leads to faster learning over trials within this category, leading to a dissociation with other categories that are learned more slowly. To test if the independence of car recognition generalizes outside of learning tasks, we created the Vanderbilt Car Memory Test (VCMT), which does not repeat stimuli across trials. The VCMT is modeled after the Vanderbilt Face Memory Test (VFMT). Each trial begins with 2 car images presented for 4 seconds, followed by a 3-AFC. We honed the test through 3 iterations to produce good internal reliability (approximately .85) with an average accuracy of approximately 60% (SD = 12%). This test, and similar tests with other non-face categories, will be useful to evaluate accounts of why car recognition ability is independent from the recognition of most other object categories. Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2016


Face Perception

January 2016

·

173 Reads

Face perception is a critical and complex cognitive operation, and it poses unique the cognitive demands. This chapter addresses the question of whether face perception can be viewed as a cognitive phenotype. Evidence from neurophysiological and neuropsychological studies are summarized that reflect specialization of parts of the visual system for face processing, for example, face-tuned neurons in the superior temporal sulcus of non-human primates, and cortical regions associated with prosopagnosia in humans. Data from cognitive neuroscience (especially functional imaging studies) are presented, illustrating that many distinct brain regions show that activity in response to faces though the lateral fusiform gyrus or “fusiform face area” (FFA) shows a particularly robust response. Current interpretations of FFA activity and how it may be functionally parsed out from the activity of the occipital fusiform area and anterior temporal lobe are then laid out, and this raises the tricky question of what differs between the perception faces and non-face objects as may be expressed along these neural centers. Our discussion on face perception as a potential cognitive phenotype suggests that the domain of operation of the face perception neural mechanism is not all or none. We point instead to a more general purpose visual learning system that happens to be critical in face perception and possibly most fully realized in face perception. The processes of face perception are especially illustrative of difficulties that can be inherent in establishing evidence for a domain-specific cognitive phenotype.


Figure 5. Density plots of subject age distributions for each of the tests discussed here: VET-car from Lee et al. (2015) and CFMT from Cho et al. (2015). 
The Vanderbilt Face Matching Test (VFMT 1.0)

September 2015

·

611 Reads

·

2 Citations

Journal of Vision

The Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT, Duchaine & Nakayama, 2006) is a measure of face recognition ability. It was designed to prevent use of salient diagnostic features, problematic in older tests, and is expected to promote holistic processing, a hallmark strategy of face recognition. However, the actual strategy used on the CFMT has not been directly tested. Attempts to correlate CFMT performance with holistic processing have produced conflicting results, but holistic processing measurements are often unreliable. A recent high-powered study with the first test designed for reliable individual differences in the measurement of holistic processing (the Vanderbilt Holistic Face Processing Test, VHPT-F; Richler et al., 2014, reliability ~.6) found no relationship between holistic processing and the CFMT. Unlike previous measures of holistic processing, faces do not repeat across trials in the VHPT-F, so we hypothesize that the correlation between the CFMT and prior holistic processing measures may stem from stimulus repetition. Because stimuli repeat in the CFMT (6 target faces are repeatedly tested), it is possible that the face learning ability measured by the CFMT is most relevant when only a handful of faces are discriminated across trials, reducing the relevant dimensions and promoting a part-based strategy. We created a new test of face matching ability similar to the CFMT but without face repetition. On each trial, 2 faces are studied for 4s, followed by a 3-AFC. We collected data from 100 subjects for two separate forms of the VFMT1.0 (each 48 trials) in an online sample. It revealed good performance on catch trials, a good spread of item difficulties, average performance of 60% (SD = 20%), and reliability of .7 for both versions. In future work, we will use item analyses to refine the test, and explore correlations with CFMT, the Vanderbilt Expertise Test with objects, and the VHPT-F. Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2015.


Citations (44)


... How contents are represented in these rhythms, and how these representations interact with visual representations during perception, need to be explored in future research. The study of visual content representations may also prove fruitful in understanding visual information coding across overarching brain states [88], traits of individual observers [89], and pathological alterations of vision [90] (see Outstanding questions). ...

Reference:

Decoding the rhythmic representation and communication of visual contents
Individual Differences in Object Recognition

Psychological Review

... Top-down models focus on the overall regional situation and are able to consider the regional global objectives to obtain a series of optimal solutions. These models mainly include mathematical models such as linear programming and objective programming and intelligent algorithms such as genetic algorithms and particle swarm algorithms, which are characterized by large openness, high efficiency, and strong problem optimization solving ability [19][20][21] . According to previous studies 22 , the demand for 3D digital technology is increasing alongside the advancements in digital earth and digital city development. ...

How holistic processing of faces relates to cognitive control and intelligence
  • Citing Article
  • September 2018

Journal of Vision

... Several studies have documented an advantage in the processing of faces of similar age to that of the observer, resulting in an own-age bias for face perception (for a meta-analysis and review see Rhodes & Anastasi, 2012). This keeps with the notion that, beyond reflecting general cognitive development (Gauthier et al., 2018), holistic processing is also influenced by domain-specific expertise with a particular class of stimuli (Chua & Gauthier, 2020;Le Grand et al., 2004). Whether the development of configural and holistic processing of others' bodies is influenced by the age similarity between the observer and the model, however, has not been previously explored. ...

How holistic processing of faces relates to cognitive control and intelligence

Attention Perception & Psychophysics

... Participants completed an online cognitive assessment measuring processing speed, visual short-term memory, attention, cognitive control, working memory, perception, general intelligence, and verbal reasoning. Six tests from the TestMyBrain digital platform 32,33 were used: (1) TestMyBrain Digit Symbol Matching; [33][34][35] (2) TestMyBrain Gradual Onset Continuous Performance Test; 33,36,37 (3) TestMyBrain Verbal Paired Associates; 33,38 (4) TestMyBrain Visual Paired Associates; 33 (5) TestMyBrain Multiple Object Tracking; 37 (6) TestMyBrain Vocabulary 35,39 (eTable 2 describes the tests and the cognitive domains covered by each test). Both Verbal and Visual Paired tests are delayed recall tests (2-3 min for verbal pairs, 4-5 min for visual pairs) that have been used in prior studies of visual and verbal long-term memory 33,40 . ...

General object recognition is specific: Evidence from novel and familiar objects

Cognition

... Proficient face recognizers are known to process faces differently than their less-proficient counterparts (Bobak et al., 2016;Duchaine & Nakayama, 2006;Russell et al., 2009). Proficient recognizers rely more on holistic processing than other face-processing strategies (DeGutis et al., 2013;Sunday et al., 2017;Wang et al., 2012). However, as masks inhibit holistic processing (Freud et al., 2020;Guerra et al., 2022), proficient performers may increasingly rely on piecemeal (i.e., feature-by-feature) sampling techniques. ...

Limited evidence of individual differences in holistic processing in different versions of the part-whole paradigm

Attention Perception & Psychophysics

... In both experiments, holistic processing was indexed by the congruency effect in a face composite task, similar to the complete design of the composite task (Richler et al., 2011(Richler et al., , 2012. Following the Vanderbilt Holistic Processing Task which aims to measure individual differences Wang et al., 2016), only aligned, but not misaligned, composites were included in this study, because aligned trials accounted for most of the variance in the task (Horry et al., 2015;Wang et al., 2016). Holistic processing was revealed by better or faster performance for congruent than incongruent trials (e.g., Cheung & Gauthier, 2010;Curby et al., 2012;Richler et al., 2011). ...

Validation of the Vanderbilt Holistic Face Processing Test

... Previous studies (e.g., Towler et al., 2019;Towler et al. 2021) have addressed this issue by separating existing face-matching tests (e.g., Glasgow Face Matching Test Mackenzie, Jennifer, & Isabel, 2015, Burton, White, & McNeill, 2010Expertise in Facial Comparison Test [EFCT], White, Phillips, Hahn, Hill, & O'Toole, 2015) and image sets (e.g., Good, Bad, and Ugly [GBU], Phillips et al., 2012) into subsets of equal difficulty. Face matching is the most commonly used task for assessing the proficiency of professional face examiners. ...

The Vanderbilt Face Matching Test (VFMT 1.0)

Journal of Vision

... However, as mentioned earlier, the link between holistic processing and FPA remains a subject of debate. Numerous definitions (Maurer et al., 2002) and measures (DeGutis et al., 2013;Richler et al., 2015) of holistic processing have been proposed in recent decades, and unfortunately, none has reached complete consensus. In this context, we have chosen the approach proposed by Gold et al. (2012) because it allows a formal comparison of part-based processing with one possible definition of holistic processingnamely, that the whole is greater than the sum of its parts (Farah et al., 1998). ...

About-face on face recognition ability and holistic processing

Journal of Vision

... However, in a recent large study of 46 DPs, Gray et al. [15] found that, compared with controls, the DP group scored significantly worse (Cohen's d = 0.49) on the Cambridge Car Memory Test (CCMT; [15,16]). The CCMT is a sensitive and widely used test of familiar object recognition that matches the Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT; [17]) in format, and cars are a category of objects for which participants typically have very high levels of experience and expertise [18]. Similar-sized decrements in car recognition have also been observed in smaller samples of DPs (e.g. ...

The contribution of general object recognition abilities to face recognition
  • Citing Article
  • August 2012

Journal of Vision

... Most of these definitions do not converge (Fitousi, 2015;Rezlescu et al., 2017). For example, when tested in Garner's speeded classification paradigm (Garner, 1974), top and bottom parts of composite faces appear as separable rather than integral dimensions (Pomerantz et al., 2003;Richler et al., 2013;Fitousi, 2015). Similarly, under matched discriminability, global and local dimensions in Navon figures, are found to be separable (non-interacting) (Pomerantz, 1983). ...

The effects of varying configuration in the composite task support an attentional account of holistic processing
  • Citing Article
  • June 2013

Visual Cognition