Jean-Michel Hupé’s research while affiliated with Université Toulouse III - Paul Sabatier and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (59)


Effondrement sociologique ou la panique morale d’un sociologue
  • Article

February 2022

·

12 Reads

·

1 Citation

Politix

Jean-Michel Hupé

·

Jérôme Lamy

·

Arnaud Saint-Martin

Avec Apocalypse cognitive , le sociologue Gérald Bronner entend traiter un problème grave : notre attention serait vampirisée par les moyens modernes de communication et d’échanges. Internet et les réseaux sociaux numériques, en particulier, auraient pris d’assaut notre « cerveau ancestral ». Cet univers numérique, fonctionnant selon lui à la façon d’un « marché libre » et « dérégulé », accaparerait des esprits rendus faibles par des contenus de qualité douteuse. G. Bronner entend démontrer en quoi l’activité en ligne révèle (sens premier de l’apocalypse du titre) les limites et faiblesses de notre fonctionnement cérébral et menace la démocratie et la civilisation. L’auteur a recours au registre alarmiste de l’« apocalypse » et distille, au gré d’une démonstration qui se veut rigoureuse, des recommandations pour sortir de l’ornière. Apocalypse cognitive ne s’adresse pas spécialement à un lectorat académique. Cet essai relève plutôt de l’intervention et de l’alerte. Pourtant, il prétend s’appuyer sur des connaissances scientifiques et son auteur ne cesse de faire appel à l’autorité des neurosciences pour accréditer des vues souvent très personnelles. Et c’est en cela que le bât blesse. G. Bronner enchaîne les affirmations péremptoires sur la « nature humaine », fonde ses interprétations sur des données biaisées, et déforme les résultats des neurosciences pour les ajuster à des opinions très tranchées. Nous documentons dans cette lecture critique en quoi cet usage de l’autorité scientifique est questionnable, sans épuiser l’inventaire des mésinterprétations et erreurs contenues dans le livre (une annexe, déposée sur l’archive ouverte HAL, en liste l’essentiel). Fourre-tout qui ne résiste pas à l’épreuve de l’argumentation scientifique, Apocalypse cognitive offre donc non pas un diagnostic sérieux mais une incantation moraliste, mobilisable dans le champ politique.


Synesthesia in children with difficulties in written language learning

July 2020

·

88 Reads

·

4 Citations

Consciousness and Cognition

We tested whether the acquisition of grapheme-color synesthesia during childhood is related to difficulties in written language learning by measuring whether it is more frequent in 79 children receiving speech and language therapy for such difficulties than in the general population of children (1.3%). By using criteria as similar as possible to those used in the reference study (Simner et al., 2009), we did not identify any synesthete (Bayesian 95% credible interval [0, 4.5]% for a flat prior). The odds of the null model (no difference between 0/79 and 1.3%) over alternative models is 28 (Bayes Factor). A higher prevalence of grapheme-color synesthetes among children with learning difficulties is therefore very unlikely, questioning the hypothesis of a link between synesthesia and difficulties in language acquisition. We also describe the difficulty of diagnosing synesthesia in children and discuss the need for new approaches to do so.


An Introduction to Synesthesia via Vladimir Nabokov

June 2020

·

275 Reads

·

1 Citation

Nabokov belongs to a small “family” of great artists also known as synesthetes, suggesting a link between synesthesia and creativity. Moreover, Nabokov’s report of precise synesthetic associations between letters and colors already in his early childhood suggests that synesthesia is a gift, possibly innate. Could we suppose that the brain of Nabokov was special because of synesthesia? Could synesthesia relate to his metaphoric creativity, his strong visual mental imagery and memory or even some aspects of his personality? Here we show that the synesthetic experiences described by Nabokov are similar to those of synesthetes studied by scientists for the last twenty years. We review what cognitive neuroscience has learnt so far about the possible mechanisms of synesthesia and whether synesthetes display any special characteristics beyond synesthesia. We show that little is known and that there is little support to naturalize Nabokov’s creative process on the basis of him having synesthesia.


Letters and digits used for each synaesthete, with their corresponding synaesthetic RGB colours (the rendering of the colours using the projector in the scanner was different) [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
Colour coordinates in the CIE L*a*b* space of the stimuli used for each synaesthete, corresponding to the idiosyncratic synaesthetic colours of letters (+) and digits (x). The colours of the crosses are arbitrary and correspond to the four categories the classifiers had to distinguish. The size of the crosses is proportional to luminance (marker size = 0.4 * L, where max(L) = 100; axes limits are ± 130, possible range being −128 to +127) [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
Multivariate (multivoxel) pattern analysis classifications. “Col” classification: The procedure was leave‐one‐run‐out. Six classifiers were trained to classify 60 colour exemplars from 5 runs in four categories, and tested on 12 independent exemplars of the remaining run. Performance was therefore averaged over seventy‐two classifications (6 classifiers * 12 tested exemplars). “Syn” classification: The procedure was the same as for the “Col” classification, based on pairs of graphemes and therefore also synaesthetic colours for synaesthetes. “C2S” classification: Training was performed by one classifier on six colour runs (seventy‐two exemplars), test on six grapheme runs (seventy‐two classifications). “S2C” classification: The procedure was the same as for the “C2S” classification. “g1g2” classification: Classifiers were trained on letters (3 runs * 12 exemplars) or digits (3 runs) and tested respectively on digits or letters. Overall performance was based on seventy‐two classifications [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
(a) Atlas‐based regions of interest (ROI) of the fusiform region. From left to right, FG1, FG2, FG3 and FG4. Colour gradients denote the probability of being in the specified ROI, from 0% (dark blue) to 100% (dark red). We considered the largest ROI as the mask of the corresponding region. (b) Parietal ROIs. From left to right, AIPS_IP1, AIPS_IP2, AIPS_IP3, IPL_PGa and IPL_PGp. See text for full names and references of these areas [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
Performance of classifiers trained on real colours and tested on letters and digits evoking similar synaesthetic colours in synaesthetes (“C2S” classification), in retinotopic areas and in the fusiform gyrus of synaesthetes (red points) and controls (blue points). Each classifier was trained and tested on beta weights computed on voxels in the native subject space with no spatial smoothing. The y‐axis represents both the performance of the “C2S” classifier (between 0 and 1, chance level = 0.25, thick green line; 95% limits of the binomial distribution of chance for each subject [0.15, 0.35], thin green lines) for individual subjects and their group average (with 95% CI, computed using mixed‐effect generalized linear models: see Methods, Statistical Analysis) and the difference of performance (grey crosses) between synaesthetes and their matched controls (0 = no difference between groups, blue line; whiskers denote 95% CI, computed using paired t tests: see Methods, Statistical Analysis). Each ROI regrouped several areas, for example the left and right parts of V1 for “retV1” in order to provide a large number of voxels in each subject and ROI (at least >60 in each ROI of each subject, and >100 voxels in most ROIs; see Methods: Data Analysis). ROIs have different number of voxels because no voxel selection was applied for this analysis. “retV1” to “retV4” were defined based on retinotopic mapping in each subject; other ROIs were defined as the intersection of the subject's grey matter mask and the mask of atlas‐based anatomical ROIs (Anatomy Toolbox for SPM8) projected into the subject's space (see Figure 4). “FG12L” = left (FG1 + FG2), etc. Please note that the scale of the Y‐axis, adjusted for better visibility, is different than in the next figures [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]

+4

Multivariate pattern analysis of fMRI data for imaginary and real colours in grapheme–colour synaesthesia
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

June 2020

·

21 Reads

·

2 Citations

Grapheme–colour synaesthesia is a subjective phenomenon related to perception and imagination, in which some people involuntarily but systematically associate specific, idiosyncratic colours to achromatic letters or digits. Its investigation is relevant to unravel the neural correlates of colour perception in isolation from low‐level neural processing of spectral components, as well as the neural correlates of imagination by being able to reliably trigger imaginary colour experiences. However, functional MRI studies using univariate analyses failed to provide univocal evidence of the activation of the “colour network” by synaesthesia. Applying multivariate (multivoxel) pattern analysis (MVPA) on 20 synaesthetes and 20 control participants, we tested whether the neural processing of real colours (concentric rings) and synaesthetic colours (black graphemes) shared patterns of activations. Region of interest analyses in retinotopically and anatomically defined visual areas revealed neither evidence of shared circuits for real and synaesthetic colour processing, nor processing difference between synaesthetes and controls. We also found no correlation with individual experiences, characterised by measuring the strength of synaesthetic associations. The whole brain searchlight analysis led to similar results. We conclude that revealing the neural coding of the synaesthetic experience of colours is a hard task which requires the improvement of our current methodology: for example involving more individuals and achieving higher MR signal to noise ratio and spatial resolution. So far, we have not found any evidence of the involvement of the cortical colour network in the subjective experience of synaesthetic colours.

View access options

Shared premotor activity in spoken and written communication

October 2019

·

44 Reads

·

9 Citations

Brain and Language

·

Jean-Michel Hupé

·

Mathieu Ruiz

·

[...]

·

Marc Sato

The aim of the present study was to uncover a possible common neural organizing principle in spoken and written communication, through the coupling of perceptual and motor representations. In order to identify possible shared neural substrates for processing the basic units of spoken and written language, a sparse sampling fMRI acquisition protocol was performed on the same subjects in two experimental sessions with similar sets of letters being read and written and of phonemes being heard and orally produced. We found evidence of common premotor regions activated in spoken and written language, both in perception and in production. The location of those brain regions was confined to the left lateral and medial frontal cortices, at locations corresponding to the premotor cortex, inferior frontal cortex and supplementary motor area. Interestingly, the speaking and writing tasks also appeared to be controlled by largely overlapping networks, possibly indicating some domain general cognitive processing. Finally, the spatial distribution of individual activation peaks further showed more dorsal and more left-lateralized premotor activations in written than in spoken language.


Multivariate pattern analysis of fMRI data for imaginary and real colours in grapheme-colour synaesthesia

September 2019

·

18 Reads

Grapheme-colour synaesthesia is a subjective phenomenon related to perception and imagination, in which some people involuntarily but systematically associate specific, idiosyncratic colours to achromatic letters or digits. Its investigation is relevant to unravel the neural correlates of colour perception in isolation from low-level neural processing of spectral components, as well as the neural correlates of imagination by being able to reliably trigger imaginary colour experiences. However, functional MRI studies using univariate analyses failed to provide univocal evidence of the activation of the ‘colour network’ by synaesthesia. Applying Multivariate (multivoxel) Pattern Analysis (MVPA) on 20 synaesthetes and 20 control participants, we tested whether the neural processing of real colours (concentric rings) and synaesthetic colours (black graphemes) shared patterns of activations. Region of interest analyses in retinotopically and anatomically defined visual regions revealed neither evidence of shared circuits for real and synaesthetic colour processing, nor processing difference between synaesthetes and controls. We also found no correlation with individual experiences, characterised by measuring the strength of synaesthetic associations. The whole brain, searchlight, analysis led to similar results. We conclude that identifying the neural correlates of the synaesthetic experience of colours may still be beyond the reach of present technology and data analysis techniques.


Figure E1. Hypothetical model to explain the decrease of percept duration with increasing speed for depth ordering bistability, if increasing grating speed decreases the input to the neural populations representing transparent motion (ITleft and ITright). Here percept duration depends only on the stability of each interpretation, and therefore on the strength on its input.
Figure E3. Model of plaid bistability if increasing grating speed decreases the input to the neural population representing transparent motion (ITright). Here we suppose that speed does not affect the input to the neural population representing coherent motion (IC) and that there is no coupling.
Figure E4. Model of plaid bistability if increasing grating speed decreases the input to the neural population representing transparent motion (ITright). Here we suppose a coupling mechanisms and that speed does not affect the input to the neural population representing coherent motion (IC).
Figure E5. Model of plaid bistability if increasing grating speed decreases the input to the neural population representing transparent motion (ITright) as well as decreases the input to the neural population representing coherent motion (IC), in the absence of coupling.
Two paradigms of bistable plaid motion reveal independent mutual inhibition processes

April 2019

·

62 Reads

·

3 Citations

Journal of Vision

Perception is sometimes bistable, switching between two possible interpretations. Levelt developed several propositions to explain bistable perception in binocular rivalry, based on a model of competing neural populations connected through reciprocal inhibition. Here we test Levelt's laws with bistable plaid motion. Plaids are typically tristable, either a coherent pattern, transparent with one component in front, or transparent with the opposite depth order. In Experiment 1, we use a large angle between component directions to prevent plaid coherence, limiting the ambiguity to alternations of grating depth order. Similar to increasing contrast in binocular rivalry, increasing component speed led to higher switch rates (analogous to Levelt's fourth proposition). In Experiment 2, we used occlusion cues to prevent one depth order and limit bistability to one transparent depth order alternating with coherence. Increasing grating speed shortened coherent motion periods but left transparent periods largely unchanged (analogous to Levelt's second proposition). Switch dynamics showed no correlation between the experiments. These data suggest that plaid component speed acts like contrast in binocular rivalry to vary switch dynamics through a mutual inhibition model. The lack of correlation between both experiments suggests reciprocal inhibition mediates bistability between a variety of neural populations across the visual system.


Magnetic resonance imaging does not reveal structural alterations in the brain of grapheme-color synesthetes

April 2018

·

165 Reads

·

18 Citations

Several publications have reported structural changes in the brain of synesthetes compared to controls, either local differences or differences in connectivity. In the present study, we pursued this quest for structural brain differences that might support the subjective experience of synesthesia. In particular, for the first time in this field, we investigated brain folding in comparing 45 sulcal shapes in each hemisphere of control and grapheme-color synesthete populations. To overcome flaws relative to data interpretation based only on p-values, common in the synesthesia literature, we report confidence intervals of effect sizes. Moreover, our statistical maps are displayed without introducing the classical, but misleading, p-value level threshold. We adopt such a methodological procedure to facilitate appropriate data interpretation and promote the “New Statistics” approach. Based on structural or diffusion magnetic resonance imaging data, we did not find any strong cerebral anomaly, in sulci, tissue volume, tissue density or fiber organization that could support synesthetic color experience. Finally, by sharing our complete datasets, we strongly support the multi-center construction of a sufficient large dataset repository for detecting, if any, subtle brain differences that may help understanding how a subjective experience, such as synesthesia, is mentally constructed.



S2 Fig

April 2018

·

12 Reads

Surface based morphometry. Differences in 48 controls vs. 32 synesthetes for cortical thickness (mm), area (mm2) and volume (mm3) for left and right hemispheres (Cluster-forming threshold p<0.001). For each parameter, regions where differences were detected are indicated. No region survived the FWEc at the cluster level. (PDF)


Citations (45)


... Cette thèse, mobilisée par certains travaux de sociologie des croyances (Bronner, 2013), émane de la psychologie cognitive et postule l'inadaptation du fonctionnement cérébral humain aux modalités de la communication numérique. Cette problématisation des croyances humaines a suscité de vives discussions (Boullier, 2021 ;Huppé et al., 2021) : elle nourrit la dispute entre approches sociologiques et neurosciences et elle éclaire aussi la fabrique de l'interdisciplinarité via la sociologie cognitive. Cette controverse semble totalement méconnue des acteurs politiques, des acteurs scientifiques biomédicaux et des acteurs médiatiques, a contrario de l'approche cognitive et individualisante, reconnue et très médiatisée. ...

Reference:

Doxa de l’« acceptabilité sociale » contre la santé publique: La « démocratie sanitaire » à l’épreuve de la pandémie de Covid-19 et au-delà
Effondrement sociologique ou la panique morale d’un sociologue
  • Citing Article
  • February 2022

Politix

... Two previous MEG studies (7,8) focused on which brain areas of single synesthetes process synesthetic colors (as have other neuroimaging studies with single cases or groups of synesthetes [e.g., refs. [9][10][11]). In contrast, we use a whole-brain multivariate approach that does not rely on selection of specific sensors, brain regions, or time points. ...

Multivariate pattern analysis of fMRI data for imaginary and real colours in grapheme–colour synaesthesia

... Although this hypothesis has not been directly tested in humans, meaning that no one has compared in the same subjects the neural pathways underlying speech (i.e., laryngeal movements) and dance movements (e.g., rhythmic arm movements), cross-studies' comparison points to an overlap between several of the regions controlling body movements in the primary motor cortex with the regions that control laryngeal movements in the primary motor cortex [28][29][30]. Further, dance has been found to increase network connectivity between the basal ganglia and premotor cortices [31], both of which are co-activated during speech [32,33]. ...

Shared premotor activity in spoken and written communication
  • Citing Article
  • October 2019

Brain and Language

... For this reason, there are no current mouse models of binocular rivalry, however there are monocular variants of visual rivalry, namely plaid perception, that can be studied the mouse model [7,66]. Crucially, plaid perception, like binocular rivalry, conforms to Levelt's laws [56,67] and also has a right skewed distribution of dominance durations that is well fit by a Gamma distribution [66]. We hypothesise, therefore, that the principles underlying the simulation of binocular rivalry in our model will also describe other forms of visual rivalry (such as plaid perception), offering a plausible means to test cellular level predictions derived through simulation. ...

Two paradigms of bistable plaid motion reveal independent mutual inhibition processes

Journal of Vision

... However, more recent studies have produced contrasting results regarding the structural differences. For instance, employing VBM, DTI, and sulcus-based morphometry methods with the "New Statistics" approach, Dojat et al. (2018) found no structural cerebral differences in 10 or 22 grapheme-color synesthetes (in the 2nd part of the study) compared to 25 control subjects. In contrast, Arend et al. (2018) showed increased gray matter in 19 synesthetes compared to controls in the left cerebellum, as well as in parts of the limbic system, namely the right amygdala. ...

Magnetic resonance imaging does not reveal structural alterations in the brain of grapheme-color synesthetes

... They are crucial for many other aspects of cognition, including motor learning (Brooks, 1986), any comparison of sensory input to predictions or internal state (e.g., novelty detection in the hippocampus; Kumaran & Maguire, 2007) and STM tasks such as delayed match to sample tasks (Cope et al., 2018;Engel & Wang, 2011). Accordingly, grammar-like rules based on sameness/difference relations can be learned in many nonlinguistic domains in humans (Dawson & Gerken, 2009;Endress, Dehaene-Lambertz, & Mehler, 2007;Marcus, Fernandes, & Johnson, 2007;Saffran, Pollak, Seibel, & Shkolnik, 2007) and by many nonhuman animals (Versace, Spierings, Caffini, Ten Cate, & Vallortigara, 2017;Martinho & Kacelnik, 2016;Smirnova, Zorina, Obozova, & Wasserman, 2015;de la Mora & Toro, 2013;Neiworth, 2013;Hauser & Glynn, 2009;Murphy, Mondragón, & Murphy, 2008;Pepperberg, 1987; but see Hupé, 2017;Langbein & Puppe, 2017;van Heijningen, Visser, Zuidema, & ten Cate, 2009), possibly through a specialized sameness detector (Endress, 2013;Endress et al., 2007) that might exist from birth (Gervain, Berent, & Werker, 2012;Gervain, Macagno, Cogoi, Peña, & Mehler, 2008;Antell, Caron, & Myers, 1985). The computations underlying sameness/ difference relations thus reflect a core linguistic mechanism whose systems-level implementation might be tractable due to its evolutionary history. ...

Comment on “Ducklings imprint on the relational concept of ‘same or different'”
  • Citing Article
  • February 2017

Science

... The power of classification algorithms depends on both the number and quality (signal to noise ratio) of estimates (called exemplars). The present compromise between quantity and quality was based on (Mumford, Turner, Ashby, & Poldrack, 2012) and on preliminary experiments (Ruiz, Hupé, & Dojat, 2012). Subjects had to fixate the centre of the screen (the fixation point, present between stimuli and at the centre of the coloured rings, or the centre of the grapheme) and pay attention to the stimuli for the whole duration of each run. ...

Use of Pattern-Information Analysis in Vision Science: A Pragmatic Examination
  • Citing Conference Paper
  • October 2012

Lecture Notes in Computer Science

... the implications of synaesthetic -like experiences happening with non-synaesthetes are manifold, and in particular we noted that synaesthesia has been shown to enhance memory recall (Bankieris & aslin, 2016;gosavi & hubbard, 2019;mulvenna et al., 2013;Paivio, 1969;smilek et al., 2002), foster creativity and problem-solving (chun & hupé, 2016;Julmi & scherm, 2015;merter, 2017;mulvenna, 2007;mulvenna et al., 2013;sitton & Pierce, 2004;Ward et al., 2008), increase curiosity and exploration (Domino, 1989;lingham, 2013;murgia & murgia, 2015;rothen & meier, 2010), and heighten sensitivity to stimuli, both internal and external (Banissy et al., 2009;Bowling et al., 2019;hossain et al., 2018;lebeau & richer, 2022). ...

Are synesthetes exceptional beyond their synesthetic associations? A systematic comparison of creativity, personality, cognition, and mental imagery in synesthetes and controls

... In the brain, this is dependant on the interplay of conduction velocity and the neural membrane time constant. Specifically, the membrane time constant, the characteristic time of the exponential decay of a neuron back to its resting state potential, is on the order of 10 ms 60 , while local traveling waves have been measured to propagate at roughly 0.1 to 0.8 m/s 15,59 . The spatiotemporal organization of neural activity into waves then makes sense as a result of this timescale imbalance and the distances signals must travel. ...

Feedforward and Feedback Connections Between Areas V1 and V2 of the Monkey Have Similar Rapid Conduction Velocities
  • Citing Article
  • April 2001

Journal of Neurophysiology