November 2024
·
20 Reads
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
November 2024
·
20 Reads
September 2024
·
40 Reads
It has been suggested that having a reputation for being prosocial is a critical part of social status across all human societies. It has also been argued that prosocial behavior confers benefits, whether physiological, such as stress reduction, or social, such as building allies or becoming more popular. Here, we investigate the relationship between helping reputation (being named as someone others would go to for help), and hair-derived chronic stress (hair cortisol concentration). In a sample of 77 women and 62 men, we found that perceived helping reputation was not related to chronic stress. Overall, the results of our study suggest that, in an egalitarian society with fluid camp membership and widely practiced generosity such as the Hadza, helping reputation does not necessarily boost stress-related health benefits through prestige-signaling mechanisms observed in hierarchical, large-scale societies.
July 2024
·
21 Reads
·
1 Citation
American Journal of Human Biology
In recent years there has been much interest in investigating the extent to which social status or prestige are related to an individual's degree of integration in social networks. It has been shown that, among hunter‐gatherers, social characteristics of an individual based on social status or prestige, such foraging reputation, friendship popularity, and pro‐social reputation, can influence the extent to which an individual is embedded in a social network. However, little is known regarding the extent to which height, a physical trait that in Western societies is often associated with social status, is associated in integration in social networks among small‐scale hunter gatherers. Here, we investigated the relationship between height and a position an individual occupies in proximity networks among Hadza men ( n = 30), hunter‐gatherers living in Northern Tanzania. The results of our study show that height is not related to the position an individual maintains in proximity networks. We argue that, in a relatively egalitarian small‐scale hunter‐gatherer societies such as the Hadza, social interactions driving proximity networks might be influenced by social traits, such as popularity and hunting reputation, rather than physical traits, such as height.
June 2024
·
66 Reads
·
2 Citations
PNAS Nexus
Behavioural research in traditional subsistence populations is often conducted in a non-native language. Recent studies show that non-native language-use systematically influences behaviour, including in widely-used methodologies. However, such studies are largely conducted in rich, industrialised societies, using at least one European language. This study expands sample diversity. We presented four standard tasks ― a ‘dictator’ game, two sacrificial dilemmas, a wager task and five Likert- risk tolerance measures ― to 129 Hadza participants. We randomly varied study languages ― Hadzane and Kiswahili ― between participants. We report a moderate impact of study language on wager decisions, alongside a substantial effect on dilemma decisions and responses to Likert-assessments of risk. As expected, non-native languages fostered utilitarian choices in sacrificial dilemmas. Unlike previous studies, non-native-language-use decreased risk preference in wager and Likert-tasks. We consider alternative explanatory mechanisms to account for this reversal, including linguistic relativity and cultural context. Given the strength of the effects reported here, we recommend, where possible, that future cross-cultural research should be conducted in participants’ first language.
February 2024
·
30 Reads
·
2 Citations
Hunter Gatherer Research
In humans, measures of social integration are usually based on self-reported friendships and interview-derived data. However, such measures do not take into account the actual number of social contacts an individual has, a variable that has been shown to have significant impacts on an individual’s health and well-being. In recent years advances in ‘bio-logging’ – an automated system that registers an individual’s (physical) position relative to others – have made it easier to quantify actual physical proximity between individuals. Here, we use reported friendships and GPS-derived proximity data obtained from 81 adult Hadza men and women living in northern Tanzania to directly compare perceived friendship networks with social networks based on physical proximity. Overall, the results of this study show that even though the pattern of social relationships is similar in both networks (ie individuals tend to have stronger proximity ties with nominated best friends and vice versa), individual measures of social integration, such as the number of social partners and network centrality, do not correspond to each other in the two types of social networks. For example, being central in self-reported friendship networks does not correspond with being central in proximity networks. We discuss these findings in light of study limitations, including a small sample size and challenges regarding comparing networks of different structure.
October 2023
·
290 Reads
·
4 Citations
The function of dreams is a longstanding scientific research question. Simulation theories of dream function, which are based on the premise that dreams represent evolutionary past selective pressures and fitness improvement through modified states of consciousness, have yet to be tested in cross-cultural populations that include small-scale forager societies. Here, we analyze dream content with cross-cultural comparisons between the BaYaka (Rep. of Congo) and Hadza (Tanzania) foraging groups and Global North populations, to test the hypothesis that dreams in forager groups serve a more effective emotion regulation function due to their strong social norms and high interpersonal support. Using a linear mixed effects model we analyzed 896 dreams from 234 individuals across these populations, recorded using dream diaries. Dream texts were processed into four psychosocial constructs using the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC-22) dictionary. The BaYaka displayed greater community-oriented dream content. Both the BaYaka and Hadza exhibited heightened threat dream content, while, at the same time, the Hadza demonstrated low negative emotions in their dreams. The Global North Nightmare Disorder group had increased negative emotion content, and the Canadian student sample during the COVID-19 pandemic displayed the highest anxiety dream content. In conclusion, this study supports the notion that dreams in non-clinical populations can effectively regulate emotions by linking potential threats with non-fearful contexts, reducing anxiety and negative emotions through emotional release or catharsis. Overall, this work contributes to our understanding of the evolutionary significance of this altered state of consciousness.
February 2023
·
88 Reads
·
1 Citation
Human Nature
Folk stories featuring prosocial content are ubiquitous across cultures. One explanation for the ubiquity of such stories is that stories teach people about the local socioecology, including norms of prosociality, and stories featuring prosocial content may increase generosity in listeners. We tested this hypothesis in a sample of 185 Hadza hunter-gatherers. We read participants a story in which the main character either swims with another person (control story) or rescues him from drowning (prosocial story). After hearing the story, participants played a dictator game with dried meat sticks and then were given a recall test of facts presented in the story. There was moderate evidence for a small effect of the prosocial story: participants who heard the prosocial story gave an estimated 0.22 [90% HDI: −0.12–0.57] more meat sticks than those who heard the control story. However, the association between generosity and sex, marital status, and region of residence was stronger; men gave more than women, unmarried participants gave more than married participants, and participants living in a region with more exposure to markets gave more than participants living further from markets. There was no evidence that the prosocial story was more easily recalled than the control story. These results provide some support for the hypothesis that prosocial stories can increase prosociality in listeners, though the effect of hearing a single story is small.
January 2023
·
87 Reads
·
4 Citations
In recent years there has been much research regarding the extent to which social status is related to long-term indices of health. The majority of studies looking at the interplay between social status and health have been conducted in industrialized societies. However, it has been argued that most of human evolution took place in small, mobile and egalitarian hunter-gatherer groups where individuals exhibited very little variation in terms of material wealth or possessions. In this study, we looked at the extent to which two domains of social status, hunting reputation (being perceived as a good hunter) and popularity (being perceived as a friend), are related to physiological stress levels among Hadza men, hunter-gatherers living in Northern Tanzania. The results of our study show that neither hunting reputation nor popularity is associated with stress levels. Overall, our data suggest that, in at least some traditional small-scale societies exhibiting an egalitarian social model, such as the Hadza, the variation in social status measures based on both popularity and hunting reputation does not translate into one of the commonly used indices of wellbeing.
January 2023
·
76 Reads
·
7 Citations
Hormones and Behavior
In recent years there has been a great deal of documentation on how social relationships are related to various aspects of human wellbeing. However, until recently most studies investigating the effects of social relationships on wellbeing have applied social network measures to reported social contacts. Recent advances in the application of bio-loggers in biological studies have now made it possible to quantify social relationships based on in-person, rather than self-reported, social interactions. We used GPS-derived in-camp and out-of-camp proximity data to analyse how in-person proximity is related to Hair Cortisol Concentration (HCC) among Hadza hunter-gatherers. Time spent in close proximity to other camp members was associated with higher HCC, especially in women. In contrast, individuals who spent more time in close out-of-camp proximity to their best friend experienced lower HCC. Our study suggests that physiological costs related to group living might be mitigated by in-person interactions with close friends. We also find that the location (i.e., in-camp vs out-of-camp) of proximity to others and self-perceived friends is associated with HCC among the Hadza.
July 2022
·
36 Reads
·
8 Citations
Humans are motivated to compete for access to valuable social partners, which is a function of their willingness to share and ability to generate resources. However, relative preferences for each trait should be responsive to socioecological conditions. Here, we test the flexibility of partner choice psychology among Hadza hunter–gatherers of Tanzania. Ninety-two Hadza ranked their campmates on generosity and foraging ability and then shared resources with those campmates. We found Hadza with greater exposure to other cultures shared more with campmates ranked higher on generosity, whereas Hadza with lower exposure showed a smaller preference for sharing with generous campmates. This moderating effect was specific to generosity—regardless of exposure, Hadza showed only a small preference for sharing with better foragers. We argue this difference in preferences is due to high exposure Hadza having more experience cooperating with others in the absence of strong norms of sharing, and thus are exposed to greater variance in willingness to cooperate among potential partners increasing the benefits of choosing partners based on generosity. As such, participants place a greater emphasis on choosing more generous partners, highlighting the flexibility of partner preferences.
... This study is a part of a larger project on proximity networks and social status among the Hadza, and some of the data used here (e.g., on cortisol levels or social status) have been previously reported in previous publications 40,69,78,85,118,119 . Data collection was conducted in 2016 and 2017 for roughly four months each year. ...
July 2024
American Journal of Human Biology
... However, some remain skeptical that music specifically causes cooperation more than comparable activities such as language do 4,6,27-30 (but cf. 31 Some concerns are general ones about biases that are increasingly recognised as limiting the validity, replicability, and generalizability of experimental psychology in general [32][33][34][35][36][37] . For example, Rennung & Göritz's meta-analysis 6 found evidence that publication bias (i.e., the tendency for journals to only publish studies showing statistically significant results) has led to inflated effect size estimates of the relationship between synchrony and cooperation. ...
June 2024
PNAS Nexus
... This study is a part of a larger project on proximity networks and social status among the Hadza, and some of the data used here (e.g., on cortisol levels or social status) have been previously reported in previous publications 40,69,78,85,118,119 . Data collection was conducted in 2016 and 2017 for roughly four months each year. ...
February 2024
Hunter Gatherer Research
... This area of the brain becomes less active during dreams (Northoff et al., 2023). There is evidence that the emotional content in dreams may have adaptive functions in control of emotion (Samson et al., 2023). Perhaps the same mechanism explains some autogenic training effects. ...
October 2023
... If prosociality is valued within a particular cultural context, one would expect the reputation for prosociality, as measured by helping reputation, to play a role in overall social status and thereby affect long-term physiological markers of stress, such as hair cortisol. However, using the same dataset we have previously shown that among the Hadza social status based on friendship popularity and foraging reputation is not related to HCC 69,78 . This is because prosocial behavior within the context of risk-pooling theory does not imply direct, prestige-based physiological rewards resulting from prosocial behavior because, as opposed to large-scale hierarchical societies, in small-scale egalitarian huntergatherer groups it is expected that people will engage in such a need-based behavior 25,79 . ...
January 2023
... For example, studies of nonhuman primates have shown that individuals that are actively involved in affiliative interactions, such as grooming, experience lower levels of physiological stress 34,39 . However, the causative direction of such relationship is still debatable 40 . For example, studies among humans have shown that individuals that experienced acute stress were more likely to be engage in prosocial behavior such as sharing and helping 41 , though studies also show that induced stress can also elicit social withdrawal, antisocial behaviour and aggression [42][43][44][45] . ...
January 2023
Hormones and Behavior
... Foreign language effects are salient for anthropologists and other cross-cultural researchers. Many methods customarily used in cross-cultural human behavioural research -giving tasks [e.g., 64,111,95,98], risk tasks [e.g., 41,2,1], and moral dilemmas [e.g., 36,7,96] -are also those which yield the clearest evidence for foreign language effects [26,21,70,25,39,48,56,9]. Moreover, widely-spoken lingua francae or other national majority languages are often used in cross-cultural research, and are expedient. ...
July 2022
... Although the Hadza do not engage in norm enforcement to the degree seen in many other societies 80 , prosocial behavior, such as food sharing, is widespread and commonly practiced 81,82 . Hadza social preferences while directing prosocial behavior have also been documented [82][83][84] as has been social prestige resulting from either friendship popularity or foraging reputation 69,78,85 . Moreover, the unpredictability of food resources, especially those acquired by men such as highly valued large game and honey 86,87 , make it very plausible that risk pooling plays a major role in prosocial behavior among the Hadza 25 . ...
October 2021
Behavioral Ecology
... Studies of children, especially from a cross-cultural perspective, also reveal high levels of peer-to-peer teaching and learning (Lancy et al., 2010). While children do report that adultsand specifically, their parents-are the primary drivers of knowledge transmission (Kline et al., 2013;Lew-Levy, Ringen, et al., 2021;Schniter et al., 2015), experimental and observational research nonetheless suggests that peer teaching is widespread (Boyette & Hewlett, 2017;Lew-Levy et al., 2020;Maynard, 2002). Similarly, studies conducted in communities outside the postindustrialized West (Maya, Tsimane, NiVanuatu) show that the majority of linguistic input received in early childhood actually comes from other children (Cristia et al., 2019(Cristia et al., , 2023Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, 2012, see also Labov, 1964). ...
May 2021
Human Nature
... Juveniles, as members of a group, also have their own energetic constraints during infancy, childhood, and adolescence (Blurton Jones et al., 1989;Crittenden et al., 2013;Hill & Hurtado, 2017;Kramer, 2005;Mace, 2000). Moreover, infants, toddlers, children and adolescents have an important influence on the group mobility strategy because of their size, physiology, and kinematic limitations (Cavagna et al., 1983;Crittenden et al., 2021;Froehle et al., 2013;Miner et al., 2014;Saibene & Minetti, 2003). ...
March 2021
Human Nature