Gordon Hodson’s research while affiliated with Brock University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (165)


Uncertainly Orientation in the Group Context: Categorization Effects on Persuasive Message Processing
  • Article

July 2003

·

29 Reads

·

16 Citations

Gordon Hodson

·

Richard M Sorrentino

Persuasive messages often originate from in-group or out-group sources. Theoretically, in-group categories could facilitate heuristic-based message processing (because of the attractiveness of in-groups and their social reality cues) or systematic-based processing (because of high personal relevance of the message). The authors expected individual differences in uncertainty orientation and socially based expectancy congruence to be important variables in understanding these processes. Participants were exposed to strong or weak, in-group or out-group messages that were either expectancy congruent (in-group agreement, out-group disagreement) or expectancy incongruent (in-group disagreement, out-group agreement). As predicted, uncertainty-oriented participants increased systematic information processing under incongruent conditions relative to congruent (i.e., relatively certain) conditions; certainty-oriented individuals processed systematically only under congruent conditions. These findings suggest that uncertainty that has been created through social-categorization conflicts is treated differently by people of different personality styles.


The theory of uncertainty orientation: A mathematical reformulation

April 2003

·

235 Reads

·

50 Citations

Journal of Mathematical Psychology

Richard M Sorrentino

·

·

Gordon Hodson

·

[...]

·

In this paper, we present a mathematical formulation derived from the theory of uncertainty orientation (In R.M. Sorrehtino, & E.T. Higgins (Eds.), the handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behaviour, Vol. 1 (pp. 379-403). New York: The Guilford press; The uncertain mind: Individual differences in facing the unknown. London: Erlbaum (UK), Taylor & Francis). This formulation integrates affective and informational influences on thought and action. It is a model of self-regulation where the need to maintain clarity and the need to attain clarity are considered as two independent informational tendencies. These informational tendencies are influenced by the degree of self-relevance and the amount of perceived situational uncertainty. In addition, they are multiplicative with affective approach and avoidance tendencies, leading to accentuation or inhibition of behaviors expressed in action. It is further demonstrated that when the nondominant action tendency is instigated, secondary sources of motivation are expressed. This model not only accounts for past data, but also permits exploration using computer simulation. In addition, many new and testable hypotheses, including those related to affect and dynamical decision theory, are generated.


Ingroup identification as a moderator of positive–negative asymmetry in social discrimination

March 2003

·

129 Reads

·

60 Citations

European Journal of Social Psychology

Group members typically favour ingroups over outgroups, particularly when distributing positive rather than negative resources. The present investigation examined whether the positive–negative discrimination asymmetry in the minimal group paradigm varies as a function of ingroup identification. After being categorized into arbitrary groups, participants expressing low to high ingroup identification allocated positive, neutral, or negative outcomes on the basis of group membership (i.e. ingroup versus outgroup recipients). The interaction between ingroup identification and outcome valence revealed that identification influenced the magnitude of discrimination asymmetry. Specifically, increases in identification led to discrimination in favour of the ingroup for positive but not negative outcomes. The implications for intergroup behaviour more generally are considered. Copyright © 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.



Public Attitudes Toward Immigration in the United States and Canada in Response to the September 11, 2001 “Attack on America”

December 2002

·

1,128 Reads

·

124 Citations

Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy

In this paper, we describe recent trends in attitudes toward immigration in North America, and we suggest how these attitudes are likely to be affected by the September 11, 2001 “Attack on America.” We begin by explaining why public attitudes toward immigration are important, and describe recent trends in these attitudes in the United States and Canada. Then, we apply psychological perspectives to predicting how these attitudes are likely to change in response to the events of September 11th. In particular, we describe expected short-term changes in immigration attitudes and expected long-term trends. We conclude by suggesting that an understanding of the psychological processes underlying unfavorable attitudes may assist in counteracting these effects; we also suggest that policy makers will be faced with the difficult task of balancing the need for a sense of security for members of the national group, and maintenance of the positive features of current immigration policies.


Distancing oneself from negative attributes and the personal/group discrimination discrepancy

September 2002

·

184 Reads

·

51 Citations

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

The personal/group discrimination discrepancy (PGDD) refers to greater reporting of discrimination at the group than personal level. We examined whether the PGDD is one example of people's general tendency to distance themselves from negative attributes related to the social categories to which they belong, and whether this tendency varies as a function of ingroup identification. Female undergraduates (n=87) rated the extent to which positive and negative attributes, including being discriminated against, characterized themselves personally and women as a social category. As expected, negative attributes, including discrimination, were more strongly associated with the category than with the self, whereas positive attributes were more strongly associated with the self than with the category. Women who were lower in group identification were especially likely to show these discrepancy effects. The implications for personal and social identity are discussed.


Why Can't We Just Get Along? Interpersonal Biases and Interracial Distrust
  • Literature Review
  • Publisher preview available

May 2002

·

1,062 Reads

·

549 Citations

Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology

The authors review a series of studies that illustrate how one form of contemporary racial bias of Whites, aversive racism, can shape different perspectives of Blacks and Whites in ways that can undermine race relations. This research demonstrates that contemporary racism among Whites is subtle, often unintentional, and unconscious but that its effects are systematically damaging to race relations by fostering miscommunication and distrust. In particular, the authors examine the effects of aversive racism on outcomes for Blacks (e.g., in selection decisions), on the ways that Whites behave in interracial interactions, in the impressions that Whites and Blacks form of each other in these interactions, and on the task efficiency of interracial dyads.

View access options

Processes in Racial Discrimination: Differential Weighing of Conflicting Information

April 2002

·

556 Reads

·

262 Citations

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

The present research explored how White college students may exhibit response patterns associated with a subtle and rationalizable contemporary bias, aversive racism. In the study, higher and lower prejudice-scoring participants evaluated applicants for admission to their university, for whom information about high school achievement and college board scores (aptitude and achievement test scores) was independently varied as strong or weak. As predicted, discrimination against Black applicants relative to White applicants did not occur when the credentials were consistently strong or weak; however, discrimination by relatively high prejudice-scoring participants did emerge when the credentials were mixed and hence ambiguous. Moreover, relatively high prejudice-scoring participants weighed the different, conflicting criteria in ways that could justify or rationalize discrimination against Black applicants. The implications of these data for understanding contemporary racism and their relation to the shifting standards model of bias are considered.



A Theory of Uncertainty Orientation

January 2002

·

639 Reads

·

4 Citations

Based on the empirical evidence from our own past research, we have presented what we believe to be the first synergistic model of motivation and cognition that links individual differences in information processing to individual differences in motivation. This model takes into account not only the informational aspects of situations, such as attaining or maintaining clarity, but the affective components, such as feeling good or bad about the self. Although the empirical data set is primarily in the domain of achievement behavior, we believe it can be extended to most areas of behavior where the role of the self and self-regulation are important. This formulation translates the conceptual meaning of “flow” into specific informational and affective components, and introduces the term “anti-flow” to incorporate the negative state one may go through in matched situations or situations important to the individual. It also suggests when and where the full range of affective consequences of undertaking an activity should occur. Future research testing the model should lead to some exciting discoveries on how and when one thinks, acts, and feels.


Citations (82)


... Over the past decade, public awareness and visibility of sexual orientation and gender diverse (SOGD) persons in Australia have notably surged, with a significant rise in political discourse concerning SOGD issues (Casey et al., 2021;Ezzy et al., 2022). Despite a seemingly positive shift in social attitudes toward SOGD minorities, sexual orientation and gender-based discrimination remains and is often justified under the guise of conservative and/or moral religious expression (Hoffarth et al., 2018). Specifically, SOGD prejudice 1 (i.e., negative evaluations against individuals on the basis of their sexual orientation, gender identity or expression; Cramwinckel et al., 2018) endures. ...

Reference:

Promoting Positive Intergroup Relations Toward Sexual and Gender Minorities: The Role of Vicarious Contact and Religiosity
When and Why Is Religious Attendance Associated With Antigay Bias and Gay Rights Opposition? A Justification-Suppression Model Approach

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... In a large-scale North American study conducted between 1983 and 2005, Lewis (2011) found that regardless of people's demographics, belongings and beliefs, having more contacts with LGB+ people enabled heterosexual persons to support LGB+ rights. More frequent contact with gays and lesbians is associated with greater support of their rights, including same-sex marriage (Barth, Overby, & Huffmon, 2009;Hoffarth & Hodson, 2016). A more recent study carried out in Italy (Piumatti & Salvati, 2020) confirmed that contacts with LGB + community positively related to the endorsement of same-sex unions and families' rights, especially in participants with lower levels of religiosity. ...

Who Needs Imagined Contact?

Social Psychology

... While some effects have been confirmed in the lab (Sobol-Sarag et al., 2023;Vázquez et al., 2021) and field experiments (Dahl et al., 2021;Finseraas et al., 2016), or longitudinally (Borell-Porta et al., 2018;Halim et al., 2021;Perales et al., 2018;Shafer & Malhotra, 2011;Wiley et al., 2021), the results are sometimes inconclusive (Borell-Porta et al., 2018;Perales et al., 2018;Shafer & Malhotra, 2011) or show bidirectional links between contact and attitudes rather than clear unidirectional effects (Halim et al., 2021). Establishing causality is essential, as recent evidence from other intergroup contexts shows limited changes in outgroup attitudes over time due to contact (e.g., Friehs et al., 2024;Hodson & Meleady, 2024;Sengupta et al., 2023). This suggests that less prejudiced individuals may seek more and better intergroup contact. ...

Replicating and Extending : Contact Predicts No Within-Person Longitudinal Outgroup-Bias Change

American Psychologist

... Most relevant to the current research is work on the 'meat-paradox', which relies on a specific form of cognitive dissonance stressing the importance of the personal and behavioral commitment in dissonance arousal (Bastian & Loughnan, 2017; but see Rothgerber, 2020). Literature on the meat-paradox demonstrates that when people experience psychological con ict between their meat-eating practices and the harm experienced by animals in the meat production process, it increases their moral discomfort and guilt, and people try to reduce their responsibility by reducing their intentions to eat meat (e.g., Earle, Hodson, Dhont, & MacInnis, 201 ;Gunther, MacInnis, Hodson, & Dhont, 2023;Kunst & Hohle, 201 ), but people also engage in dementalization mechanisms to reduce the negative emotions raised by their consumptions (Bastian, Loughnan, Haslam, & Radke, 2012). When people face objectifiedanimals (i.e., meat-animals) and the moral implications of eating them, then denying the ualities that make those animals morally relevant, namely their capacity to think and feel, is an efficient way to minimize and rationalize the harm animals experience during the meatproduction process (Loughnan, Bastian, & Haslam, 2014). ...

Addressing Behavior and Policy Around Meat: Associating Factory Farming With Animal Cruelty “Works” Better Than Zoonotic Disease

... Gruplar arası insanlıktan çıkarma, insanların hayvanlardan üstün olduğu inancıyla bağlantılıdır. Bu inanca, diğer hayvanların insanlara kıyasla daha az korunmaya ve hakka sahip olduğu düşüncesi eşlik etmektedir (Hodson ve Dhont, 2023). Kimi insan gruplarının alt statüye yerleştirilen hayvan gruplarına benzetilmesi ile söz konusu gruplar insan kategorisinin dışına atılmaktadır. ...

An Integrated Psychology of (Animalistic) Dehumanization Requires a Focus on Human-Animal Relations
  • Citing Article
  • June 2023

Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology

... The addition of RWA in Model 3 rendered religious fundamentalism a marginally significant predictor of reduced prejudiced. This pattern has been observed in other research (e.g., Hall et al., 2010), but has been attributed to a suppression effect due to an overlap in the conceptualizations and measurements of RWA and religious fundamentalism (Hodson & Prusaczyk, 2024;Mavor et al., 2009). To address this issue, we followed the lead of Mavor et al. (2009) who reduced the overlap by using only an aggression subscale of RWA. ...

A cautionary note on interpreting research findings in the presence of statistical suppression
  • Citing Article
  • April 2023

... Unfortunately, in real life, affectively polarized partisans are not particularly motivated to build connections with each other. If anything, they are highly motivated to avoid interacting with those on the other side (6)(7)(8)(9)(10). With these social forces at work, how can researchers intervene? ...

Ideologically‐based contact avoidance during a pandemic: Blunt or selective distancing from ‘others’?
  • Citing Article
  • March 2023

European Journal of Social Psychology

... Theoretically, the present research provides one possible mechanism, dehumanization, that explains why satire can damage an individual's reputation. In addition, while focused on satire, the present research also contributes to our understanding of humor more generally and its unintended negative consequences (e.g., Hodson et al., 2010;Hodson & MacInnis, 2016;Hodson & Prusaczyk, 2021). For instance, prior research has shown humor can have negative consequences by disparaging others and keeping them in disadvantaged positions (for a review see Hodson & MacInnis, 2016). ...

Cavalier humor beliefs
  • Citing Chapter
  • March 2021

... We used themes and wording from other scholars (Cameron, 2004;Earle & Hodson, 2022;Sellers, 2013) to create this measure of support for the ideology of White identity pride. Using a 5-point scale ranging from "strongly disagree" to "strongly agree," participants responded to four statements, including "I am proud to be White." ...

Dealing with declining dominance: White identification and anti-immigrant hostility in the US
  • Citing Article
  • April 2022

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations

... We have demonstrated that prejudices can arise from both socialization processes (e.g., learned ideology), and reactions to ongoing societal changes (e.g., lost privileges). Future research on prejudice formation should explicitly consider both mechanisms together, as they have often been treated separately from each other (for socialization, see Levine et al., 1998; for societal changes, see Hodson et al., 2022). ...

Privilege lost: How dominant groups react to shifts in cultural primacy and power
  • Citing Article
  • April 2022

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations