March 2025
·
4 Reads
·
1 Citation
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
March 2025
·
4 Reads
·
1 Citation
February 2025
·
17 Reads
·
1 Citation
Social Science & Medicine
January 2025
·
17 Reads
Societal threats such as climate change, economic crises, and wars shape citizens’ political attitudes and behaviors. Yet, the structure of threat perceptions and their socio-demographic and ideological correlates remain underexplored. Using a six-wave Dutch survey (N = 685) and a network approach, we uncover the complexity of societal threat perceptions. First, we show that societal threat perceptions vary in their interconnectedness, with security threats as crime, the war in Ukraine, and asylum seekers emerging as central nodes. Second, ideology, age, and education are the most relevant variables linked to societal threat perceptions. Third, we replicate the structure of societal threat perceptions over time, highlighting the robustness of our conclusions. Our results reveal that societal threat perceptions form a stable and complex network, offering insights into how citizens process and respond to societal challenges. Future research should examine how contextual factors and interventions may reshape these networks.
December 2024
·
165 Reads
·
1 Citation
Journal of Experimental Political Science
Empathy has been proposed as a solution to alleviate interparty antipathy. Recent findings from the US suggest that one aspect of empathy – empathic concern – increases rather than decreases affective polarization. Perspective-taking, another aspect of empathy, has no effect on affective polarization. In this article, we describe a preregistered replication and extension of these findings in the contrasting political context of the Netherlands, to see whether this relationship generalizes beyond the US. First, using a cross-sectional nationally representative sample of 1,258 Dutch voters, we show that empathic concern indeed fuels affective polarization while at the same time we find that perspective-taking reduces it. Second, using a two-arm survey experiment (n = 438), we show that perspective-taking reduces ingroup bias, whereas empathic concern does not. Reflecting on the American and Dutch findings, we conclude that while empathic concern likely contributes to affective polarization, perspective-taking may reduce it.
November 2024
·
11 Reads
·
5 Citations
Political Psychology
A prominent theory in political psychology contends that individual differences in negativity bias explain political ideology: people who are more sensitive to negative stimuli find solace and comfort in conservative approaches to politics. Using self‐reported measures of negativity bias, the evidence is relatively consistent. Yet, using physiological indicators of the negativity bias, the results are, across the board, inconsistent with the negativity bias argument. Building on recent neuroscience work, we propose and test two innovations. First, unconscious and conscious processes are not necessarily aligned. Therefore, we propose that there are different – conscious and unconscious – paths that could link ideology with the negativity bias. Second, individuals vary in their ability and motivation to down‐regulate arousal and reflect on their emotions. As such, the ideology‐negativity bias may emerge in some individuals but not in others. In two preregistered laboratory studies (US and the Netherlands), we find limited evidence for these claims. In a subsequent registered report, we replicate some inconsistent patterns that emerged in study 1. We confirm that self‐reported social conservatism and implicit social conservatism (measured with an IAT) are positively correlated. We also establish that negativity bias is unrelated to social conservatism (both explicit and implicit). We conclude that negativity bias is most likely not the answer to why people on the left and right differ.
October 2024
·
8 Reads
·
1 Citation
Communication Methods and Measures
October 2024
·
31 Reads
Political interest is fundamental to democratic engagement. Yet, its conceptualization remains debated. In this study, we offer a new but important perspective by conceptualizing political (dis)interest as a social identity. We introduce two innovative measures: the Positive Political Interest Identity Scale (PPIS) and the Negative Political Interest Identity Scale (NPIS). Employing Item Response Theory, we validate the construct validity of both scales in a pre- registered study in the Netherlands (N = 2,060). Using the same dataset, we also demonstrate their predictive validity. Importantly, the PPIS and NPIS uncover substantial differences in political attitudes and behaviors both between and within individuals who identify as politically (dis)interested. Recognizing political (dis)interest as a social identity enriches our understand- ing of the concept and its implications for politically relevant attitudes and behaviors, while also informing interventions to foster political interest equally across all citizens.
September 2024
·
116 Reads
·
3 Citations
iScience
We revisit the hypotheses that conservatism positively correlates with amygdala and negatively with anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) gray matter volume.Using diverse measures of ideology and a large and representative sample (Amsterdam Open MRI Collection [n = 928]), we replicate a small positive relationship between amygdala volume and conservatism. However, we fail to find consistent evidence in support of the ideology-ACC volume link. Using a split-sample strategy,we conducted exploratory whole-brain analyses on half the data, preregistered the findings, and then conducted subsequent confirmatory tests that additionally highlight weak, positive associations between the right fusiform gyri and conservatism. This is the largest preregistered replication study in the context of political neuroscience. By using Dutch as opposed to British or American data, we also extend the amygdala-conservatism link to a multiparty, multidimensional political context. We discuss the implications for future investigations of the neural substrates of ideology.
August 2024
·
10 Reads
A prominent theory in political psychology contends that individual differences in negativity bias explain political ideology: people who are more sensitive to negative stimuli find solace and comfort in conservative approaches to politics. Using self-reported measures of negativity bias, the evidence is relatively consistent. Yet, using physiological indicators of the negativity bias, the results are, across the board, inconsistent with the negativity bias argument. Building on recent neuroscience work we propose and test two innovations. First, unconscious and conscious processes are not necessarily aligned. Therefore we propose that there are different - conscious and unconscious - paths that could link ideology with the negativity bias. Second, individuals vary in their ability and motivation to down-regulate arousal and reflect on their emotions. As such, the ideology-negativity bias may emerge in some individuals but not in others. In two preregistered laboratory studies (US and the Netherlands) we find limited evidence for these claims. In a subsequent registered report, we replicate some inconsistent patterns that emerged study 1. We confirm that self-reported social conservatism and implicit social conservatism (measured with an IAT) are positively correlated. We also establish that negativity bias is unrelated to social conservatism (both explicit and implicit). We conclude that negativity bias is most likely not the answer to why people on the left and right differ.
June 2024
·
53 Reads
Frontiers in Political Science
Who supports the populist radical right (PRR)? And under what circumstances? We theorize that social status-related envy (SSRE) is the construct that integrates personality- and grievance-based theories of PRR support. To assess our theory, we estimate psychological network models on German survey data to map the complex relationships between PRR support, Big Five personality traits, facets of narcissism, political attitudes, and multiple constructs measuring objective and subjective social status. Our findings confirm previous studies detecting two routes to PRR support: a disagreeable and an authoritarian one. The Bifurcated Model of Status-Deprived Narcissistic Right-Wing Populism claims that SSRE is the distant predictor of PRR support and the two constructs are connected by two pathways. The middle-class route is characterized by disagreeable narcissism (Rivalry) and nativism, while the lower-class route by Neuroticism (potentially Vulnerable Narcissism) and authoritarian right-wing populism. Moreover, we find preliminary support for our expectation that PRR voting is explained by the activation of narcissistic traits by SSRE.
... Consequently, loneliness as an alarm may motivate social (re)affiliation (Langenkamp, 2021). However, if mainstream social networks fail to provide a sense of belonging and reintegration into them is perceived as unattainable, lonely people may seek alternative sources of social inclusion, including radical organizations (Peterson et al., 2025;Pfundmair et al., 2024). In this situation, although the (re)affiliation process seems to be functional for the individual, potentially increasing self-esteem and reducing loneliness, it is nonfunctional socially because it increases the risk of radicalization (Opozda-Suder et al., 2024). ...
February 2025
Social Science & Medicine
... They did not find evidence for a presumably reducing effect of dispositional perspective-taking. Gillissen et al. (2023) replicated the effect of empathic concern on strengthening ingroup liking and relative ingroup liking. In addition, they reported a decrease in outgroup liking. ...
December 2024
Journal of Experimental Political Science
... The ideology and negative stereotypes suggested in [11] could be linked through a variety of conscious and unconscious pathways, which do not always work together and people differ in their drive and capacity to control their reactions and engage in emotional reflection. As a result, it verified the favorable correlation between latent social conservatism (as determined by an IAT) and self-expressed conservative social views. ...
November 2024
Political Psychology
... Natural language processing has seen a revolutionary development, primarily due to the advancements made possible by deep learning and advanced neural network architectures. Among these developments, the pre-trained transformer-based model BERT has been crucial in changing the field of NLP applications [18]. In contrast to conventional unidirectional models, BERT's bidirectional approach considers both words that come before and after to grasp the context of a sentence. ...
October 2024
Communication Methods and Measures
... The field of decision neuroscience has grown recently, with some common brain areas frequently identified to be fundamental for making choices about money, food, and political preferences (Figure 1). The brain regions commonly documented to be associated with political attitudes and behavior are emotional regions, including the amygdala (Gozzi et al., 2010;Kanai et al., 2011;Knutson et al., 2006;Petalas et al., 2024;Rule et al., 2010), insular cortex (Kaplan et al., 2007;Krosch et al., 2021;Schreiber et al., 2013;Westen et al., 2006), anterior cingulate cortex (Amodio et al., 2007;Kanai et al., 2011;Kaplan et al., 2007;Westen et al., 2006), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) (Knutson et al., 2006;Mitchell et al., 2006;Zamboni et al., 2009), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) (Mitchell et al., 2006;Zamboni et al., 2009), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) (Kaplan et al., 2007;Kato et al., 2009;Zamboni et al., 2009), ventral striatum (Gozzi et al., 2010;Tusche et al., 2013;Westen et al., 2006;Zamboni et al., 2009), and precuneus (Fowler & Schreiber, 2008;Gordon et al., 2019;Kaplan et al., 2016;Moore et al., 2021). Tusche et al. (2013) suggest that partisan bias may operate even in the absence of explicit attention to political content, yet few studies have examined the link between political ideology, brain activity, and non-political content in experiments. ...
September 2024
iScience
... Although the findings from the original study could largely be reproduced using the same data, in the replication attempt, only slightly more than half of the posts could be recollected, which led to substantial differences in the results. Dubèl et al. (2024) also replicate a previous study that found that viewers are more aroused by negative than positive news, by conducting a laboratory study that combined physiological measures with self-reports. They also extend the result of the previous study by repeating the study in another country and using additional measures. ...
April 2024
Media and Communication
... The findings of this study align with the broader literature on political communication and persuasion, which emphasizes the role of emotions and affect in shaping political speech. While existing research has strongly focused on emotions in offline contexts such as campaigns, speeches, and parliaments (Pipal et al., 2024;Valentim and Widmann, 2023), less attention has been given to emotional discourse on social media platforms (Widmann, 2022). Online, rhetorical strategies may diverge from traditional speeches and party manuals, using more informal speech, such as insults, capital letters or emojis. ...
February 2024
... The second approach is based on the analysis of various physiological processes (signals) synchrony, such as heart rate (Park et al., 2019;Coutinho et al., 2020), heart rate variability (Bizzego et al., 2019), electroencephalogram EEG (Long et al., 2021;Zhou et al., 2024), electromyography (Schumacher et al., 2024), galvanic skin response (Prochazkova et al., 2021) and micromovements (Minkin, 2012). The synchrony of a couple's physiological processes depends not only on personal relationships, but also on external factors, the environment, and the stimuli presented (Danyluck, Page-Gould, 2019; Bizzego et al., 2019; Long et al., 2021). ...
January 2024
Politics and the Life Sciences
... opposite emotional reactions 18 . Since this process happens partly unconscious, we extend this line of research by examining whether the emotional displays of in-party politicians are already differently processed at the neural level, compared to the emotions displayed by out-party politicians. ...
November 2022
... Political figures, leaders, and social movement organizers utilize emotions in their online communication to amplify message reach and reinforce perceived social norms (Brady et al., 2017). Populism is intertwined with emotions, given populism's inherent emotional intensity aimed at evoking affective responses by appealing to "the people" or blaming "others" (Martella and Bracciale, 2022;Schumacher et al., 2022). Populists employ, for instance, an ordinariness strategy to elicit positive emotions like pride, enthusiasm, and hope (Albertazzi and Bonansinga, 2023) when connecting with the public. ...
May 2022
Political Psychology