Francis Yuen’s research while affiliated with University of British Columbia and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (16)


Moral Babies? Evidence for Core Moral Responses in Infants and Toddlers
  • Chapter

February 2025

·

28 Reads

·

Francis Yuen

The Cambridge Handbook of Moral Psychology is an essential guide to the study of moral cognition and behavior. Originating as a philosophical exploration of values and virtues, moral psychology has evolved into a robust empirical science intersecting psychology, philosophy, anthropology, sociology, and neuroscience. Contributors to this interdisciplinary handbook explore a diverse set of topics, including moral judgment and decision making, altruism and empathy, and blame and punishment. Tailored for graduate students and researchers across psychology, philosophy, anthropology, neuroscience, political science, and economics, it offers a comprehensive survey of the latest research in moral psychology, illuminating both foundational concepts and cutting-edge developments.


FIGURE 2 Sample image of choice phase. Note: Choice phase foam 3-D stimuli presented on a white board. Board dimensions: 45 cm × 60 cm; Shape dimensions: 9.9 cm × 9.9 cm (blue square), 14.8 cm (base) × 13 cm (height; yellow triangle).
FIGURE 3 Age × Choice plot. Note: Probability of choosing the Helper/Push-up character (over Hinderer/Push-down character) across ages. The smoothing line shows the predicted marginal effects from our noninformative Bayesian regression model along with their credible interval. The dashed line on the y-axis represents chance performance. Data are jittered slightly on the vertical axis to avoid overplotting.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 6 Plot of median testing date × proportion of infants choosing Helper/Push-up character in each laboratory. Note: Proportion of infants choosing the Helper/Push-up character (over Hinderer/Push-down character) in each laboratory plotted by the laboratory's median testing date. The dashed line on the y-axis represents chance performance. The smoothing line shows the estimated effects with credible intervals. Data are jittered slightly on the vertical axis to avoid overplotting. Each point represents data from one lab.

+3

Infants’ Social Evaluation of Helpers and Hinderers: A Large‐Scale, Multi‐Lab, Coordinated Replication Study
  • Article
  • Full-text available

December 2024

·

278 Reads

·

2 Citations

Developmental Science

·

Francis Yuen

·

·

[...]

·

J Kiley Hamlin

Evaluating whether someone's behavior is praiseworthy or blameworthy is a fundamental human trait. A seminal study by Hamlin and colleagues in 2007 suggested that the ability to form social evaluations based on third-party interactions emerges within the first year of life: infants preferred a character who helped, over hindered, another who tried but failed to climb a hill. This sparked a new line of inquiry into the origins of social evaluations; however, replication attempts have yielded mixed results. We present a preregistered, multi-laboratory, standardized study aimed at replicating infants' preference for Helpers over Hinderers. We intended to (1) provide a precise estimate of the effect size of infants' preference for Helpers over Hinderers, and (2) determine the degree to which preferences are based on social information. Using the ManyBabies framework for big team-based science, we tested 1018 infants (567 included, 5.5-10.5 months) from 37 labs across five continents. Overall, 49.34% of infants preferred Helpers over Hinderers in the social condition, and 55.85% preferred characters who pushed up, versus down, an inanimate object in the nonsocial condition; neither proportion differed from chance or from each other. This study provides evidence against infants' prosocial preferences in the hill paradigm, suggesting the effect size is weaker, absent, and/or develops later than previously estimated. As the first of its kind, this study serves as a proof-of-concept for using active behavioral measures (e.g., manual choice) in large-scale, multi-lab projects studying infants.

Download

Infants’ Social Evaluation of Helpers and Hinderers: A Large-Scale, Multi-Lab, Coordinated Replication Study

November 2024

·

237 Reads

Developmental Science

Evaluating whether someone's behavior is praiseworthy or blameworthy is a fundamental human trait. A seminal study by Hamlin and colleagues in 2007 suggested that the ability to form social evaluations based on third-party interactions emerges within the first year of life: infants preferred a character who helped, over hindered, another who tried but failed to climb a hill. This sparked a new line of inquiry into the origins of social evaluations; however, replication attempts have yielded mixed results. We present a preregistered, multi-laboratory, standardized study aimed at replicating infants’ preference for Helpers over Hinderers. We intended to (1) provide a precise estimate of the effect size of infants’ preference for Helpers over Hinderers, and (2) determine the degree to which preferences are based on social information. Using the ManyBabies framework for big team-based science, we tested 1018 infants (567 included, 5.5–10.5 months) from 37 labs across five continents. Overall, 49.34% of infants preferred Helpers over Hinderers in the social condition, and 55.85% preferred characters who pushed up, versus down, an inanimate object in the nonsocial condition; neither proportion differed from chance or from each other. This study provides evidence against infants’ prosocial preferences in the hill paradigm, suggesting the effect size is weaker, absent, and/or develops later than previously estimated. As the first of its kind, this study serves as a proof-of-concept for using active behavioral measures (e.g., manual choice) in large-scale, multi-lab projects studying infants.


EPISTEMIC STATE-BASED ACTION ANTICIPATION Registered Report Action Anticipation Based on an Agent's Epistemic State in Toddlers and Adults

September 2024

·

102 Reads

·

2 Citations

Do toddlers and adults engage in spontaneous Theory of Mind (ToM)? Evidence from anticipatory looking (AL) studies suggests they do. But a growing body of failed replication studies raised questions about the paradigm’s suitability, urging the need to test the robustness of AL as a spontaneous measure of ToM. In a multi-lab collaboration we examine whether 18- to 27-month- olds’ and adults’ anticipatory looks distinguish between two basic forms of epistemic states: knowledge and ignorance. In toddlers [ANTICIPATED n = 520 50% FEMALE] and adults [ANTICIPATED n = 408, 50% FEMALE], we found [SUPPORT/NO SUPPORT] for epistemic state-based action anticipation. Future research can probe whether this conclusion extends to more complex kinds of epistemic states, such as true and false beliefs.


Able and Willing: Infants Selectively Seek Help From Competent and Benevolent Others

Developmental Psychology

Young children often encounter unsolvable problems with which they require others’ help. To receive adequate assistance, children must be savvy about whom they seek help from: Effective helpers must possess both the ability to help (e.g., competence) and a willingness to do so (e.g., benevolence). Although past work suggests that information about competence and benevolence can inform young children’s help-seeking behavior, it remains unclear how and whether children utilize said factors independently of each other. Furthermore, it is unclear whether they can generalize potential helpers’ competence from one task to another. The current experiments examined whether 22- to 23-month-olds confronted with a broken toy selectively sought help from agents who had previously demonstrated either competence (Experiment 1) or benevolence (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, infants preferred to seek help from a competent agent who successfully opened a closed box over one who failed to do so. In Experiment 2, infants selectively sought help from a benevolent agent who helped a third party by returning a lost ball, over an agent who stole the ball instead. These patterns of selectivity were not driven by associative valence matching; in Experiment 3, infants showed no preference for an agent who was itself helped versus an agent who was hindered. These results suggest that before their second birthday, infants independently utilize cues to both competence and benevolence to inform their help seeking, using information generalized from novel contexts. We discuss the potential nature of this generalization as well as directions for future work.



Validation of an open source, remote web‐based eye‐tracking method (WebGazer) for research in early childhood

October 2023

·

307 Reads

·

17 Citations

Infancy

Measuring eye movements remotely via the participant's webcam promises to be an attractive methodological addition to in‐person eye‐tracking in the lab. However, there is a lack of systematic research comparing remote web‐based eye‐tracking with in‐lab eye‐tracking in young children. We report a multi‐lab study that compared these two measures in an anticipatory looking task with toddlers using WebGazer.js and jsPsych. Results of our remotely tested sample of 18‐27‐month‐old toddlers ( N = 125) revealed that web‐based eye‐tracking successfully captured goal‐based action predictions, although the proportion of the goal‐directed anticipatory looking was lower compared to the in‐lab sample ( N = 70). As expected, attrition rate was substantially higher in the web‐based (42%) than the in‐lab sample (10%). Excluding trials based on visual inspection of the match of time‐locked gaze coordinates and the participant's webcam video overlayed on the stimuli was an important preprocessing step to reduce noise in the data. We discuss the use of this remote web‐based method in comparison with other current methodological innovations. Our study demonstrates that remote web‐based eye‐tracking can be a useful tool for testing toddlers, facilitating recruitment of larger and more diverse samples; a caveat to consider is the larger drop‐out rate.


A Unified Approach to Demographic Data Collection for Research With Young Children Across Diverse Cultures

October 2023

·

200 Reads

·

21 Citations

Developmental Psychology

Culture is a key determinant of children’s development both in its own right and as a measure of generalizability of developmental phenomena. Studying the role of culture in development requires information about participants’ demographic backgrounds. However, both reporting and treatment of demographic data are limited and inconsistent in child development research. A barrier to reporting demographic data in a consistent fashion is that no standardized tool currently exists to collect these data. Variation in cultural expectations, family structures, and life circumstances across communities make the creation of a unifying instrument challenging. Here, we present a framework to standardize demographic reporting for early child development (birth to 3 years of age), focusing on six core sociodemographic construct categories: biological information, gestational status, health status, community of descent, caregiving environment, and socioeconomic status. For each category, we discuss potential constructs and measurement items and provide guidance for their use and adaptation to diverse contexts. These items are stored in an open repository of context-adapted questionnaires that provide a consistent approach to obtaining and reporting demographic information so that these data can be archived and shared in a more standardized format.


Selective BTS initiatives (arranged alphabetically). For each initiative, we approximated the percentage of authors who contributed to each aspect of the project by having two researchers (H.A.B. and N.A.C.) code the author contributions for each paper. For each project, darker shades indicate that a relatively high percentage of authors reported contributing to each aspect of the project. Citations in figure refer to [9,12–14,22]. Coding information used to generate this figure is available at https://osf.io/2p4ct/.
How to build up big team science: a practical guide for large-scale collaborations

June 2023

·

168 Reads

·

18 Citations

The past decade has witnessed a proliferation of big team science (BTS), endeavours where a comparatively large number of researchers pool their intellectual and/or material resources in pursuit of a common goal. Despite this burgeoning interest, there exists little guidance on how to create, manage and participate in these collaborations. In this paper, we integrate insights from a multi-disciplinary set of BTS initiatives to provide a how-to guide for BTS. We first discuss initial considerations for launching a BTS project, such as building the team, identifying leadership, governance, tools and open science approaches. We then turn to issues related to running and completing a BTS project, such as study design, ethical approvals and issues related to data collection, management and analysis. Finally, we address topics that present special challenges for BTS, including authorship decisions, collaborative writing and team decision-making.


We aren't especially fearful apes, and fearful apes aren't especially prosocial

May 2023

·

39 Reads

Behavioral and Brain Sciences

Grossmann posits that heightened fearfulness in humans evolved to facilitate cooperative caregiving. We argue that three of his claims - that children express more fear than other apes, that they are uniquely responsive to fearful expressions, and that expression and perception of fear are linked with prosocial behaviors - are inconsistent with existing literature or require additional supporting evidence.


Citations (10)


... It is The copyright holder for this preprint this version posted https://doi.org/10.1101https://doi.org/10. /2025 3 2007, 2010; Holvoet et al., 2016;Margoni & Surian, 2018;Scarf et al., 2012), a recent large-scale and multi-lab replication study showed no preference for the helper over the hinderer social agents in children under 10 months (Lucca et al., 2025). ...

Reference:

Tonkean macaques do not prefer the helper or the hinderer in the hill paradigm
Infants’ Social Evaluation of Helpers and Hinderers: A Large‐Scale, Multi‐Lab, Coordinated Replication Study

Developmental Science

... 12 In the light of my arguments pressing for more observational work in developmental psychology, practitioners in that field may take umbrage with the implication that their research lacks reliable conceptual frameworks. Yet in the wake of the replication 'crisis' that has swept through that field and many others in the psychological sciences, where findings that have grounded previously well-established theories are proving hard to replicate (e.g., Schuwerk et al., 2022), such a conclusion may not be as far-fetched as it originally sounds. The problem, I believe, is one of presentation. ...

EPISTEMIC STATE-BASED ACTION ANTICIPATION Registered Report Action Anticipation Based on an Agent's Epistemic State in Toddlers and Adults
  • Citing Article
  • September 2024

... Although our work highlights challenges with commensurability and multiplicative constraints on generalizability, it also provides proof-of-concept for a potential methodological response: big team science [53][54][55][56] . Big team science effectively allowed us to leverage the wisdom-of-crowds to evaluate a challenging theoretical question in emotion research. ...

How to build up big team science: A practical guide for large-scale collaborations.
  • Citing Chapter
  • January 2024

... Empirical research has shown that a mean sampling rate of 19 Hz (range 5-30 Hz) is sufficient for capturing a prediction effect in a webcam-based VWP study [55]. Notably, a growing number of studies have shown that webcam-based eye-trackers can replicate in-lab findings [58][59][60][61][62], suggesting the efficacy of webcam-based eye-trackers. ...

Validation of an open source, remote web‐based eye‐tracking method (WebGazer) for research in early childhood

Infancy

... The participating children lived in German university cities and their surroundings. Further demographic data were not collected due to the local data protection rules (see Singh et al. 2024). ...

A Unified Approach to Demographic Data Collection for Research With Young Children Across Diverse Cultures

Developmental Psychology

... extension (Papoutsaki et al., 2016). This extension was provided and tested by Steffan et al. (2023) in a project called the 'ManyBabies Project' (Byers-Heinlein et al., 2020), comparing a sample of 125 online participants to 65 lab-based participants, providing validation for this web-based method of eye-tracking. Despite potential challenges like lower sampling rates from personal computers, we took proactive measures to ensure data quality, such as backup protocols and participant instructions to minimise data loss and a rigorous calibration process involving initial and post-stimulus quality assessments to maintain data integrity, as recommended by Steffan et al. (2023). ...

Validation of an Open Source, Remote Web-based Eye-tracking Method (WebGazer) for Research in Early Childhood
  • Citing Preprint
  • January 2023

... There are efforts underway to establish norms for demographic reporting for infant research that tackle the question of what to report. In particular, ManyBabies Demographics aims to provide a generalizable and adaptable framework for demographic reporting in infant research (Singh, Barakova et al., 2022). This effort aims to provide the "lowest common denominator" of demographic information that merits inclusion when collecting infant data. ...

A Unified Approach to Demographic Data Collection for Research with Young Children Across Diverse Cultures

... Prior work has revealed how people use momentary cues such as facial expressions 53,54 , behaviour [55][56][57] and contexts [58][59][60] to infer each other's mental states. Some recent studies have looked beyond momentary cues and investigated how knowledge or inferences of others' more stable characteristics (for example, whether someone is a good or bad social partner in general) shape mental state inferences, and vice versa 38,39,61 . However, the trait knowledge and inferences used in prior work were predominantly based on more comprehensive information (for example, participants' knowledge of their families and friends' traits or The y axis indicates different dependent variables, one for each mental state. ...

Socially evaluative contexts facilitate mentalizing
  • Citing Article
  • November 2022

Trends in Cognitive Sciences

... Large-scale multi-lab projects are beginning to quantify such sources of variance by running the same single experimental paradigm in many labs around the world, which enables the modeling of the effects of sociodemographic variability (e.g., Frank et al., 2020;Kosie et al., 2024;Lucca et al., 2022;Schuwerk et al., 2021;Visser et al., 2022). The Developing Belief Network, led by Rebekah Richert and Kathleen Corriveau (Richert et al., 2022), provides a similar structure. ...

Action anticipation based on an agent's epistemic state in toddlers and adults
  • Citing Preprint
  • February 2021