Frances J. White’s research while affiliated with University of Oregon and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (85)


Figure 1: Percentage of self-directed grooming bouts that involved self-directed hair plucking versus social grooming bouts that involved social hair plucking per individual.
Figure 2: Mean urinary cortisol levels per individual. Error bars represent standard error across all samples per individual.
TABLE 2 . Correlation analysis 
Hair plucking, stress, and urinary cortisol among captive bonobos (Pan paniscus)
  • Article
  • Full-text available

September 2016

·

622 Reads

·

18 Citations

Zoo Biology

·

·

·

[...]

·

J. Josh Snodgrass

Hair plucking has been observed in many captive primate species, including the great apes, however, the etiology of this behavioral pattern is poorly understood. While this behavior has not been reported in wild apes, an ethologically identical behavior in humans, known as trichotillomania, is linked to chronic psychosocial stress and is a predominantly female disorder. This study examines hair plucking (defined here as a rapid jerking away of the hair shaft and follicle by the hand or mouth, often accompanied by inspection and consumption of the hair shaft and follicle) in a captive group of bonobos (N=13) at the Columbus Zoo and Aquarium in Columbus, Ohio. Plucking data were collected using behavior and all-occurrence sampling; 1450 social and self-directed grooming bouts were recorded during 128 hours of observation. Twenty-one percent of all grooming bouts involved at least one instance of plucking. Urine samples (N=55) were collected and analyzed for the stress hormone cortisol. Analyses of urinary cortisol levels showed a significant positive correlation between mean cortisol and self-directed plucking for females (r=0.88, p<0.05) but not for males (r=-0.73, p=0.09). These results demonstrate an association between relative self-directed hair plucking and cortisol among female bonobos. This is the first study to investigate the relationship between hair plucking and cortisol among apes. Overall, these data add to our knowledge of a contemporary issue in captive ape management.

Download

Figure 1. Distribution of Pan paniscus (green) and the locations of the long-term bonobo research areas: 1-Lomako, 2-Wamba, 3-LuiKotale.
Table 1 . Genetic diversity measures per locus.
Figure 2. Map of fecal sample collection locations. Each circle represents a successfully genotyped fecal sample. Colors indicate the association network for each sample and correspond to Figure 3. The base camp is represented by a star.  
Initiation of genetic demographic monitoring of bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Iyema, Lomako Forest, DRC

September 2016

·

274 Reads

·

10 Citations

Primate Conservation

Research on wild apes is not only fundamental for elucidating human origins but for their conservation as well. Despite their relative size, apes are difficult to observe in the wild prior to habituation, limiting our ability to accurately assess demography and kin relations. Non-invasive genetic sampling provides an indirect source of this information. Here, we present findings of a pilot genetic survey of a wild community of bonobos that are in the initial stages of being studied. Fifty-three fecal samples were collected over eight days near the Iyema field site in the Lomako Forest, DRC. DNA was first extracted and quantified using a qPCR assay. Samples with a sufficient amount of DNA were genotyped at 11 microsatellite loci and sexed using an amelogenin assay. Thirty-three of 53 samples yielded a sufficient amount of DNA for complete genotyping. We identified 19 individuals, including six males and 13 females. Mean allelic richness across all loci was 5.7 and expected heterozygosity was 0.69. Estimates of population size indicate between 26 and 66 individuals are present in the study area, but more than one community may be present. These results contribute to our ongoing efforts to study and monitor the bonobos at Iyema to better understand their demography, behavior, and conservation. Our study also highlights the utility of genetic analyses in pilot and survey research.


Genetic estimation of bonobo (Pan paniscus) community size at Iyema, DRC

Accurate estimates of population sizes are essential to effective biomonitoring and conservation strategies. Genetic capture-recapture methods have been used to confidently assess abundance in African apes without having to completely habituate communities. The objective of this study was to use this method on a semi-habituated bonobo community at the Iyema field site, located in the Lomako Forest Reserve, Democratic Republic of Congo. Previous behavioral research has resulted in an estimated community size of 20 - 50 individuals. We collected 53 fecal samples for DNA analysis. DNA was extracted and quantified from each sample and then genotyped at 11 polymorphic microsatellite loci. Unique individuals were sexed using an amelogenin assay. We estimated community size using the software program Capture and the R Package Capwire. Capture estimated 23 - 34 individuals are present in this community. Using Capwire, the equal capture model (ECM) indicated approximately 26 community members whereas the two innate rates model (TIRM) indicated approximately 38 individuals are present (95% Confidence Interval: 26-65). Finally, we tested both Capwire models and found TIRM to better fit our data (LR=8.72, bootstraps=500, p<0.05). All three estimates fall within the previous estimates for the community although more intensive sampling is required to obtain a more accurate community size estimation.


This table shows the differences in community size, average party size, and ranging area of the communities at Lomako.
Average group size of the four species of monkeys found during transects conducted at Lomako.
The Effects of War on Bonobos and Other Nonhuman Primates in the Democratic Republic of the Congo

July 2016

·

347 Reads

·

10 Citations

War is awful for people and for wildlife. The “Great African War” of the 1990s and 2000s was the worst in modern African history. An estimated 3.9 million people died while wildlife clearly suffered. The purpose of this chapter is to look at the effects of the war and subsequent political instability on the people and nonhuman primates living in the Lomako Forest. To do this, we compared the prewar and postwar ranging and community demographics data of two bonobo communities living near the N’deli site in the Lomako Forest. One community was not directly observed in the study area after the war while the second moved outside of their former range and saw a decrease in community number and average group size. We also compared the prewar and postwar demographics of four monkey species living in the forest. For all of the species, average group size was lower while for three out of the four species the population density decreased as well. Overall, the population density of monkeys living in Lomako went from 165 to 83.6 individuals/km2. These results suggest an increase in hunting pressures faced by nonhuman primates because of an increasingly hungry human population with little income. Finally, we comment on the ethics of habituating primates in politically unstable regions.


Intersexual affiliation in a troop of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta)

The occurrence of female dominance among the Lemuriformes has prompted extensive research into the mating strategies and mate choice of several species, particularly ring-tailed lemurs. Female mate choice has been suggested to be particularly significant when understanding these complex mating strategies. The focus on female dominance, however, may obscure the complexities of intersexual affiliations that can have consequences for later mating opportunities. While males may use short-term strategies to increase their chances of breeding with females, continuous residence with females provides the opportunity for the use of long-term reproductive strategies. We studied intersexual relationships among adult individuals using grooming and proximity as measures of affiliation. Subjects were a group of semi-free-ranging ring-tailed lemurs (N=17) on St. Catherines Island, Georgia. Data were collected on social behavior using all occurrence sampling. Focal animal sampling was also used to collect behavioral and proximity data. When excluding related individuals, male-female grooming occurred less frequently than male-male and female-female dyads. However, social grooming was not equally distributed across all male-female dyads. The lowest ranking females were groomed by every adult male whereas the high ranking females were only groomed by certain males. Low ranking females were also observed to be in closer proximity to males more often than high ranking females; 20% and 6% of proximity scans, respectively. These data highlight asymmetries in intersexual relationships among these lemurs. These relationships should be considered when studying reproductive strategies and mate choice.


A genetic survey of the bonobos (Pan paniscus) at the Iyema field site, DRC

Research on wild apes is not only fundamental to elucidating human origins but to their conservation as well. Despite their relative size, apes are difficult to observe in the wild prior to habituation, limiting our ability to infer demography, relatedness, and kinship. Non-invasive genetic sampling provides an indirect source of this information. Here, we present preliminary findings of a genetic “survey” of a wild semi-habituated community of bonobos. 58 fecal samples were collected from nesting parties at the Iyema field site in the Lomako Forest, DRC. DNA was first extracted and quantified using a qPCR assay and samples that yielded >0.01 ng of DNA were further genotyped at 11 microsatellite loci and sexed using an amelogenin assay. 34/58 samples yielded a sufficient amount of DNA for complete genotyping. We identified 20 individuals, including 7 males and 13 females. Genetic diversity was high; mean allelic richness across all loci was 5.2 and expected heterozygosity was 0.70. We detected several kin relationships between individuals, including 7 parent-offspring dyads. Mean relatedness among males was only slightly higher when compared to the relatedness among females, which is consistent with results reported from wild chimpanzee communities. Our genetic survey indicates that this community has retained high genetic diversity. These results also validate our methods for the use of non-invasive genetic sampling of the bonobos at Iyema and are essential to our long-term research and conservation goals at the Iyema field site.


Table 1 .
Figure 3. Variation in human marriage, mating and parenting as arising from variation in male bargaining power. As men gain bargaining power (through increased control over the means of production, greater mate quality, a female-biased sex ratio, or more social support), their contribution to existing partnerships decreases (blue line). With lower male parenting effort, monogamous marriage becomes less stable, and male pursuit of additional matings increase, resulting in higher levels of polygyny at the individual and population level (red line). See Section 2.3 for examples and details. This figure is published in colour in the online edition of this journal, which can be accessed via http://booksandjournals.brillonline.com/content/ journals/1568539x.  
Figure 5. Intergroup correlation in production (resource abundance) changes the gains to raiding or trading. If the correlation is low, one group will have plenty when others have little and vice versa, creating the opportunity to benefit from reciprocal land use and trade, e.g., among most foragers and perhaps bonobos and Taï chimpanzees where females may visit neighbouring groups for extended periods of time. When neighbours experience good and bad times simultaneously, this increases the gains to territoriality and warfare, e.g., among complex foragers and agriculturalists. The benefits from both kinds of intergroup interactions are reduced when the risk of shortfalls is lower (dashed vs solid lines). After Kelly (2013), his Figures 6-8. See Section 4 for details and examples. This figure is published in colour in the online edition of this journal, which can be accessed via http://booksandjournals.brillonline. com/content/journals/1568539x.
Obstacles and catalysts of cooperation in humans, bonobos, and chimpanzees: behavioural reaction norms can help explain variation in sex roles, inequality, war and peace

February 2016

·

868 Reads

·

42 Citations

Behaviour

Our closest living relatives, bonobos and chimpanzees, along with small-scale human societies figure prominently in debates about human nature. Here we emphasize and explain behavioural variation within and among these three species. In the logic of behavioural ecology, individuals have been selected to adjust their behaviour along evolved reaction norms that maximize fitness given current socio-ecological conditions. We discuss variation in three behavioural contexts: relationships between the sexes, hierarchy and inequality, and intergroup interactions. In each context, behavioural variation can be related to two broad socio-ecological conditions: (i) the defensibility of contested resources, and (ii) differences in bargaining power. When defensibility of resources and differences in bargaining power are great, interactions are rife with conflict; when they are minimal, interactions are more harmonious. These socio-ecological conditions therefore constitute key catalysts and obstacles of cooperation. We conclude that human nature should be seen as consisting of evolved reaction norms.


A 3D geometric morphometric analysis of cranial morphology of a putative hybrid species, Trachypithecus pileatus (Primates, Colobinae)

The langurs (subfamily: Colobinae) of Southeast Asia have undergone many phylogenetic revisions. Incongruent results from recent genetic work suggest Trachypithecus pileatus and Trachypithecus geei are the result of ancient hybridization between Semnopithecus and Trachypithecus species. Geographically, these species are located between the distributions of Semnopithecus to the west and Trachypithecus to the east. The phylogenetic positions of T. pileatus and T. geei are thus uncertain, and they may represent a distinct genus from both Semnopithecus and Trachypithecus. In order to resolve their phylogeny, more data is needed on these poorly-known species. Forty-five 3D cranial landmarks were collected using a Microscribe-3DX on a sample of 172 adult crania from wild populations of Semnopithecus and Trachypithecus species, as well as 24 adult crania of Trachypithecus pileatus. Pairwise permutation tests with 1000 replications were run between subgroups of shape data to quantify and assign statistical significance to the differences between the subgroups. All subgroups were significantly different from each other in shape (Semnopithecus and Trachypithecus p=0.00, Procrustes distance=0.062277; Semnopithecus and T. pileatus p=0.00, Procrustes distance=0.08815; Trachypithecus and T. pileatus p=0.00, Procrustes distance=0.04415). Species within a genus were also signficantly different from each other, but their Procrustes distances fell within the range of permuted Procrustes distances. The Procrustes distances between T. pileatus and both Semnopithecus and Trachypithecus were not in the ranges of permuted Procrustes distances. This data suggests Trachypithecus pileatus and probably T. geei represent a distinct lineage and in conjunction with recent genetic work, may warrant a new genus designation.




Citations (39)


... Recent research has suggested that the Member 2, Member 4 and Jacovec Cavern breccias at Sterkfontein may be as old as 4-3.5 Ma [34,35], but this older age is inconsistent with biochronological evidence (e.g. [36,37]). ...

Reference:

Dart and the Taung juvenile: making sense of a century-old record of hominin evolution in Africa
Reply to Granger et al.: Multiple, independent lines of evidence suggest Sterkfontein is less than 2.8 My old
  • Citing Article
  • March 2023

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

... 6,7 The method is analogous to that which has been applied to nonhominin fauna from East and South Africa. Notably, Frost et al. 3 Irish et al. 35 These results align remarkably well with independent uranium-lead and palaeomagnetic dates pointing to an age of 1.98 Ma. 36 The underlying message is that the hominin biochronological method developed by Thackeray and Dykes 6 is reliable in this particular instance, with the expectation that it can be applied to other Plio-Pleistocene specimens of Australopithecus in South Africa. In the case of StW 573, it is recognized that there is extreme occlusal wear affecting enamel of the first mandibular molars, hence the need to use MD and BL measurements from the enamel-dentine junction ( Figure 2). ...

Biochronology of South African hominin-bearing sites: A reassessment using cercopithecid primates
  • Citing Article
  • October 2022

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

... Immigration events, elevated infanticide risk, and associated social instability are stressful situations and are associated with elevated glucocorticoid levels in chacma baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) (Engh et al. 2006a(Engh et al. , 2006bCapitanio and Cole, 2015). Lactating and pregnant females experience more increased glucocorticoid levels than cycling females, suggesting that male immigration or alpha male takeover impacts females differently depending on their reproductive status (Beehner et al. 2005), and some studies have found an association between glucocorticoid levels and gut microbiota composition (Allen-Blevins et al. 2017;Hickmott et al. 2022;Marin et al. 2017;Petrullo et al. 2022;Stothart et al. 2016; but see Rudolph et al. 2022). ...

A comparison of faecal glucocorticoid metabolite concentration and gut microbiota diversity in bonobos (Pan paniscus)
  • Citing Article
  • August 2022

Microbiology

... The next event in the two ape lineages is the divergence of the ancestral populations of the living species from now extinct ghost lineages, ∼3.0 Ma in the case of Pan [30], and 3.4 Ma in the case of Gorilla [31]. The two species of Pan (Pan paniscus and Pan troglodytes) diverged between 1.5 and 2.0 Ma [16,30,[32][33][34][35], and the two Gorilla species (Gorilla beringei and Gorilla gorilla) ∼1.0 Ma [16,33,35]. Eastern (P.t. ...

Estimating bonobo ( Pan paniscus ) and chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ) evolutionary history from nucleotide site patterns
  • Citing Article
  • April 2022

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

... Morphological variation driven by environmental factors may require revisions to this taxonomy. Recent studies have highlighted the utility of captive/wild comparison studies for understanding phenotypically plastic morphological differences between populations of wild animals, and thus the criteria for subspecific taxonomy [15,16]. Boundaries of subspecies, recognised and defined by morphological differences, influence conservation management and so have real, practical importance for the future of these species. ...

Comparison of captive and wild fascicularis ‐group macaques (Primates, Cercopithecidae) provides insight into cranial form changes in response to rapid environmental changes

American Journal of Biological Anthropology

... There are wellbeing implications for transferring animals to new social groups and unfamiliar environments. The social life of different species of animal varies from predominantly solitary animals which raise their own young (tigers Panthera tigris [35]), small family groups (gorillas Gorilla gorilla [36][37][38]), fission-fusion (spider monkeys Ateles spp. [39], dolphins [40]), stable long-term social groups (elephants [41]), and all manner of other social styles. ...

Survey of current group demographics and management practices of bachelor groups of western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) across North America
  • Citing Article
  • January 2022

Zoo Biology

... Although not an exact proxy, the fecal microbiome reflects the trillions of microbes that reside in the gastrointestinal tract; microbes that collectively contribute to host digestion, immunity and pathogen defense, and intestinal barrier homeostasis [1]. While the composition of the fecal microbiome is highly dynamic and can correlate with a range of host factors including age [2,3], sex [4], body condition [5], diet [6,7], habitat [8,9], and antibiotic use [10], more persistent changes in composition have also been associated with disease and infections [11,12]. Compared to their healthy counterparts, the fecal microbiome of animals with the disease or infection may have reduced microbial diversity [13], decreased abundances of functionally important microbes like fermentative or short-chain fatty acid (SCFA)-producing bacteria [14], or elevated abundances of pathogenic taxa [15,16]. ...

A Test of Foraging Models Using Dietary Diversity Indices for the Lomako Forest Bonobos
  • Citing Article
  • November 2021

Folia Primatologica

... Subsequently, understanding the behavior of male WLG in bachelor groups and ensuring these groupings are managed optimally has been a major focus of study for several decades (Coe et al. 2009;Gartland et al. 2018Gartland et al. , 2021Gartland et al. , 2022Leeds et al. 2015Leeds et al. , 2018Schaefer and Steklis 2014;Stoinski et al. 2001Stoinski et al. , 2002Stoinski et al. , 2013Stoinski et al. 2004a, b). Concerns regarding the long-term practicalities of managing bachelor groups have been raised (e.g., Parnell 2002) given the predominant social group-type observed in nature are mixedsex groups composed of a single adult male, several adult females, and offspring (Breuer et al. 2010;Gatti et al. 2004;Stokes 2004). ...

Preliminary Relationship between Overnight Separation and Wounding in Bachelor Groups of Western Lowland Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)
  • Citing Article
  • June 2021

Applied Animal Behaviour Science

... Females may prefer to mate with particular categories of males, depending on the characteristics of males, such as their maturity, capacity for investment, or genetic background [16,17]. Males that are selected by females can achieve high reproductive success [18][19][20][21][22]. Variance in male reproductive success has been investigated within social groups. ...

Dominance Rank, Female Choice, and Reproductive Success in Semi-Free Ranging Adult Male Japanese Macaques (Macaca fuscata)

American Journal of Primatology

... Behavioral observation studies of African apes have documented variability in positional behavior attributable to subspecies or population (Table 1; Hunt, 1992;Doran & Hunt, 1994;Doran, 1996), age (Doran, 1997;Sarringhaus et al., 2014), sex (Doran, 1993b;White et al., 2020), and body size (Hunt, 1994). Some have broadly characterized chimpanzees as more arboreal (climbing) than gorillas in comparative investigations of hominoid long bone diaphyseal structure (Marchi, 2005(Marchi, , 2007(Marchi, , 2015aRuff, 2002), as well as when inferring formfunction relationships in homininan long bones (Marchi, 2015b;Marchi et al., 2019). ...

Sex differences in bonobo (Pan paniscus) terrestriality: implications for human evolution

Journal of Anthropological Sciences