Eric van Dijk’s research while affiliated with Leiden University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (238)


Prototype features of enthusiasm, exemplars, centrality ratings (Study 2), frequencies (Study 1)
Ratings of central and peripheral features in enthusiasm and control condition
The internal structure of enthusiasm: a prototype analysis
  • Article
  • Full-text available

March 2025

·

26 Reads

Motivation and Emotion

Rijn Vogelaar

·

Eric van Dijk

·

The term enthusiasm is used frequently in both day-to-day language and professional settings. Scientifically, however, enthusiasm is not clearly defined. It is conceptualized and measured in different ways. In the present research, we examined the internal structure of enthusiasm. First, 28 features of enthusiasm were identified (Study 1.1) and rated on their centrality (Study 1.2). Results showed that features indicating joy and motivation were rated as central to the concept of enthusiasm, whereas features indicating restlessness and impatience were rated as less central. The validity of the central features was supported in three follow-up studies. More specifically, we found that the more central features were recalled better (Study 2.1), recognized faster (Study 2.2), and more often mentioned in autobiographical recalls of enthusiasm (Study 2.3). Taken together, the findings indicate that enthusiasm is prototypically structured, and that prototypical enthusiasm is a positive, energetic feeling that is associated with goal orientation and often involves interpersonal contact.

Download



The appraisal patterns and response types of enthusiasm: a comparison with joy and hope

November 2024

·

12 Reads

Enthusiasm is a relatively under-explored emotion. The current research explores the unique characteristics of enthusiasm by examining its cognitive appraisals (Study 1, N = 300) and response types (Study 2, N = 298) and comparing it with joy and hope. Participants in both studies recalled and rated events where they felt enthusiasm, joy, or hope. Study 1 revealed that enthusiasm occurs in pleasurable, intense situations linked to desired goals. More than joy, it is driven by goal-achievement anticipation. Compared to hope, enthusiasm is associated with more control, less uncertainty, and immediate relevance. Study 2 defines enthusiasm as a positive, energetic state marked by smiling, presence, fulfilment, and thoughts of positive outcomes. Compared to joy, it incites more eagerness, risk willingness, and inclination to join a movement. More than hope, it triggers immediate action without contemplation of negative outcomes. We conclude that enthusiasm is a positive, energetic condition often triggered by pleasurable, intense situations aligning with desired goals. It differs from joy and hope. Enthusiasm drives action when goals are attainable, and risks will likely pay off. Hope emerges when a goal is uncertain and distant. Joy typically follows goal accomplishment and is associated with feelings of connection and a desire to savour the moment.



The Indirect Effects of Communication Type, Emotion Type, and Their Interaction on the Choice of an Opponent via Morality, Sociability, and Competence. Note. Standardized coefficients β are reported. Dotted lines indicate non-significant effects (p > .05). Solid lines indicate significant effects; bolded lines indicate significant indirect effects (***p < .001)
The Indirect Effects of Communication Type, Emotion Type, and Their Interaction on the Choice of an Opponent (A)/a Representative (B) via Morality, Sociability, and Competence.Note. Standardized coefficients β are reported. Dotted lines indicate non-significant effects (p > .05). Solid lines indicate significant effects; bolded lines indicate significant indirect effects (*p < .05, ***p < .001)
Evaluating Negotiators Who Deceptively Communicate Anger or Happiness: On the Importance of Morality, Sociability, and Competence

October 2024

·

22 Reads

Journal of Business Ethics

Research has shown that negotiators sometimes misrepresent their emotions, and communicate a different emotion to opponents than they actually experience. Less is known about how people evaluate such negotiation tactics. Building on person perception literature, we investigated in three preregistered studies (N = 853) how participants evaluate negotiators who deceptively (vs. genuinely) communicate anger or happiness, on the dimensions of morality, sociability, and competence. Study 1 employed a buyer/seller setting, Studies 2 and 3 employed an Ultimatum Bargaining Game (UBG). In all studies, participants learned a negotiator’s (the target’s) experienced and communicated emotions (anger or happiness), before evaluating the target. Across studies, targets were evaluated lower on morality if they deceptively (vs. genuinely) communicated anger or happiness. Notably, negotiators deceptively communicating anger were evaluated lower on morality and sociability but higher on competence than those deceptively communicating happiness. Studies 2 and 3 investigated behavioral consequences by examining whether in a future negotiation participants chose the target to be their opponent or representative. Results showed that for opponents, participants preferred targets who genuinely communicated happiness (vs. anger), which was associated with their perceived morality or sociability. For representatives, participants not only preferred targets who had genuinely communicated happiness (vs. anger), but also targets who had deceptively communicated anger (vs. happiness), which was associated with their perceived competence. These findings show that when evaluating deceptive (vs. genuine) communication strategies, people distinguish between morality, sociability, and competence. The importance they attach to these dimensions is also contingent on the behavioral decisions they face.


The effects of social exclusion on distributive fairness judgments and cooperative behaviour

September 2024

·

37 Reads

British Journal of Social Psychology

In this article we investigate how being socially excluded (vs. included) affects people’s distributive fairness judgments and their willingness to cooperate with others in subsequent interactions. For this purpose, we conducted three experiments in which we assessed individual differences in having experienced being socially excluded (Experiment 1, N = 164), and manipulated social exclusion (Experiment 2, N = 120; Experiment 3, N = 492). We studied how this impacted fairness judgments of three different outcome distributions (disadvantageous inequality, advantageous inequality, and equality) both within-participants (Experiments 1 and 2) and between-participants (Experiment 3). To assess behavioural consequences, we then also assessed participants’ cooperation in a social dilemma game. Across the three experiments, we consistently found that social exclusion impacted fairness judgments. Compared to inclusion, excluded participants judged disadvantageous inequality as more unfair and advantageous inequality as less unfair. Moreover, compared to socially included participants, socially excluded participants were more willing to cooperate after experiencing advantageous rather than disadvantageous inequality, and feelings of acceptance served as a mediator in these associations.


Investigating lived ostracism: Valid causal inference requires articulating the causal estimand

August 2024

·

5 Reads

Our field is witnessing a surging interest in understanding lived experiences of ostracism instead of experiences induced in experiments. How do researchers draw valid causal conclusions about naturally occurring experiences of ostracism without relying on experimental designs? In this article, we put forth that a critical first step is to clearly state the causal quantity researchers aim to estimate (known as the causal estimand). This step should precede any data analysis. Using an intuitive example, we illustrate why the default causal estimand (average treatment effect) does not necessarily align with the causal effect of substantive interest pertaining to lived ostracism. We further review a selection of causal estimands which are more suited to answer causal queries of lived ostracism. We believe that selecting a causal estimand carefully tailored to the research question enhances the rigor and precision of research studying ostracism as a naturally occurring phenomenon.




Citations (77)


... Research by Simonse et al. (2024) explains that households with lower incomes are often financially stressed, which pressures family members to financially support the household, even children or extended family members. In comparison, higher-income families usually have more comprehensive economic resources and are thus able to delegate more to the household. ...

Reference:

Familial Responsibility and Involvement Towards Children on the Autism Spectrum
Economic Predictors of the Subjective Experience of Financial Stress.
  • Citing Article
  • April 2024

Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance

... Harga transfer yang dinegosiasikan adalah metode penetapan harga transfer yang banyak digunakan dan berpotensi berdampak pada laba divisi dan kompensasi manajer (Ghosh, & Boldt, 2006). Oleh karena itu, banyak negosiasi harga transfer yang tidak dapat mencapai kesepakatan yang integratif sehingga menimbulkan konflik antar divisi (Koning & van Dijk, 2013). ...

Motivated cognition in negotiation
  • Citing Chapter
  • June 2013

... The expression of happiness often elicits high demands or low concessions (Van Kleef et al., 2004a, 2004b. The expression of anger in general induces generous offers (Ye et al., 2023), but may also backfire when it elicits anger in the opponent . ...

So You Want Me to Believe You’re Happy or Angry? How Negotiators Perceive and Respond to Emotion Deception

Group Decision and Negotiation

... For post-experimental assessment of negative affect, we used two single items ranging from (not at all) to (very much) on a five-point Likert scale. These single face valid items for sadness (i.e., I felt sad) and anger (i.e., I felt angry) are well established and have been used in previous studies in the field of social rejection (Gutz et al., 2011;Meral et al., 2023;Renneberg et al., 2012;Williams, 2009;Wirth & Williams, 2009). In addition to assessing temporary mood changes, a single item for sadness has also been used for depression screening and mortality prediction (Lachs et al., 1990;St John & Montgomery, 2009). ...

Do targets of ostracism truthfully communicate their emotional reactions to sources?

Acta Psychologica

... What is often overlooked is that group cooperation requires defining who is part of the group and allowed to benefit from shared resources, and who is excluded 63 . As a result, distinct group boundaries and identities can emerge [64][65][66][67] , setting the stage for intergroup conflict and the inability to address common challenges collaboratively across group boundaries 63,68 . ...

Discriminatory punishment undermines the enforcement of group cooperation

... Veel onderzoek richt zich op de verschillende soorten drempels en barrières die mensen zelf ervaren bij het zoeken naar en het aanvragen van ondersteuning. Daarbij spelen verschillende factoren een rol, zoals onder meer een gebrek aan kennis (mensen weten niet waar zij terecht kunnen), bureaucratische barrières (ingewikkeld taalgebruik, onpersoonlijke behandeling, onbegrijpelijke formulieren), en psychologische drempels (de behoefte om het gevoel van zelfstandigheid of regie zoveel mogelijk te behouden, eerdere negatieve ervaringen, stigma, wantrouwen, schaamte) (Friele et al. 2018;Madsen et al. 2020;Marangos et al. 2013;Moynihan et al. 2015;Van Oorschot 1996a;Simonse et al. 2023). Dergelijke onderzoeken naar niet-gebruik hebben waardevolle inzichten opgeleverd, die eveneens relevant zijn in de context van deze studie. ...

Psychological barriers to take-up of healthcare and child support benefits in the Netherlands
  • Citing Article
  • April 2023

Journal of European Social Policy

... This could perhaps be so because, in this situation, employees are not very likely to craft their social resources and call on their support. In a similar vein, both cross-sectional and experimental vignettes studies found that the lower the intensity and the higher the ambiguity attributed to negative social acts, the more recipients resort to minimizing the situation instead of seeking support (Meral et al., 2022). ...

Intensity, intent, and ambiguity: Appraisals of workplace ostracism and coping responses

Aggressive Behavior

... Although a substantial body of evidence has documented the impact of pandemic financial stressors like reduced work or income on mental health, other basic needs stressors have received less attention [10]. Importantly, as infection levels rose, lockdowns impacted access to key services, including medical care, food, and housing, as well as overall feelings of safety and security, especially in Low-and-Middle Income (LMIC) settings [11]. ...

The role of financial stress in mental health changes during COVID-19

npj Mental Health Research

... The procedural fairness perceptions complement the distributional fairness perceptions and refer to cooperative members' perceptions of the objectivity and fairness of the cooperative process and decision-making procedures. The procedural fairness perceptions are conducive to promoting the construction of mutually beneficial and harmonious cooperative relationships between partners and enhancing cooperative performance [22]. The interaction fairness perceptions, on the other hand, are a further dimension based on the distributional and procedural fairness perceptions, focusing on the perception of the way of interaction and communication between the two parties within the cooperation. ...

Procedural fairness facilitates cooperative behavior by enhancing cooperative expectations

Social and Personality Psychology Compass

... On average, participants reported earning $53,902 per year (SD = $59,939, min = $0, max = $900,000). To manipulate participants' cooperative world belief, they first played an online ball tossing game called Social Ball which was designed to study ostracism (Meral et al., 2022). In this animated game, participants play a ball game with two other (fictitious) virtual players in which they are either included in or excluded from the game. ...

Social Ball: An immersive research paradigm to study social ostracism