Eric Potsdam’s research while affiliated with University of Florida and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (49)


The Malagasy phrasal comparative
  • Article

February 2024

·

11 Reads

Syntax

Eric Potsdam

There is no consensus in the literature on the analysis of phrasal comparatives. Both reduced clause analyses, in which the standard phrase contains elided clausal structure, and direct analyses, in which the standard of comparison is a direct complement to the standard marker, have been proposed. This paper argues for a direct analysis of the phrasal comparative in Malagasy, an Austronesian language spoken on the island of Madagascar. Evidence for the direct analysis comes from the lack of overt clausal comparatives, Binding Theory, scope, and Malagasy‐specific characteristics of the standard. The conclusion contributes to the rapidly expanding picture of cross‐linguistic variation in comparative syntax.


Malagasy framing demonstratives and the syntax of doubling
  • Article
  • Full-text available

January 2024

·

38 Reads

·

1 Citation

Glossa a journal of general linguistics

Malagasy demonstratives appear simultaneously initially and finally within the DP and must be identical: ity boky ity ‘DEM book DEM’ “this book”. We argue that the unusual doubling pattern arises from multiple pronunciation of a single demonstrative formative and not from base-generation of two independent formatives. The primary goal is to show that doubling, which has been amply discussed in the verbal domain, particularly in the literature on verb doubling in predicate clefts, also occurs in the nominal domain and can be successfully analyzed with existing theoretical machinery.

Download

Exceptive constructions in Japanese *

March 2023

·

180 Reads

We presented and analyzed the expression of exception in Japanese, formally marked with the postposition igai. As a postposition, igai combines with a noun phrase. The internal structure of that noun phrase can be quite complex; in particular, it can include a nominalized CP. Japanese allows both connected and exceptives, which differ, among other things, by whether the exception and the associate form a constituent (yes for the former, no for the latter). We have shown that Japanese free exceptives always include an underlying nominalized CPs (sometimes headed by a null nominal head), with elided material. This kind of ellipsis is different from clausal ellipsis in exceptives in languages like English where no nominal or determiner head is attested. Until now, only two types of free exceptives have been recognized: non-clausal phrasal ones (unattested so far) and clausal (with ellipsis), as in English or Egyptian Arabic (Soltan 2016). The novel Japanese results thus enrich the existing typology of exceptive constructions by recognizing a nominalized CP as another source of exceptive constrictions.


Deriving VOS from VSO in Tongan

August 2021

·

374 Reads

·

1 Citation

Research on word order has established several possible ways in which VOS order can be derived from VSO order. This chapter considers the derivational relationship between VSO and VOS in the Polynesian language Tongan. VSO order is basic in Tongan, and we address the derivation of VOS from this basic order in the context of multiple possibilities. We argue that Tongan VOS is better analyzed as rightward displacement of the subject as opposed to leftward displacement of the object proposed by Otsuka (2005a,c). The clause-final subject shows many of the hallmarks of rightward movement, including information-structural restrictions, locality with respect to the matrix clause, lack of clitic doubling, and connectivity with respect to case and binding. Given that rightward movement has an uneasy place in syntactic theory, we take pains to establish that the analysis is successful and worth further scrutiny (141 words). VSO appears to be the contextually most neutral order; in particular, it is found in the opening to narratives or conversations (Tchekoff 1981, Otsuka 2000, Custis 2004), is appropriate as a response to broad-focus and verum-focus questions (Polinsky 2016), and in general is used in a broader range of contexts than VOS. In this work, we follow other researchers who assume it as the basic order. However, Tongan also allows VOS order, (1b), in a number of situations. Relevant subjects here and below are underlined.


Malagasy extraposition

February 2021

·

79 Reads

·

2 Citations

Natural Language & Linguistic Theory

Extraposition is the non-canonical placement of dependents in a right-peripheral position in a clause. The Austronesian language Malagasy has basic VOXS word order, however, extraposition leads to VOSX. Extraposed constituents behave syntactically as though they were in their undisplaced position inside the predicate at both LF and Spell Out. This paper argues that extraposition is achieved via movement at Phonological Form (PF). I argue against alternatives that would derive extraposition with syntactic A’ movement or stranding analyses. Within a Minimalist model of grammar, movement operations take place on the branch from Spell Out to PF and have only phonological consequences.


Control Phenomena

August 2017

·

1,046 Reads

·

4 Citations

Control phenomena have been at the center of syntactic theorizing almost since the inception of generative grammar. To first approximation, control is an interpretive dependency between two arguments: a lexicalized noun phrase (the controller) and a null argument in a dependent clause (the controllee), as in Sandy hopes [Ø to succeed]. The primary concerns of syntactic theories have been accounting for various characteristics of the null controllee and the identification of its controller. This chapter surveys some of the phenomena that have driven our current understanding of these issues. Within the domain of obligatory control, some phenomena speak to the controller: the behavior of polyadic predicates, implicit control, variable control and control shift, and exceptional cases like promise. Other phenomena speak to the controllee: exhaustive control, partial control, split control, and control in embedded questions. Assuming that nonobligatory control is a kind of elsewhere case, the phenomena relevant to nonobligatory control are those where obligatory control restrictions on controller choice fail to materialize: control into subjects, extraposed clauses, and some adjunct clauses. The chapter also discusses phenomena with a non-canonical realization of the control relation: backward control and copy control. Within the context of these phenomena, the chapter surveys current generative approaches to the analysis of control: dominant PRO-based approaches, the movement theory of Control, and restructuring analyses. Relative strengths and weaknesses are highlighted.



Left edge topics in Russian and the processing of anaphoric dependencies

November 2014

·

129 Reads

·

9 Citations

Journal of Linguistics

This paper investigates the cost of processing syntactic versus extra-syntactic dependencies. The results support the hypothesis that syntactic dependencies require less processing effort than discourse-derived dependencies do (Reuland 2001, 2011; Koornneef 2008). The point is made through the analysis of a novel paradigm in Russian in which a preposed nominal stranding a numeral can show number connectivity (paucal) with a gap following the numeral or can appear in a non-agreeing (plural) form, as in cathedral-paucal/plural, there were three.paucal __. Numerous syntactic diagnostics confirm that when there is number connectivity, the nominal has been fronted via A-chain dependency. In the absence of connectivity, the construction involves a hanging topic related via discourse mechanisms to a base-generated null pronoun. The constructions constitute a minimal pair and Reuland's proposals correctly predict that the A-chain is more easily processed than an anaphoric dependency involving a null pronoun, which must be resolved by discourse-based mechanisms.


Topics at the left periphery in Russian

February 2014

·

40 Reads

In this tribute to Knud Lambrecht, a pioneer of Information Structure, a diverse group of scholars examines the intersection of syntax, discourse, pragmatics, and semantics. The six chapters in the first section of the volume consider issues of grammar with new theoretical and applied insights, pertaining to grammatical constructions such as left dislocation, unaccusatives, null complements, and passives. While the first half of the book presents studies involving a range of languages from Russian to Irish to Italian, the second section is dedicated to papers focused on French. These five chapters feature the application of Construction Grammar and/or Information Structure frameworks to prosody and second language processing, as well as to several distinctive spoken French constructions: clefts, left dislocations, and interrogatives. Collectively, this book offers substantial reading for those interested in the juncture of structure and context, notably a critical take on the important legacy of a preeminent linguist.


CP-Negation and the Domain of Quantifier Raising

October 2013

·

69 Reads

·

4 Citations

Linguistic Inquiry

It is widely stated that the scope of quantifiers is clause-bound (Chomsky 1977, May 1977, Farkas 1981, 1997, Fodor and Sag 1982, Aoun and Hornstein 1985, Beghelli 1993, Abusch 1994, Hornstein 1995, Fox and Sauerland 1997, and numerous others). This claim is based on the observation that (1a) has no reading in which reviewers covary with plays, while (2a) does. 1. a. A reviewer thinks every play will fail this season. b. *[TP every playi [TP a reviewer thinks [CP ei will fail this season]]] 2. a. A reviewer attended every play this season. b. [TP every playi [TP a reviewer attended ei this season]] Current theories capture the contrast by making Quantifier Raising (QR), the covert syntactic operation that assigns scope to quantified noun phrases (QPs), clause-bound. Only in (2a), in which the universally quantified QP every play and the indefinite a reviewer are clausemates, can the universal QP raise to scope over the indefinite, yielding the Logical Form representation (LF) in (2b). This operation is prohibited for (1a) because the two NPs are not clausemates; hence, the LF in (1b) is illicit. While this observation seems empirically well-grounded, its theoretical basis is less secure. Such clause-boundedness makes QR rather more restricted than one would expect a representative Ā-movement to be (see Reinhart 1997, Cecchetto 2004 for discussion). In what follows, I recap Fox's (1995, 2000) theory of Scope Economy, which provides an explanation for QR's clause-boundedness. I then introduce new data involving the interaction between QPs and certain instances of negation that are problematic for this approach. I conclude by sketching an alternative. Like many who research QR, Fox (1995, 2000) implicitly adopts what Beghelli and Stowell (1997) call Scope Uniformity: QR applies uniformly to all QPs and is not landing-site-selective. Any QP can be adjoined to any (nonargument) XP where it is interpretable. Further restricting this quite general assumption, economy considerations dictate that QR can apply only if it has an effect on semantic interpretation. QR cannot apply if the derivation without it would yield the same meaning. 3. Scope Economy (Fox 2000:23) QR must have a semantic effect. Fox (2000:62-66) proposes that the clause-boundedness of QR follows from Scope Economy. By (3), every application of QR must induce a change in semantic interpretation. At the same time, given that QR is an instance of Ā-movement, each application is subject to locality constraints on movement, which Fox formulates as Shortest Move. 4. Shortest Move (Fox 2000:23) QR moves a QP to the closest position in which it is interpretable. The impossibility of cross-clausal QR follows from a tension between Scope Economy and Shortest Move. QR that does not obey Shortest Move is illicit, but QR that targets a clausal node, obeying Shortest Move, will not normally yield a new semantic interpretation, violating Scope Economy. Returning to (1a), the two constraints derive the unavailability of the non-clause-bound reading of the universal QP in this example, repeated below as (5a). Two potential LFs for the wide scope reading of the QP every play are given in (5b-c). In (5b), every play raises directly to a position above the matrix subject; however, this violates Shortest Move since adjunction to the embedded clause is a closer interpretable position. In (5c), the QP every play targets the embedded clausal node to satisfy Shortest Move; however, Scope Economy is now violated because the move has no semantic consequence. As a result, the embedded QP has no extra, wide scope interpretation, as desired. 5. a. A reviewer thinks every play will fail this season. b. *[TP every playi [TP a reviewer thinks [CP[TP ei will fail this season]]]] c. *[TP every playi [TP a reviewer thinks [CP ei [CP[TP ei will fail this season]]]]] Fox (2000:63) points out that the Scope Economy account makes a surprising prediction: QR's clause-boundedness could be overridden if adjunction to CP (as in (5c)) had semantic motivation. Specifically, if the CP projection contained an element...


Citations (37)


... 2: it is the sole mechanism that gives rise to unmarked postverbal orders beyond VSOD. This initial result suggests that apparent cases of rightward scrambling cannot be universally derived from conspiracies of movement to the left (Bhatt and Dayal 2007;Manetta 2012;Simpson and Choudhury 2014;Polinsky and Potsdam 2021;pace Mahajan 1997). In other words, these facts of postverbal constituency deliver an argument that the language faculty allows movement to positions that are linearized to the right. ...

Reference:

The prosody of the extended VP
Deriving VOS from VSO in Tongan

... In other languages, different underlying structures for sluicing have been proposed. Many Austronesian languages employ the pseudocleft structure for whquestion formation, where the wh-phrase is the predicate and a headless relativeclause is the subject (Potsdam 2006;Potsdam and Polinsky 2011). Consequently, in several Austronesian languages, it has been argued that sluicing is derived from pseudocleft structures (Potsdam 2007;Wei 2011;Paul and Potsdam 2012;Borise 2016;Drummond To appear). ...

Questions and word order in Polynesian

... Especially prominent, in this perspective, is work exploring the link between a speaker's pragmatic behavior and politeness inferences, a variety of social inferences that listeners draw about the type of interpersonal relationship that a speaker aims to establish with the interlocutor -e.g., how respectful, tactful, rude or threatening towards the interlocutor the speaker comes across as depending on the amount and type of information communicated. In foundational contributions to this topic, Lakoff (1974) and Brown and Levinson (1987) suggest that politeness inferences often arise as the result of a violation of one or more Gricean maxims. For example, uttering seemingly irrelevant ("It's cold in here") or verbose utterances ("Would you mind closing the window?") in lieu of more 2 These temporary dispositions are often referred to as stances in the sociolinguistics and discourse analysis literature. ...

Proceedings of Chicago Linguistic Society 37
  • Citing Article
  • January 2001

... In addition to Almoaily [14] who discussed variation in Gulf Pidgin Arabic, Potsdam and Alanazi [15] proposed a unified analysis of the preposition ‫/ﻓﻲ/‬ 'in/there's' in this variety, and found that it has several uses such as: a preposition, an existential expletive, and the verb of possession 'have'. ...

Fi in Gulf Pidgin Arabic

Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics

... However, this appears to be too strong, as antireconstruction and surface scope relations are found with such sentences (Bailyn 2006). Thus, (63) shows a preference for surface scope (the only option for some speakers), consistent with the analysis of Russian scope in Antonyuk 2015, which is exactly the opposite of what is claimed to be found with Japanese longdistance scrambling (Saito 1992 Examples such as (63) demonstrate clearly, then, that the only viable base-generation theory of such constructions would be one similar to Polinsky and Potsdam's (2014) account of the topical genitive plural constructions illustrated in footnote 43, in which a high base-generated element is discourse-related to a lower pro element in the structure. Such a theory could explain (63) without sacrificing base-generation: the surface left-dislocated element could serve as the locus of semantic interpretation, allowing for the high scope reading. ...

Left edge topics in Russian and the processing of anaphoric dependencies

Journal of Linguistics

... It has since been claimed for several languages, including Japanese (Fujii 2006), Malagasy (Potsdam 2009), Romanian and Greek (Alboiu 2007, Alexiadou et al. 2010, Mizo (Sino-Tibetan) , Telugu (Dravidian) (Haddad 2009), and Assamese (Haddad 2011). Some of these languages license alternating control structures in which the overt element in a control relation may be pronounced either in the embedded clause or in the matrix clause (Haddad and Potsdam 2013). ...

Linearizing the Control Relation: A Typology

... This idea, which can be traced back at least to Laka (1990), was also advanced by proponents of the VP-internal origin theory, like De Clercq (2013; 2020a). Kramer & Rawlins (2011) also propose a left-peripheral ΣP/PolP, which hosts answer particles, and an association between negation and CP, especially with respect to quantifier scope, is explored by Potsdam (2013). More recently, Greco (2019;b) proposed that expletive negation, and in particular an Italian incarnation of expletive negation participating in a marked construction which he names Surprise Negation, is base-generated in the higher left periphery, selecting a FocP whose Specifier is filled by movement of the TP. ...

CP-Negation and the Domain of Quantifier Raising
  • Citing Article
  • October 2013

Linguistic Inquiry

... There has been much written on the merits of the MTC. Empirically, the movement approach is capable of accounting for backward and copy control structures (Polinsky and Potsdam 2001, Potsdam 2006, Boeckx, Hornstein, and Nunes 2007, Haddad 2009, Hornstein and Polinsky 2010b. This success is due to analyzing control as movement coupled with the Copy Theory of Movement and the option of pronouncing non-highest copies (Nunes 2004). ...

Backward Object Control in Malagasy: Against an Empty Category Analysis
  • Citing Article
  • January 2006