December 1999
·
117 Reads
·
2 Citations
The American Journal of Psychology
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
December 1999
·
117 Reads
·
2 Citations
The American Journal of Psychology
December 1999
·
3 Reads
The American Journal of Psychology
June 1997
·
86 Reads
·
183 Citations
Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance
Three experiments examined whether negative priming is a dually determined effect produced by inhibitory mechanisms and by a memorial process. Younger adults (Experiment 1) and older adults (Experiments 1-3) were tested in procedures that varied the likelihood of inducing retrieval of the prior trial. This was done by making test-trial target decoding difficult (Experiments 1 & 2) or by making prior information useful on some nonnegative priming trials (Experiment 3). Younger adults demonstrated negative priming under retrieval and nonretrieval conditions, with patterns of performance indicating different sources of negative priming effects. Older adults showed negative priming only under retrieval-inducing conditions, consistent with the view of deficient inhibitory mechanisms for older adults. The data suggest that contextual variables critically determine whether negative priming is largely due to inhibition or to episodic retrieval.
July 1996
·
282 Reads
·
138 Citations
This chapter discusses the status of the inhibitory view of working memory and its relation to aging. It considers evidence for the conceptualizations of working memory that stress its purported limited capacity and presents an alternative view which focuses on the inhibitory control of the contents of working memory. The chapter also discusses current research evidence concerning adult age differences in attention, memory, and language.
July 1996
·
1,214 Reads
·
139 Citations
The purpose of this book is to compare and contrast different conceptions of working memory. This is one of the most important notions to have informed cognitive psychology over the last twenty years, and it has been used in a wide variety of ways. This, in part, is because contemporary usage of the phrase "working memory" encapsulates various themes that have appeared at different points in the history of research into human memory and cognition. The book presents three dominant views of working memory.
June 1996
·
78 Reads
·
32 Citations
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
In two experiments, the pattern of persistence of negative priming effects across delay intervals of 500 and 2,500 msec was assessed using a within-subjects, random sequencing of delays. Neill and Valdes (1992; Neill, Valdes, Terry, & Gorfein, 1992) have argued that a within-subject experimental design is required for decay of negative priming to be seen, in contrast to results reported elsewhere (e.g., Tipper, Weaver, Cameron, Brehaut, & Bastedo, 1991) showing stable negative priming effects across delays. In neither experiment was substantial evidence of decay detected, raising questions for the notion that suppression necessarily declines across brief temporal intervals and for the assertion that episodic retrieval is the sole source of negative priming.
March 1994
·
368 Reads
·
284 Citations
Psychology and Aging
Two experiments sought to elicit distractor suppression in older adults (aged 62–78 yrs). Exp 1 used a procedure that increased suppression in younger adults (aged 17–25 yrs), thus creating a more sensitive measure of suppression in older adults. To compensate for older adults' slowed processing, Exp 2 used a longer stimulus exposure duration. Neither experiment produced suppression in older adults; both experiments, however, included trial types that elicited parallel facilitatory effects for both age groups. Older adults thus seemed to process distractors but failed to engage inhibitory mechanisms in their rejection of distracting stimuli. Finally, both experiments tested the relationships among suppression, interference, and everyday cognitive failure. Neither experiment suggested relationships between reaction time (RT) effects and self-reported cognitive lapses. Results are discussed within L. Hasher and R. T. Zacks's (1988) attentional framework. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
September 1993
·
380 Reads
·
252 Citations
Psychological Science
Across two studies comparing younger and older adults, age differences in optimal performance periods were identified (Study 1), and then shown to be an important determinant of memory differences (Study 2). A norming study showed that while most younger adults were Evening or Neutral types, as determined by a standard questionnaire, the vast majority of older adults were Morning types. A second study compared the recognition performance of younger and older adults tested in the morning or in the late afternoon. Substantial age differences were found in the late afternoon, when younger but not older adults were at their optimal times. However, no age differences in memory performance were found in the morning, when older but not younger adults were at their peak period. Thus, synchrony between optimal performance periods and the time at which testing is conducted may well be a critical variable in determining group differences in intellectual performance, particularly between older and younger adults.
July 1993
·
315 Reads
·
158 Citations
Journal of Gerontology
Previous work (Hasher, Stoltzfus, Zacks, & Rypma, 1991) suggested the existence of adult age-related differences in the ability to suppress or inhibit irrelevant information. This investigation explored age differences in the time course of suppression. Experiments 1 and 2 showed that younger adults demonstrate the same level of suppression at 300 as they do at 1,700 ms after a selection response. Older adults consistently show no suppression. Experiments 2 and 3 also examined the relationship between suppression and the degree to which distractors interfere with concurrent selection. The absence of a reliable relationship--both within and across age groups--together with other findings in the literature, raise questions about the function of suppression as a mechanism of concurrent selection. Another function, one that aids in the establishment of a coherent thought stream, is proposed.
January 1993
·
18 Reads
·
9 Citations
... Changes in cognitive control have been used to explain the development of cognitive abilities (Diamond & Gilbert, 1989;Ridderinkhof, vanderMolen, Band, & Bashore, 1997) and age-related declines in cognitive abilities (Hasher, Rypma, Stoltzfus, & Zacks, 1989). Moreover, an abundance of research illustrates that individual differences in cognitive control are related to individual differences in working memory span, reading comprehension, problem solving, general cognitive ability, and judgment and decision making (De Beni, Palladino, Pazzaglia, & Cornoldi, 1998;Dempster & Corkill, 1999;Dougherty & Hunter, 2003;Friedman & Miyake, 2004;Gernsbacher, 1993;Kane & Engle, 2002). ...
January 1989
Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
... However, Stoltzfus, Hasher, and Zacks (1996) have suggested that there has been a debate in the field of cognition between cognitive theorists to find the best definition for W.M. ...
Reference:
MY dissertation
July 1996
... Given the nature of nonconscious goal pursuit, it seems plausible to suggest that it recruits relatively implicit working memory, or a nonconscious executive. The idea of implicit WM (whether as a mode of operation, or as a separate mechanism) is undoubtedly foreign to the current zeitgeist in the cognitive sciences (e.g., Baars & Franklin, 2003;Baddeley, 1993;Cowan, 1999;Dudai, 2004;Gathercole, 2007;Kintsch, Healy, Hegarety, Pennington, & Salthouse, 1999;O'Reilly, Braver, & Cohen, 1999). Our data, however, are not the only data that are consistent with it. ...
December 1999
The American Journal of Psychology
... In a between-subjects design, inhibition of negative priming can be observed in responses even when the delay between the prime and probe is up to 7 s (Tipper et al., 1991). When varying delay intervals are introduced in a within-subjects design, the negative priming persists for intervals of up to 2500 ms (Hasher et al., 1996). ...
June 1996
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
... Additionally, since the majority of participants in the current study had an intermediate chronotype, it could be interesting to expand this research and include individuals with more extreme chronotypes, such as morning or evening. For instance, the elderly population, presenting a more prominent morning 33,34 , could be an interesting model to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the real link between chronotype and memory consolidation, and thus, broaden the applicability of our findings. ...
September 1993
Psychological Science
... Over the last two years, we have been exploring a vision for the future of intelligent and autonomous systems as cognitive cyber-physical systems [9,10]. The main idea is to endow cyber-physical systems with cognitive capabilities such as memory, attention, learning, problem solving, etc. [11][12][13][14]. In this paper, we will explore the idea that the human brain functions as a "preadaptive organ" endowing the human the ability to adapt proactively by anticipating changes instead of reactively [15,16]. ...
July 1996
... The decrease in attentional filtering/distractor suppression in older adults has been extensively studied in a variety of contexts. For example, Kane et al. (1994) showed that compared to younger adults, older adults have a reduced ability to filter out irrelevant information. Specifically, older adults, but not younger adults, were faster to name a word in the target color of a test display if it matched a to-be-ignored word in the distractor color from the previous prime display. ...
March 1994
Psychology and Aging
... 62 Moreover, the inhibition of the NP effect does not decline over time. 20 Thus, the inhibition of overlearned strategy on the prime stage could sustain to the probe stage. In other words, the inhibition may not only exist in incongruent items but also in congruent items following the incongruent items. ...
January 1991
Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition
... In the domain of creativity, primed elements are used to provide examples for creative tasks that follow (Marsh et al. 1999) and enhance the flexibility of creative performance, as well as impacting convergent thinking process because the priming effect activates remote associations (Sassenberg et al. 2017). However, creative performance influenced by the priming effect is restricted as the generated ideas show homogeneity with the provided example in the priming stage (Rubin et al. 1991). In VE, instead of the traditional way (i.e., exposing the priming objects before the task), the priming stimuli can be merged with the general environment (i.e., contextual priming) (Bhagwatwar et al. 2018). ...
February 1991
Memory & Cognition
... These findings are consistent with previous evidence [7,27] and provide interesting insight into the developmental trajectories of inhibitory control. In fact, although studies have largely addressed the substantial changes in inhibitory control that occur in childhood [34][35][36] and in late adulthood [37,38] little evidence has considered changes across the lifespan [39]. Typically, studies comparing only two (or three) samples, such as young versus older adults (e.g [40]) or children versus adolescents versus young adults (e.g [41]), could be useful for highlighting age differences but not for defining the developmental trajectories of inhibitory abilities. ...
July 1993
Journal of Gerontology