Elin Thordardottir's research while affiliated with McGill University and other places
What is this page?
This page lists the scientific contributions of an author, who either does not have a ResearchGate profile, or has not yet added these contributions to their profile.
It was automatically created by ResearchGate to create a record of this author's body of work. We create such pages to advance our goal of creating and maintaining the most comprehensive scientific repository possible. In doing so, we process publicly available (personal) data relating to the author as a member of the scientific community.
If you're a ResearchGate member, you can follow this page to keep up with this author's work.
If you are this author, and you don't want us to display this page anymore, please let us know.
It was automatically created by ResearchGate to create a record of this author's body of work. We create such pages to advance our goal of creating and maintaining the most comprehensive scientific repository possible. In doing so, we process publicly available (personal) data relating to the author as a member of the scientific community.
If you're a ResearchGate member, you can follow this page to keep up with this author's work.
If you are this author, and you don't want us to display this page anymore, please let us know.
Publications (62)
Purpose
Bilingual development has been shown to be highly dependent on language-specific exposure. This study extended the study of bilingual development to adolescence and to the production of complex syntax in two contexts: conversation and expository discourse.
Method
English–French bilinguals (EFbil; n = 27) and French L1 speakers ( n = 14) ag...
Aims
The sparse available evidence on verbal fluency in bilingual children shows them to perform lower than monolinguals on semantic fluency (taken as indicating vocabulary) but on par or better on phonemic fluency (taken as indicating executive functioning). This study takes a more detailed look at verbal fluency skills in bilingual children by ex...
Objectifs. La haute valeur diagnostique des tests de répétitions de non-mots (RNM) pour identifier le trouble développemental du langage (TDL) a été largement démontrée pour les enfants monolingues et bilingues d’âge préscolaire et scolaire. Cette étude étend l’analyse de la RNM à des enfants plus âgés. Méthodologie. Un test existant de RNM en lang...
Purpose
This study examines the degree to which adolescents in Iceland are judged by native Icelandic speakers to have a foreign accent both in Icelandic and in English, two languages that are learned under different conditions, as the community and school language, and through school and incidental exposure.
Method
Fifty-eight adolescents, 27 wit...
In 2016, for English-speaking countries, the CATALISE project, agreed on common terminology and criteria for reporting unexplained oral language impairment in children. The term "Developmental Language Disorder" (DLD) was recommended for language difficulties that cause functional impact in everyday life and that are associated with a poor prognosi...
Language acquisition and Language Maintenance (LM) both depend on ample opportunity and motivation. Currently, many national language societies are undergoing Language Shift (LS) to English, impacting their acquisition as first (L1) and possibly even more as second (L2) language. This study interviewed 44 adolescents, including 24 L1 and 20 L2 spea...
This study examined the long-term language outcomes of adolescents in the complex multilingual context of Iceland, where children learn Icelandic as a first (L1) or second language (L2), in a background of incidental English from internet and media sources.
Method
50 adolescents enrolled in grades 8, 9 and 10 in Reykjavik (Iceland) public schools...
Public awareness of language impairment in childhood (Developmental Language Disorder (DLD)) has been identified as an important determiner of research and clinical service delivery, yet studies directly assessing public awareness are lacking. This study surveyed awareness across 18 countries of Europe.
Method
A questionnaire developed by an inter...
In children with a hearing loss who receive cochlear implants (CIs) under the age of 2, regular assessments are conducted to monitor auditory and linguistic progress. However, the collection of authentic, representative, and reliable expressive language data on young children with CIs remains a challenge. The purpose of the study was to determine w...
Objective:
This article examines the efficacy of language intervention services for monolingual and immigrant children in a public clinic in Montreal, Canada. Intervention is provided in French for a preset number of sessions regardless of intervention needs. The study assessed immediate gains after intervention, their maintenance over 2 months, a...
Evidence based practice calls for clinical decisions to be based on a combination of research evidence, clinical expertise and client perspectives. A relatively small proportion of the research evidence on language intervention efficacy has focused specifically on bilingual children. This article reviews early research as well as recent findings on...
In response to the recent sharp increase of L2 students in Reykjavik schools, allocation criteria for special L2 services were adopted that were based on length of residence and on whether children’s home language was tonal or not tonal. This study set out to evaluate the appropriateness of these criteria, and to replicate previous findings of a sm...
Objectives
This study examined the extent to which the language performance of school-age bilingual children is impacted by the amount of language exposure they have received in each language versus the timing of this exposure in terms of the age of first exposure (AoE).
Methods
Receptive and expressive vocabulary and word morphology measures were...
Purpose:
Grammatical morphology continues to be widely regarded as an area of extraordinary difficulty in children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). A main argument for this view is the purported high diagnostic accuracy of morphological errors for the identification of SLI. However, findings are inconsistent across age groups and across la...
We present a new set of subjective age-of-acquisition (AoA) ratings for 299 words (158 nouns, 141 verbs) in 25 languages from five language families (Afro-Asiatic: Semitic languages; Altaic: one Turkic language: Indo-European: Baltic, Celtic, Germanic, Hellenic, Slavic, and Romance languages; Niger-Congo: one Bantu language; Uralic: Finnic and Ugri...
Amount of language exposure is correlated with bilingual lexical development, but findings are mixed on how exposure relates to nonword repetition (NWR), a complex skill involving both short-term processing and long-term vocabulary knowledge. We extend previous work to a younger age group by investigating the role of exposure on NWR versus vocabula...
Purpose:
This study investigated the clinical effectiveness of monolingual versus bilingual language intervention, the latter involving speech-language pathologist-parent collaboration. The study focuses on methods that are currently being recommended and that are feasible within current clinical contexts.
Method:
Bilingual children with primary...
Purpose:
The study examined the effect of bilingual input on the grammatical development of bilingual children in comparison to monolingual peers.
Method:
Spontaneous language samples were collected in English and French from typically-developing bilingual and monolingual pre-schoolers aged 3 years (n = 56) and 5 years (n = 83). Within each age...
The goal of the present study is to present a tool for screening speech sound disorders among French-speaking preschool-aged children. Presently, there are no tools supported by research and normative data available to evaluate consonant production in French-speaking preschool-aged children. The present screening tool consists of 40 words. The prel...
Speech-language evaluation of Franco-Ontarian children is a complex task for speech-language pathologists due to the lack of tools and regional standards. This study first replicated, with 26 Franco-Ontarian monolingual children, a study from Quebec (Thordardottir, Keheyia, Lessard, Sutton et Trudeau. 2010). Children in the study were matched for a...
Abrégé
Le but de cette étude est de présenter un outil de dépistage des troubles du développement des
sons de la parole pour les enfants franco-canadiens d’âge préscolaire. Présentement, il n’existe
pas d’outil appuyé par des données normatives pour évaluer la production des consonnes chez les
enfants francophones d’âge préscolaire. L’outil de dépi...
Purpose:
Nonword repetition (NWR) and sentence imitation (SI) are increasingly used as diagnostic tools for the identification of Primary Language Impairment (PLI). They may be particularly promising diagnostic tools for bilingual children if performance on them is not highly affected by bilingual exposure. Two studies were conducted which examine...
School-age children (n=39) acquiring Icelandic as a second language were tested yearly over three years on Icelandic measures of language knowledge and language processing. Comparison with native speaker norms revealed large and significant differences for the great majority of the children. Those who scored within the normal monolingual range had...
The relationship between amount of bilingual exposure and performance in receptive and expressive vocabulary in French and English was examined in 5-year-old Montreal children acquiring French and English simultaneously as well as in monolingual children. The children were equated on age, socio-economic status, nonverbal cognition, and on minority/...
This study provides a systematic description of French consonant acquisition in a large cohort of pre-school aged children: 156 children aged 20-53 months participated in a picture-naming task. Five analyses were conducted to study consonant acquisition: (1) consonant inventory, (2) consonant accuracy, (3) consonant acquisition, (4) a comparison of...
Unlabelled:
Evidence-based practice requires that clinical decisions be based on evidence from rigorously controlled research studies. At this time, very few studies have directly examined the efficacy of clinical intervention methods for bilingual children. Clinical decisions for this population cannot, therefore, be based on the strongest forms...
Research on the diagnostic accuracy of different language measures has focused primarily on English. This study examined the sensitivity and specificity of a range of measures of language knowledge and language processing for the identification of primary language impairment (PLI) in French-speaking children. Because of the lack of well-documented...
We explored the relationship between working memory (WM) and visually controlled attention (CA) in young bilingual and monolingual children. Previous research has shown that balanced bilingual children outperform monolinguals in CA. However, it is unclear whether this advantage is truly associated with bilingualism or whether potential WM and/or la...
It is generally recommended that bilingual children be assessed in both of their languages. However, specific procedures for such bilingual assessment and for interpretation of the results are lacking. Normally developing French – English bilingual preschool-age children were compared to monolingual children (n = 28) on expressive and receptive mea...
Previous research has indicated that the manifestation of specific language impairment (SLI) varies according to factors such as language, age, and task. This study examined the effect of task demands on language production in children with SLI cross-linguistically.
Icelandic- and English-speaking school-age children with SLI and normal language (N...
Neurologic and radiologic findings in children with well-defined developmental language impairment have rarely been systematically assessed. Children aged 7 to 13 years with developmental language impairment or normal language (controls) underwent language, nonverbal cognitive, motor and neurological assessments, standardized assessment for subtle...
This study examines inflectional abilities in French-speaking children with specific language impairment (SLI) using a verb elicitation task. Eleven children with SLI and age-matched controls (37–52 months) participated in the experiment. We elicited the passé composé using eight regular and eight irregular high frequency verbs matched for age of a...
This study examines inflectional abilities in French-speaking children with specific language impairment (SLI) using a verb elicitation task. Eleven children with SLI and age-matched controls (37–52 months) participated in the experiment. We elicited the passé composé using eight regular and eight irregular high frequency verbs matched for age of a...
Studies on specific language impairment (SLI) in French have identified specific aspects of morphosyntax as particularly vulnerable. However, a cohesive picture of relative strengths and weaknesses characterizing SLI in French has not been established. In light of normative data showing low morphological error rates in the spontaneous language of F...
Our goal was to evaluate detailed school-age language, nonverbal cognitive, and motor development in children with developmental language impairment compared with age-matched controls.
Children with developmental language impairment or normal language development (controls) aged 7 to 13 years were recruited. Children underwent language assessment (...
Children with Down syndrome (DS) have cognitive disabilities resulting from trisomy 21. Language-learning difficulties, especially expressive language problems, are an important component of the phenotype of this population. Many individuals with DS are born into bilingual environments. To date, however, there is almost no information available reg...
The Lidcombe Program is an operant treatment for early stuttering. Outcomes indicate that the program is effective; however, the underlying mechanisms leading to a successful reduction of stuttering remain unknown. The purpose of this study was to determine whether fluency achieved with the Lidcombe Program was accompanied by concomitant reduction...
Although a number of studies have been conducted on normal acquisition in French, systematic methods for analysis of French and normative group data have been lacking.
To develop a systematic method for the analysis of language samples in Quebec French, and to provide preliminary normative data on early lexical and syntactic development in French w...
In spite of the complexity of verb argument structure, argument structure errors are infrequent in the speech of children with specific language impairment (SLI). The study examined the spontaneous argument structure use of school-age children with SLI and with normal language (NL) (n = 100). The groups did not differ substantially in frequency of...
The use of complex syntax was investigated in narrative language samples of older children and adolescents with Down syndrome (n = 24) and a group of typically developing children matched on mean length of utterance. Both groups used conjoined and subordinate sentence forms and did not differ significantly in either the proportion of utterances con...
Linguistic nonfluencies known as mazes (filled pauses, repetitions, revisions, and abandoned utterances) have been used to draw inferences about processing difficulties associated with the production of language. In children with normal language development (NL), maze frequency in general increases with linguistic complexity, being greater in narra...
Linguistic nonfluencies known as mazes (filled pauses, repetitions, revisions, and abandoned utterances) have been used to draw inferences about processing difficulties associated with the production of language. In children with normal language development (NL), maze frequency in general increases with linguistic complexity, being greater in narra...
Accounts of language development vary in whether they view lexical and grammatical development as being mediated by a single or by separate mechanisms. In a single mechanism account, only one system is required for learning words and extracting grammatical regularity based on similarities among stored items. A strong non-linear relationship between...
Low verb diversity and heavy reliance on a small set of high-frequency 'general all purpose (GAP)' verbs have been reported to characterize specific language impairment (SLI) in preschool children. However, discrepancies exist about the severity of this deficit, particularly in whether these children's verb diversity is commensurate with their MLU...
Adaptations of the widely used MLU measure have been developed in several languages. Such adaptations require numerous modifications, especially in languages that are highly inflected. This study involved the development of a systematic procedure for coding language samples from Icelandic toddlers. Results are reported in terms of mean length of ut...
This study used a single-case alternating treatments design to compare the effectiveness of monolingual and bilingual clinical treatment approaches in teaching English vocabulary to a bilingual child with language impairment. In contrast to the widespread belief that regular exposure to two languages should not be recommended for children with lang...
Citations
... When receptive vocabulary size has been taken into account, some studies have found no significant differences between mono-and bilinguals (Zeng, Kalashnikova & Antoniou, 2019;Friesen, Luo, Luk & Bialystok, 2015), while others have found a bilingual advantage (Friesen, Edwards & Lamoureux, 2021;Pino Escobar, Kalashnikova & Escudero, 2018), when category fluency was assessed in one of the bilingual's languages. In contrast, when receptive vocabulary size was not controlled for, a bilingual disadvantage in semantic fluency in the majority language has been reported in children in Grade 3 (mean age 8.9 years), but not in Grade 1 (mean age 6.8 years; Brandeker & Thordardottir, 2022). ...
... Phonological level.A few studies suggested that single phonological tasks could identify children with DLD(Coady & Evans, 2008;Thordardottir & Reid, 2022).However, in a recent metaanalysis,Schwob et al. (2021) found that the diagnostic accuracy of the Nonword repetition task was generally near the threshold of DLD identification, and to obtain more robust measures, the phonological task should be combined with other tasks. Similar conclusions were reported byBoerma & Blom (2017) for bilingual children with DLD: the authors suggested to combine the Nonword repetition with narrative tasks. ...
... Therefore, the role of educators are very influential. Teachers can use the methods that fun to make interested students learn to speak English (Thordardottir, 2021;Wimolmas, 2013;Fryer, 2019). Because what is being faced is students with various character, then the teacher factor that's fun enough needed. ...
... Margt bendir til þess að skortur sé á þekkingu og úrraeðum innan íslenskra skóla til að maeta þörfum fjöltyngdra barna á viðunandi hátt, baeði námslega og félagslega (Hermína Gunnþórsdóttir o.fl., 2017;Hulda Karen Daníelsdóttir og Hulda Skogland, 2018;Mennta-og menningarmálaráðuneyti, 2020). Rannsóknir á stöðu barnanna í íslensku sýna að framfarir eru haegar á grunnskólaárunum (Elín Þórðardóttir, 2021;Elín Þórðardóttir og Anna Guðrún Júlíusdóttir, 2013;Sigríður Ólafsdóttir, 2015) og niðurstöður PISA sýna að námsleg staða þeirra er mun slakari en hjá eintyngdum jafnöldrum þeirra og hefur versnað á undanförnum árum (Menntamálastofnun, 2019). Í rannsókn Hermínu Gunnþórsdóttur o.fl. ...
... Esto puede ser debido a la mayor presencia de la primera en el ámbito universitario y por el papel que en su difusión ejerce la ONCE en el caso de la discapacidad visual. El mayor conocimiento del trastorno del espectro autista puede deberse a su mayor presencia en los medios de comunicación y en las películas y series de televisión (Thordardottir et al., 2021). Los trastornos de conducta son los menos conocidos por el profesorado universitario. ...
... We are also fully cognizant of the human resource limitations U.S. schools face. There is a well-documented shortage of bilingual teachers to work with DLLs (National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, 2017) in general and an even bigger shortage of bilingual speechlanguage pathologists (Thordardottir, 2020). The current context makes it challenging to ensure it is feasible for DLLs, even those from commonly spoken home languages (e.g., Spanish), to be assessed bilingually. ...
... The lack of significant differences occurred possibly because there was substantial overlap in the range of vocabulary size at these time points due to individual variabilities. This observation was consistent with Bavin et al. (2018) and Duchesne et al. (2020). ...
... Kremer-Sadlik (2005 suggested that intervention in the Heritage language might also be associated with the positive aspects of home, as parents use the Heritage language to communicate between themselves, with other siblings, family members, and the community (Fillmore 1991). Thordardottir and Topbaş (2019) reported that speech and language therapists recommend parents speak the Heritage language at home, based on the individual needs of the child. Implementing the Heritage language in intervention requires a partnership between professionals and parents. ...
... Thordardottir and Rioux (2019) compared first and third grade monolingual students to age-matched simultaneous and sequential bilinguals and found that both groups of bilingual students had smaller conceptual vocabularies compared to monolingual peers. The input-related factor that accounted for the most variation in vocabulary size was the amount of daily exposure to each language, not the initial age of acquisition (Thordardottir and Rioux 2019). This implies that vocabulary size and growth cannot be properly assessed by only focusing on order or age of acquisition for school-age bilingual children. ...
... Camilleri & Law, 2007;Hemsley et al., 2010;Hoff et al., 2012;Restrepo & Silverman, 2001). Furthermore, even when language assessments in the appropriate language do exist, the normative sample may not represent the specific dialect or culture of the child (Garcia & Desrochers, 1997;Taylor, 1986;Thordardottir, 2006). Consequently, typically developing EAL children assessed with these tools would perform below monolingual norms, and could be incorrectly identified as language impaired (Armon-Lotem et al., 2015, Genesee, Paradis & Crago, 2004. ...