Edward K. Vogel’s research while affiliated with University of Chicago and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (260)


Fig 2. Editing vs No Editing hypotheses at output gating in Exp 1. If participants actively edited their visual working memory during the test phase, they should retain only a single item by that point (upper). In contrast, if no such editing occurred, neural activity during the test phase should continue to reflect the number of items originally stored (lower).
Fig 10. Neural working memory load (CDA) sustaind even as memory-based decision making has been made (P3 and LRP). The sustained CDA differences between set size 2 and 4 disappear shortly after a motor response has been made.
Mechanisms of Output Gating for visual working memory
  • Preprint
  • File available

May 2025

·

38 Reads

Chong Zhao

·

Temilade Adekoya

·

Sintra Horwitz

·

[...]

·

Edward K. Vogel

Working memory tasks often require comparing remembered visual arrays to test displays, yet little is known about how people edit the contents of working memory at test. Across three experiments, we used contralateral delay activity (CDA) as a neural index of working memory load to examine how memory representations are selectively accessed at test. In Experiment 1, when a single test item was probed, CDA amplitudes increased with larger set sizes, indicating that untested items were still actively maintained, suggesting minimal editing based on spatial location. To test whether this was due to spatial grouping, Experiment 2 presented memory items sequentially in different temporal frames but identical spatial locations. The continued maintenance of all items at test suggested that simple spatial grouping could not explain the lack of editing effect seen in Experiment 1. In Experiment 3, however, when items belonged to distinct mnemonic categories, CDA amplitudes at test were reduced, consistent with selective editing based on category relevance. These findings suggest that working memory editing during retrieval is guided by categorical structure rather than spatial position. Supporting this, analysis of the P3 old-new effect revealed that decision speed and strength were influenced by the number of items maintained at test. Together, our results show that while people do not edit their working memory load based on spatial cues, they edit their working memory based on categorical relevance, allowing for more efficient retrieval of task-relevant information.

Download

Figure 1: Example task for each WM task. In each task, a stimulus was presented (front), the participant was asked to remember a feature of the stimulus across a WM delay (middle), and then reported the remembered feature value during a response period (back).
Figure 2: Estimation of time-resolved aperiodic and aperiodic-adjusted oscillatory parameters for individual trials. (A) Voltage traces from one trial for two occipital channels (O1, O2), one parietal channel (Pz), and one frontal channel (Fz). The gray bounding box highlights the onesecond temporal window that serves as input for multitaper decomposition to estimate the spectral properties for the time point 0.5 seconds after stimulus presentation. (B) Multitaper spectrogram for one occipital channel (O1) for the same trial as shown in (A). The vertical grey dashed line denotes the time point 0.5 seconds after stimulus presentation. (C) Power spectrum for the time window and channel denoted in (B), which is used to estimate the "instantaneous" spectral parameters. Alpha total power (hashed window) is the full area under the curve (AUC) within the alpha band (8-12 Hz, light purple), while alpha oscillatory power is the alpha total power minus the power attributed to the aperiodic exponent (dark purple). (D) Time-resolved estimates of alpha total power, alpha oscillatory power, and the aperiodic exponent for the same trial as shown in (A) for channel O1. (E) Spatial distribution of alpha total power, alpha oscillatory power, and aperiodic exponent for same time point and trial shown in (A).
Figure 3: Procedure for fitting inverted encoding models to estimate strength of representation for correct spatial location. (A) Training stimulus presented at 45°. (B) Predicted spatial channel response for neural populations encoding each of the eight location bins for the stimulus presented at 45°. The spatial channel response is modeled using Eq. (1). This corresponds to one row of C train . (C) Average EEG activity for each sensor for one example training block. The topomap shown is the average, aperiodic-adjusted alpha oscillatory power at 0.5 seconds for each sensor for one participant. Each row in B train represents alpha oscillatory power at 0.5 seconds across each sensor for each trial. (D) The estimated channel weights fit from the predicted channel responses and alpha oscillatory power using Eq. (3). The weights corresponding to the training stimulus shown at 45° are highlighted for each sensor. (E) Test stimulus presented at 225°. (F) Inverted channel weights as shown in Eq. (4). The weights corresponding to the test stimulus shown at 225° are highlighted for each sensor. (G) Average EEG activity for each sensor for one example test block. The topomap shown is the average alpha oscillatory power at 0.5 seconds for each sensor for one participant. Each row in B test represents alpha oscillatory power at 0.5 seconds across each sensor for each trial. (H) Estimated spatial channel response for neural populations encoding each of the eight location bins for a stimulus presented at 225°, as calculated by Eq. (4). (I) Channel tuning function (CTF) slope, calculated after the spatial estimated channel response shown in (H) is circularly shifted to the correct spatial location and reflected about that spatial location. The dotted line shows the slope of the linear regression of this CTF for stimuli presented at 225° for this test block.
Figure 5: Comparison of representation strength time courses by periodic and aperiodic parameters. (A) Representation strength during encoding and delay periods for each participant by alpha total power, (B) alpha oscillatory power, and (C) aperiodic exponent. Significance above each cluster reflects the deviation from baseline for each time period, while the significance between clusters reflects the paired difference between the encoding and delay time periods across participants. (D) Comparison of representation strength by alpha oscillatory power and alpha total power during the delay period for each participant. Values above the diagonal indicate stronger representation of the correct spatial location by alpha oscillatory power than alpha total power. The significance of the paired t-test for each participant's representation strength of the correct spatial location by alpha oscillatory power versus that by alpha total power across each task is shown in the legend.
Differential representations of spatial location by aperiodic and alpha oscillatory activity in working memory

March 2025

·

101 Reads

Decades of research have shown working memory (WM) relies on sustained pre-frontal cortical activity and visual extrastriate activity, particularly in the alpha (8-12 Hz) frequency range. This alpha activity tracks the spatial location of WM items, even when spatial position is task-irrelevant and there is no stimulus currently being presented. Traditional analyses of putative oscillations using bandpass filters, however, conflate oscillations with non-oscillatory aperiodic activity. Here, we reanalyzed seven different human electroencephalography (EEG) visual WM datasets to test the hypothesis that aperiodic activity–which is thought to reflect the relative contributions of excitatory and inhibitory drive–plays a distinct role in visual WM from true alpha oscillations. To do this, we developed a novel, time-resolved spectral parameterization approach to disentangle oscillations from aperiodic activity during WM encoding and maintenance. Across all seven tasks, totaling 112 participants, we captured the representation of spatial location from total alpha power using an inverted encoding model (IEM), replicating traditional analyses. We then trained separate IEMs to estimate the strength of spatial location representation from aperiodic-adjusted alpha (reflecting just the oscillatory component) and aperiodic activity, and find that IEM performance improves for aperiodic-adjusted alpha compared to total alpha power that blends the two signals. We also discover a novel role for aperiodic activity, where IEM performance trained on aperiodic activity is highest during stimulus presentation, but not during the WM maintenance period. Our results emphasize the importance of controlling for aperiodic activity when studying neural oscillations while uncovering a novel functional role for aperiodic activity in the encoding of visual WM information. Significance statement Working memory is a crucial component of cognition, yet its neural mechanisms are not fully understood. Research shows that alpha activity – presumed to reflect neural oscillations – tracks the location of items we hold in memory. However, these analyses assume that all alpha power is oscillatory, even though oscillations are mixed with non-oscillatory, aperiodic activity that may be physiologically and functionally distinct. Here, we use a novel analytical approach for separating alpha oscillations and aperiodic activity dynamically across time. Our results reveal distinct roles for each in human visual working memory: aperiodic activity encodes the spatial location of information whereas alpha oscillations maintain the location of that information.


Lapses of sustained attention occur when goals compete: Evidences from the switch CPT

March 2025

·

50 Reads

Sustained attention is the ability to maintain focus on a specific goal over time, but lapses in attention are frequent. Many theories have attributed these lapses to a transient failure of control in maintaining the goal in mind. However, these proposals have been challenged because recent findings have shown more engagement of cognitive control during states more prone to lapses. We hypothesized that lapses occur during periods of high competition between goals, requiring stronger cognitive control. To test this goal-competition hypothesis, we developed a Switch-Continuous Performance Task in which subjects alternated task goals between blocks-either switching or holding the same goal-in an effort to manipulate periods of higher and lower competition between goals. Participants viewed a bilateral display showing a scene (indoor/outdoor) and a face (male/female) on each trial. At the start of each 20-trial block, a cue instructed participants to perform either the scene task (e.g., press for frequent indoor scenes, not infrequent outdoor scenes) or the face task (e.g., press for frequent male faces, not infrequent female faces). Results showed more attention lapses during switch than repeat blocks, suggesting that lapses occur during periods of high competition between goals. In a second study, we monetarily rewarded performance on only one task (scene or face) to create unequal competition between goals. We found that switching to an unrewarded goal-but not a rewarded goal-produced more lapses. Together, these findings support the goal-competition hypothesis as an explanation for the occurrence of sustained attention lapses.


Lapses of sustained attention occur when goals compete: Evidences from the switch CPT

March 2025

·

4 Reads

·

1 Citation

Sustained attention is the ability to maintain focus on a specific goal over time,but lapses in attention are frequent. Many theories have attributed these lapses to atransient failure of control in maintaining the goal in mind. However, these proposalshave been challenged because recent findings have shown more engagement ofcognitive control during states more prone to lapses. We hypothesized that lapses occurduring periods of high competition between goals, requiring stronger cognitive control.To test this goal-competition hypothesis, we developed a Switch-ContinuousPerformance Task in which subjects alternated task goals between blocks—eitherswitching or holding the same goal—in an effort to manipulate periods of higher andlower competition between goals. Participants viewed a bilateral display showing ascene (indoor/outdoor) and a face (male/female) on each trial. At the start of each 20-trial block, a cue instructed participants to perform either the scene task (e.g., press forfrequent indoor scenes, not infrequent outdoor scenes) or the face task (e.g., press forfrequent male faces, not infrequent female faces). Results showed more attention lapsesduring switch than repeat blocks, suggesting that lapses occur during periods of highcompetition between goals. In a second study, we monetarily rewarded performance ononly one task (scene or face) to create unequal competition between goals. We foundthat switching to an unrewarded goal—but not a rewarded goal—produced more lapses.Together, these findings support the goal-competition hypothesis as an explanation forthe occurrence of sustained attention lapses.


Sustained attention is more closely related to long-term memory than to attentional control

March 2025

·

55 Reads

Individuals differ in their ability to sustain attention. However, whether differences in sustained attention reflect differences in processes related to attentional control and working memory or long-term memory (LTM) remains underexplored. In Experiment 1, we conducted an online study (n = 136) measuring participants' sustained attention, attention control and working memory, and LTM. We measured sustained attention with an audio-visual continuous performance task (avCPT) in which participants responded to images while inhibiting responses to infrequent targets; attention control and working memory with Flanker, change localization, and Simon tasks; and LTM with recognition and source memory tests. Factor analyses revealed that sustained attention formed a distinct factor from attention control and working memory and LTM. Individual differences in the sustained attention factor robustly predicted individual differences in LTM and, to a lesser extent, attention control and working memory. In Experiment 2, to test how neural signatures of sustained attention related to attention control and working memory and LTM, we analyzed fMRI functional connectivity patterns collected as 20 participants performed the avCPT. A pre-trained connectome-based model of sustained attention predicted participants' performance on out-of-scanner LTM, but not attention control and working memory, tasks. Together these results suggest that individual differences in sustained attention, although correlated with attention control and working memory, are more closely related to LTM.


Objective markers of sustained attention fluctuate independently of mind-wandering reports

February 2025

·

30 Reads

·

1 Citation

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

Sustained attention fluctuates between periods of good and poor attentional performance. Two major methodologies exist to study these fluctuations: an objective approach that identifies “in-the-zone” states of consistent response times (RTs) and “out-of-the-zone” states of erratic RTs and a subjective approach that asks participants whether they are on-task or mind wandering. Although both approaches effectively predict attentional lapses, it remains unclear whether they capture the same or distinct attentional fluctuations. We combined both approaches within a single sustained attention task requiring frequent responses and response inhibition to rare targets to explore their consistency (N = 40). Behaviorally, both objective out-of-the-zone and subjective mind-wandering states were associated with more attentional lapses. However, the percentage of time spent out-of-the-zone did not differ between on-task and mind-wandering periods and both objective and subjective states independently predicted error-proneness, suggesting that the two methods do not capture the same type of attention fluctuations. Whereas attentional preparation before correct inhibitions was greater during out-of-the-zone compared with in-the-zone periods, preparation did not differ by subjective state. In contrast, posterror slowing differed by both objective and subjective states, but in opposite directions: slowing was observed when participants were objectively out-of-the-zone or subjectively on-task. Overall, our results provide evidence that objective and subjective approaches capture distinct attention fluctuations during sustained attention tasks. Integrating both objective and subjective measures is crucial for fully understanding the mechanisms underlying our ability to remain focused.


Individual Differences in Working Memory and Attentional Control Continue to Predict Memory Performance Despite Extensive Learning

January 2025

·

41 Reads

·

1 Citation

Individual differences in working memory predict a wide range of cognitive abilities. However, little research has been done on whether working memory continues to predict task performance after repetitive learning. Here, we tested whether working memory ability continued to predict long-term memory (LTM) performance for picture sequences even after participants showed massive learning. In Experiments 1–3, subjects performed a source memory task in which they were presented a sequence of 30 objects shown in one of four quadrants and then were tested on each item’s position. We repeated this procedure for five times in Experiment 1 and 12 times in Experiments 2 and 3. Interestingly, we discovered that individual differences in working memory continually predicted LTM accuracy across all repetitions. In Experiment 4, we replicated the stable working memory demands with word pairs. In Experiment 5, we generalized the stable working memory demands model to attentional control abilities. Together, these results suggest that people, instead of relying less on working memory, optimized their working memory and attentional control throughout learning.





Citations (51)


... For example, the occurrence of an error after a no-go trial is typically followed by an increased frontal midline theta power (Chidharom, Krieg, Pham, et al., 2021;van Driel et al., 2015). Such frontal power increase is an indicator of performance monitoring and predicts subsequent enhancements in post-error reaction-time slowing (Cavanagh et al., 2009), an adaptation mechanism to maintain performance on task and avoid future lapses (Cavanagh et al., 2009;Chidharom et al., 2025). ...

Reference:

Lapses of sustained attention occur when goals compete: Evidences from the switch CPT
Objective markers of sustained attention fluctuate independently of mind-wandering reports
  • Citing Article
  • February 2025

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

... For instance, some studies have shown that retrieval becomes more efficient when the recall context matches the encoding context (Tulving and Thomson 1973;Özdemir et al. 2024), and other studies have found that changes in the context of memorized items in episodic LTM evoke larger neural responses during WM tasks for the same items (Reinhart et al. 2016;Özdemir et al. 2024;Şentürk et al. 2024). Together, the shared context-dependency of activity-silent WM and episodic LTM provides a novel perspective for bridging these two memory systems and supports a recent view arguing that activity-silent WM can be more parsimoniously explained by the involvement of episodic LTM (Beukers et al. 2021;Foster et al. 2024). ...

Working Memory as Persistent Neural Activity
  • Citing Chapter
  • July 2024

... Few studies have investigated "natural" forgetting of no longer relevant information. Tsubomi et al. (2024) investigated natural VWM item removal and found that neural signals and explicit measures of VWM contents degrade within the space of 1,000 ms after task completion or when the information was cued to be irrelevant (see also Hamblin-Frohman & Pratt, 2025). In the current design, we observe a longer lasting influence of the no longer relevant information, however, here we use implicit behavioral measures (e.g., Harrison et al., 2021) that may show influence where neural and explicit (e.g., H. Chen & Wyble, 2015) fail to register. ...

Task Termination Triggers Spontaneous Removal of Information From Visual Working Memory
  • Citing Article
  • June 2024

Psychological Science

... Early work has long-posited that "chunking" and prior knowledge are important means to get more mileage out of capacity-limited WM (e.g., it is much easier to hold in mind "DOG-CAT-TURTLE" than "XKY-QJC-PLZWDU"; Bower, 1972;Ebbinghaus, 1885Ebbinghaus, , 1913. New work is shedding light on how familiarity and meaningfulness of stimuli boost WM performance (Brady & Störmer, 2022;Asp, Störmer, & Brady, 2021;Xie & Zhang, 2017), how chunks are acquired and "built" via learning (Adam, Zhao, & Vogel, 2024;Soni & Frank, 2024;Zhong, Katkov, & Tsodyks, 2024;Musfeld, Souza, & Oberauer, 2023;Ngiam, Brissenden, & Awh, 2019;Brady, Konkle, & Alvarez, 2009), and how neural representations transform with long-term learning Miller & Constantinidis, 2024;Miller et al., 2022;Starr, Srinivasan, & Bunge, 2020). However, although it is increasingly recognized that WM interfaces with LTM, the boundaries of that interaction and parallels in their functioning are in question (Hirschstein & Aly, 2023;Cotton & Ricker, 2022;Beukers, Buschman, Cohen, & Norman, 2021;Forsberg, Guitard, & Cowan, 2021). ...

Behavioral signatures of the rapid recruitment of long-term memory to overcome working memory capacity limits

Memory & Cognition

... A power analysis was conducted using InteractionPoweR (Baranger et al., 2023) to determine the minimum sample size required to test the study hypothesis (i.e., Aptitude × Treatment interaction effect). We ran 1,000 simulations for each of the power estimation hyperparameters and assumed that our working memory measures and LTM measures were both reliable (reliability of 0.8 and 0.9, respectively, according to Zhao & Vogel, 2024). With a small effect (interaction r = 0.2), to achieve 80% power with α = .05, ...

Working Memory and Attentional Control Abilities Predict Individual Differences in Visual Long-Term Memory Tasks
  • Citing Preprint
  • April 2024

... Specifically, test data were fit to the inverted weights matrix to reconstruct channel-tuning functions for each location separately. [69] indicate that the spatial selectivity of saccade preparation may not be limited to alpha-band activity. Therefore, we here instead focused on analyzing voltage patterns across the scalp [46,47] as voltage measures have popularly been employed in studying presaccadic processes [57,[98][99][100][101][102]. ...

Encoded and updated spatial working memories share a common representational format in alpha activity

iScience

... Rather, depending on its size, memory sets might be learned and retrieved within smaller subsets, thereby allowing to learn repeated patterns in a more flexible way. This idea is further supported by reaction time patterns observed in visual repetition learning experiments, which suggest that, depending on the size of a learned visual array, information is retrieved from long-term memory within smaller subsets (Adam et al., 2023). ...

Behavioral signatures of the rapid recruitment of long-term memory to overcome working memory capacity limits

... This would complement previous work that has suggested that sequential and simultaneous presentations rely on similar WM processes. For example, Woodman and colleagues showed that sequential and simultaneous presentation resulted in similar WM accuracy across different set sizes (Woodman, Vogel, and Luck, 2012), while Zhao and Vogel showed that performance is highly correlated in sequential and simultaneous WM tasks across individuals (Zhao and Vogel, 2023). Finally, similar to work done by Waschke and colleagues (Waschke, Wöstmann, and Obleser, 2017), researchers could experimentally manipulate the stimuli to bias aperiodic activity in the visual cortex and examine how such biasing affects WM encoding. ...

Sequential encoding paradigm reliably captures the individual differences from a simultaneous visual working memory task
  • Citing Article
  • January 2023

Attention Perception & Psychophysics

... Moreover, some studies suggest WM can be unconscious (Barton et al., 2022;Soto et al., 2011;Velichkovsky, 2017). Thus, a more tangible definition of WM could be neurally active representations instead of conscious storage of task-relevant information over brief intervals (Awh and Vogel, 2020;Kamiński et al., 2017;Scotti et al., 2021;Vogel and Machizawa, 2004). Moreover, the concept of cortical reinstatement, originally associated with WM tasks, has been observed during episodic memory retrieval and at event boundaries. ...

Online and Off-Line Memory States in the Human Brain
  • Citing Chapter
  • May 2020