Donna L. Maney’s research while affiliated with Emory University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (106)


Sex/gender diversity and behavioral neuroendocrinology in the 21st century
  • Article

May 2024

·

22 Reads

·

2 Citations

Hormones and Behavior

Kathleen V Casto

·

Donna L Maney

Training in the implementation of sex and gender research policies: an evaluation of publicly available online courses
  • Literature Review
  • Full-text available

April 2024

·

32 Reads

·

2 Citations

Biology of Sex Differences

Background Recently implemented research policies requiring the inclusion of females and males have created an urgent need for effective training in how to account for sex, and in some cases gender, in biomedical studies. Methods Here, we evaluated three sets of publicly available online training materials on this topic: (1) Integrating Sex & Gender in Health Research from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR); (2) Sex as a Biological Variable: A Primer from the United States National Institutes of Health (NIH); and (3) The Sex and Gender Dimension in Biomedical Research , developed as part of “Leading Innovative measures to reach gender Balance in Research Activities” (LIBRA) from the European Commission. We reviewed each course with respect to their coverage of (1) What is required by the policy; (2) Rationale for the policy; (3) Handling of the concepts “sex” and “gender;” (4) Research design and analysis; and (5) Interpreting and reporting data. Results All three courses discussed the importance of including males and females to better generalize results, discover potential sex differences, and tailor treatments to men and women. The entangled nature of sex and gender, operationalization of sex, and potential downsides of focusing on sex more than other sources of variation were minimally discussed. Notably, all three courses explicitly endorsed invalid analytical approaches that produce bias toward false positive discoveries of difference. Conclusions Our analysis suggests a need for revised or new training materials that incorporate four major topics: precise operationalization of sex, potential risks of over-emphasis on sex as a category, recognition of gender and sex as complex and entangled, and rigorous study design and data analysis.

Download


Best practices to promote rigor and reproducibility in the era of sex-inclusive research

November 2023

·

41 Reads

·

12 Citations

eLife

To enhance inclusivity and rigor, many funding agencies and journals now mandate the inclusion of females as well as males in biomedical studies. These mandates have enhanced generalizability and created unprecedented opportunities to discover sex differences. However, education in sound methods to consider sex as a subgroup category has lagged behind, resulting in a problematic literature in which study designs, analyses, and interpretations of results are often flawed. Here, we outline best practices for complying with sex-inclusive mandates, both for studies in which sex differences are a primary focus and for those in which they are not. Our recommendations are organized within the “4 Cs of Studying Sex to Strengthen Science: Consideration, Collection, Characterization and Communication,” a framework developed by the Office of Research on Women’s Health at the National Institutes of Health in the United States. Following these guidelines should help researchers include females and males in their studies while at the same time upholding high standards of rigor.


Figure 1. Gender prevalence ratios for common neuropsychiatric disorders. Modified from Eliot et al. (2021). Sources: autism (Loomes et al., 2017); ADHD (Polanczyk et al., 2007); alcohol use disorder (Grant et al., 2004); dyslexia (Rutter et al., 2004); dementia (Buckley et al., 2019); anxiety disorders (McLean et al., 2011); depression (Salk et al., 2017); and eating disorders (Hudson et al., 2007).
Why and How to Account for Sex and Gender in Brain and Behavioral Research

September 2023

·

533 Reads

·

26 Citations

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

Long overlooked in neuroscience research, sex and gender are increasingly included as key variables potentially impacting all levels of neurobehavioral analysis. Still, many neuroscientists do not understand the difference between the terms “sex” and “gender,” the complexity and nuance of each, or how to best include them as variables in research designs. This TechSights article outlines rationales for considering the influence of sex and gender across taxa, and provides technical guidance for strengthening the rigor and reproducibility of such analyses. This guidance includes the use of appropriate statistical methods for comparing groups as well as controls for key covariates of sex (e.g., total intracranial volume) and gender (e.g., income, caregiver stress, bias). We also recommend approaches for interpreting and communicating sex- and gender-related findings about the brain, which have often been misconstrued by neuroscientists and the lay public alike.


Considering Sex as a Variable at a Research University: Knowledge, Attitudes, and Practices

August 2023

·

25 Reads

·

6 Citations

Journal of Women's Health

Biomedical research has a history of excluding females as research subjects, which threatens rigor, reproducibility, and inclusivity. In 2016, to redress this bias, the U.S. National Institutes of Health (NIH) implemented a policy requiring the consideration of sex as a biological variable (SABV) in all studies involving vertebrate animals, including humans. Unless strongly justified, females and males must be included in all studies and results reported disaggregated by sex. Recent evidence indicates, however, that misunderstandings of the policy and other significant barriers impede its implementation. To shed light on those barriers at our home institution, we conducted a study funded by the Emory University Specialized Center of Research Excellence on Sex Differences (SCORE). In semistructured interviews of Emory principal investigators in the biological sciences, we noted their knowledge of what the policy entails and why it was implemented, their attitudes toward it, and the extent to which it has or has not changed their research practices. Although attitudes toward SABV were generally positive, most researchers face challenges with respect to its implementation. We suggest interventions that can be mounted at the level of home institutions, such as raising awareness of locally available core facilities, to help address these challenges. More training is needed on what the policy asks of researchers, how sex is defined, the nonhormonal ways that sex differences can manifest, and best practices for statistical analysis of sex-based data. Home institutions may also want to explore ways to lessen the stress associated with rollout of SABV policy.


Experimental timeline and developmental trajectory of song learning in male zebra finches. Soon after fledging at ~ 18 days post-hatch (dph), male zebra finches enter an auditory phase of song learning during which they memorize adult song³² and start practicing singing³³. Singing rates increase dramatically around age 50 dph. At approximately the same time, juveniles enter the “plastic song” phase34,35, during which some memorization may still occur but ends by 65 dph³². Song crystallization, during which the song takes its final form, begins by 77 dph and finishes by 90 dph33,34. In this study, male zebra finch “pupils” were reared in sound-attenuating chambers. Fathers were removed at 4 dph, such that pupils were thereafter isolated from male song until tutoring sessions commenced at 27 dph. During tutoring sessions, pupils were treated with either an oxytocin antagonist (OTA) during exposure to a tutor singing “OTA song” or treated with saline (control) during exposure to a tutor singing “control song” (see “Methods”). Treatments/tutors were alternated in counterbalanced order. At 35 dph, when they were nutritionally independent³³, the pupils were transferred to an operant chamber equipped with keys that played tutor songs according to a reinforcement schedule that allowed us to measure preference while balancing exposure to the two songs (Fig. 3; see also ref. 8). Song preference, namely preference for control song over OTA song, was measured daily while the pupils remained in the operant chamber, until 99 dph. We recorded the songs of the pupils at 101–103 dph and compared them to the control and OTA songs. Illustrations of finches by DataBase Center for Life Science, shared under CC BY 4.0, https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0.
Effects of oxytocin antagonist (OTA) on behavior during tutoring. Approach-related pupil behaviors are shown in panels (A) and (B); attention-related pupil behaviors are in panels (C) through (E), and panel (F) shows a tutor behavior. There was no effect of OTA on the time spent close to the tutor (A). Regardless of treatment, pupils tended to spend most of their time in a “tutor zone” within 12 cm of the side of their cage closest to the tutor. OTA significantly decreased the number of pecks to tutor (B), or the number of times the pupil touched its beak to the wall of its own cage closest to the tutor (no pecks occurred on other walls in this study). There was no effect of OTA on bouts of flying, operationalized as two-foot contacts on cage walls (C), or on pupil vocalizations (D). OTA decreased the percentage of time pupils spent preening (E), which may be related to attention³⁷. Tutors sang at significantly lower rates when presented with OTA-treated pupils than when presented with control-treated pupils (F); however, the number of live tutor songs did not predict the pupils’ preferences or learning (see “Results”). In (A–E) n = 9 pupils; in F, n = 8 tutors. Colors represent individual pupils as per the key on the right. All panels show mean scores (in black) for saline and OTA trials. To accurately depict the within-subjects variation in the error bars, the between-subjects variation was removed using the summarySEwithin function in R (v4.04) which implements the method described by Cousineau³⁸ and the correction factor described by Morey³⁹. The plotted standard error of the mean (SEM) is therefore equal between conditions for each score. *p < 0.05. See Supplementary Tables S1and S2 for statistics. Data are presented in Supplementary Table S4.
Key-pressing set-up and reinforcement schedule. (A) The operant chamber consisted of a 36 × 36 × 40 cm cage, inside which two keys were placed on opposite walls. One key was associated with playback of control song and the other with playback of OTA song. Outside the cage, one speaker assigned to each key played the songs. (B) demonstrates the reinforcement schedule. Keys I and II are associated with a higher probability of playing song I and song II, respectively⁸. Here we present an extreme example to illustrate how the schedule is able to balance exposure despite a strong preference. In this scenario, the bird prefers song I and presses key I only. The probability of playing song I by pressing that key is high at the beginning of the session, to help the bird form the association between that key and the song. As the bird keeps pressing key I, the probability decreases step-wise from 1 to 0.5, to prevent song II from lagging far behind in the playback count. This decrease, however, is not enough to balance exposure, and therefore, if the bird switches keys, key II plays only song II until the playback count of song II is balanced with song I. After enough presses on key I, song I eventually reaches a quota of 30 playbacks and ceases to be played. Afterwards, only song II is played, until that song also reaches the quota. Importantly, there is always a large difference between the keys with respect to the probability of hearing the preferred song. When the key associated with preferred song is playing that song only 50% of the time, the other key plays non-preferred song 100% of the time. The code used in this study, as well as an updated version, is available from Rodriguez-Saltos et al.⁸. Photo of finch “Australian zebra finch” by Lip Kee Yap, shared under CC BY-SA 2.0, https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/2.0. Background in the photo has been removed.
Effects of oxytocin receptor antagonist (OTA) during tutoring on song preference and learning. (A) Generalized additive model showing the trajectory of song preferences over the entire period of vocal development. The white line shows the average trajectory of the preference for the song of the tutor in the control condition, or ‘control song’. The black area indicates the 95% confidence interval (CI). Concentric blue lines represent the 2D density kernel estimation of the daily preference scores. Bandwidth was estimated via Normal Reference Distribution8,41,42. The horizontal dashed line at 0.5 indicates chance; values above the line indicate preference for control song and values below indicate preference for OTA song. Vertical dashed lines indicate boundaries between developmental phases (see Fig. 1). On average, preference for control song was significant during the auditory learning phase (CI does not include 0.5). Data are presented in Supplementary Tables S5 and S6. (B) Similarity between pupil song and tutor song was higher in the control condition than the OTA condition (maximum similarity is shown; for average similarity, see Supplementary Table S3). See caption of Fig. 2 for how the error bars were calculated. The effect size (Cohen’s dav) was large, at 1.15. See Supplementary Table S3 for statistics and Supplementary Table S7 for data. Colors represent individual pupils; see Fig. 2C The preference for control song over OTA song (averaged over 38–99 dph; plotted on the X-axis) significantly predicted the degree to which birds learned control song better than OTA song (Y-axis). Colors refer to individual pupils as per Figs. 2 and 4B. See Supplementary Table S8 for the values used to make the graph.
Oxytocin receptor antagonism during early vocal learning reduces song preference and imitation in zebra finches

May 2023

·

117 Reads

·

2 Citations

In species with vocal learning, acquiring species-typical vocalizations relies on early social orienting. In songbirds, for example, learning song requires dynamic social interactions with a “tutor” during an early sensitive period. Here, we hypothesized that the attentional and motivational processes that support song learning recruit the oxytocin system, which is well-understood to play a role in social orienting in other species. Juvenile male zebra finches naïve to song were each tutored by two unfamiliar adult males. Before exposure to one tutor, juveniles were injected subcutaneously with oxytocin receptor antagonist (OTA; ornithine vasotocin) and before exposure to the other, saline (control). Treatment with OTA reduced behaviors associated with approach and attention during tutoring sessions. Using a novel operant paradigm to measure preference while balancing exposure to the two tutor songs, we showed that the juveniles preferred to hear the song of the control tutor. Their adult songs more closely resembled the control tutor’s song, and the magnitude of this difference was predicted by early preference for control over OTA song. Overall, oxytocin antagonism during exposure to a tutor seemed to bias juveniles against that tutor and his song. Our results suggest that oxytocin receptors are important for socially-guided vocal learning.




Experimental set-up. (A) Experimental timeline (top) and developmental trajectory of song learning in male zebra finches. Zebra finches were reared by their parents in a room where they could also listen to, but not see, adult male zebra finches other than the father (“neighbors”). At 35–37 dph, when finches are nutritionally independent³⁷, the juveniles were transferred to an operant chamber equipped with keys that were associated with playback of father’s or neighbor’s song. Preference for father’s or neighbor’s song was measured daily while the bird remained in the operant chamber, until 90 dph. This timeline of operant conditioning covers most of the developmental trajectory of song learning. At the beginning of our assay, at 35–37 dph, zebra finches are still in an auditory phase of learning, during which they are known to memorize adult song⁶⁶ and start practicing singing³⁷. Singing rates start increasing by age 50 dph. Approximately at the same time, the finches enter the “plastic song” phase26,27, during which some memorization may still occur but ends by 65 dph⁶⁶. Finally, song crystallization begins by 77 dph and finishes by 90 dph26,37. In the crystallization phase, between 80 and 90 dph, we recorded vocalizations of the juveniles and compared them to father’s and to neighbor’s song. (B) An adult zebra finch presses a key in an operant chamber to elicit playback of conspecific song. The same setup was used with the juveniles in our experiment. Photo by CAR-S. (C) The operant chamber consisted of a 14 × 15 × 17 inch cage, inside which two keys were placed on opposite walls. One key was associated with playback of father’s song and the other with playback of neighbor’s song. Outside the cage, one speaker assigned to each key played the songs. Photo of finch by Lip Kee Yap, shared under the Creative Commons Attribution-Share Alike 2.0 Generic license.
Reinforcement schedule to detect song preference while balancing exposure. A bird is presented with keys I and II, which are associated with a higher probability of playing song I and song II, respectively. Here we present an extreme example to illustrate how the schedule is able to balance exposure despite a strong preference. In this scenario, the bird prefers song I and presses key I only. The probability of playing song I by pressing that key is high at the beginning of the session, to help the bird form the association between that key and the song. As the bird keeps pressing key I, the probability decreases step-wise from 1 to 0.5, to prevent song II from lagging far behind in the playback count. This decrease, however, is not enough to balance exposure, and therefore, if the bird switches keys, key II plays only song II until the playback count of song II is balanced with song I. After enough presses on key I, song I eventually reaches a quota of 30 playbacks and ceases to be played. Afterwards, only song II is played, until that song also reaches the quota. Importantly, until the quota of the preferred song is exhausted, there is always a large difference between the keys with respect to the probability of hearing the preferred song. When the key associated with preferred song is playing that song only 50% of the time, the other key plays non-preferred song 100% of the time. See the Supplemental Methods for details about the reinforcement schedule. Key presses occurring after the quota of the preferred song was exhausted each day were not used in the analysis.
Relationship between the preference for father’s song and imitation of that song. (A) The white line shows the average trajectory of the preference for father’s song. The black area indicates the 95% confidence interval. The horizontal dashed line indicates no preference for either song. Vertical dashed lines indicate boundaries between developmental phases (see Fig. 1A). Concentric blue lines represent the 2D density kernel estimation of the daily preference scores. Ticks near the bottom of the plot indicate the age at which individual birds reached their peak preference for father’s song prior to the crystallization phase (for individual trajectories, see Fig. S2). Given that crystallization may begin as early as 70 dph²⁶, we chose the maximum preference reached before that age (see Fig. S5 for an alternative analysis using the peak preference over the entire learning trajectory, including crystallization). Ticks are drawn with an offset to prevent overlap. Preferences peaking during the phase known as auditory learning (see Fig. 1) are marked in orange, and those peaking during the plastic song phase are marked in teal. (B) Peak preference for father’s song was significantly correlated with mean similarity to that song. The curve is the fit of a beta regression. Vertical bars show the standard error of the mean similarity score. Data points are colored according to the phase during which the bird reached maximum preference for father’s song, as indicated in (A). (C) Spectrograms of the songs of three pupil exemplars are shown next to the spectrograms of the songs of their respective father and neighbor. The spectrograms indicate that these three pupils imitated father song and not neighbor song. All other pupils also imitated father song or, in the case of one individual, did so most of the time (see text). Spectrograms were generated in R⁷³ using Hanning windows containing 512 samples each (11.61 ms for recordings sampled at 44,100 kHz), with 50% overlap between windows.
Song preferences predict the quality of vocal learning in zebra finches

January 2023

·

68 Reads

·

6 Citations

In songbirds, learning to sing is a highly social process that likely involves social reward. Here, we tested the hypothesis that during song learning, the reward value of hearing a particular song predicts the degree to which that song will ultimately be learned. We measured the early song preferences of young male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) in an operant key-pressing assay; each of two keys was associated with a higher likelihood of playing the song of the father or that of another familiar adult (“neighbor”). To minimize the effects of exposure on learning, we implemented a novel reinforcement schedule that allowed us to detect preferences while balancing exposure to each song. On average, the juveniles significantly preferred the father’s song early during song learning, before actual singing occurs in this species. When they reached adulthood, all the birds copied the father’s song. The accuracy with which the father’s song was imitated was positively correlated with the peak strength of the preference for the father’s song during the sensitive period of song learning. Our results show that preference for the song of a chosen tutor, in this case the father, predicted vocal learning during development.


Citations (84)


... Animal models, particularly mouse models, are widely used to investigate the role of biological sex in both health and disease. The recent emphasis on understanding how biological sex influences physiology has led to increased inclusion of both male and female mice in preclinical studies, and the inclusion of sex as a biological variable in preclinical and clinical research is now expected by major biomedical research funding agencies in the United States, Canada, and the European Union [1]. ...

Reference:

The Four Core Genotypes mouse model: evaluating the impact of a recently discovered translocation
Training in the implementation of sex and gender research policies: an evaluation of publicly available online courses

Biology of Sex Differences

... Another area of controversy relates to whether sex and gender should be examined categorically by comparing outcomes between females versus males or by focusing on specific sex-and gender-related variables, such as hormone levels, chromosomes, or alignment with gender norms (12,13). This debate has arisen in part from the valid desire to ensure that already marginalized sex and gender groups such as people with innate variations in sex characteristics, trans, and non-binary, do not get left behind as we improve the representation of cisgender women in research. ...

Sex contextualism in laboratory research: Enhancing rigor and precision in the study of sex-related variables
  • Citing Article
  • March 2024

Cell

... Additionally, captive data suggest that relatively low concentrations of circulating testosterone are generally adequate to maintain male reproductive function 91 , while elevated testosterone levels imply costs 92 , including increased metabolic rates and immunosuppression 93 , which would prove unnecessary in captive settings. ...

Toward an Ecological Basis of Hormone-Behavior Interactions in Reproduction of Birds
  • Citing Chapter
  • October 1999

... Women from ethnic minority groups have disproportionately borne the brunt of the inadequacies within the UK's technological birthing system, which is frequently underfunded and understaffed [32,33]. The technocratic approach, which emphasises clinical tasks and safety measures over patientcentred care, has been linked to adverse psychological and social outcomes [32,34]. Recognising the particularly severe impact of this approach on ethnic minority women as evidenced by MBRRACE mortality and morbidity statistics-the Continuity of Midwifery Care (CMC) policy was developed [18]. ...

Best practices to promote rigor and reproducibility in the era of sex-inclusive research

eLife

... The influence of changing hormone levels on cognition and affect throughout the female reproductive lifecycle has often been either ignored or perceived as an unwanted source of variance, even though hormones have significant neurobiological effects 88 . This has led to biased recruitment, and a lack of understanding of how hormonal fluctuations can influence affective symptoms, such as in the premenstrual dysphoric syndrome [89][90][91] . Computational modelling of the dynamic interplay between affect, symptoms and hormones in females can therefore not only importantly inform future treatment development studies but can also serve as an excellent use-case for developing tools that are sensitive enough to capture subtle changes in affect over long time periods. ...

Why and How to Account for Sex and Gender in Brain and Behavioral Research

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

... Further, even when sex is included as a factor, appropriate analytical approaches are infrequently employed [10]. Studies of policy implementation have identified gaps in researcher knowledge of the policies and how to implement them [11,12], for example whether sample sizes reviewed each course with respect to their coverage of (1) What is required by the policy; (2) Rationale for the policy; (3) Handling of the concepts "sex" and "gender;" (4) Research design and analysis; and (5) Interpreting and reporting data. All three discussed the importance of including males and females to better generalize results, discover potential sex differences, and tailor treatments to men and women. ...

Considering Sex as a Variable at a Research University: Knowledge, Attitudes, and Practices
  • Citing Article
  • August 2023

Journal of Women's Health

... In the short time it has been published, our methodology and proposal for a universal vertebrate gene nomenclature for the OT-VT ligand and receptor families has been adopted in a variety of studies across vertebrate lineages [52][53][54][55] and it has influenced the NCBI and ENSEMBL annotation groups in using synteny as the primary evidence when it comes to annotate newly sequenced genomes, such as those that I contributed to in the context of the Vertebrate Genomes Project [56]. Further, our methods for synteny analyses have worked as the foundation of a subsequent project I co-coordinated on the evolution of the OT pathway genes in both vertebrates and invertebrates [57]. ...

Oxytocin receptor antagonism during early vocal learning reduces song preference and imitation in zebra finches

... LIBRA stated, for example, "Study outcome measures, that is the effects of treatment, separately in each sex. " Although separate analyses do not allow for statistical comparison between females and males and in fact constitute a widespread and well-described logical error [10,11,20,[26][27][28], CIHR and LIBRA clearly considered such an approach an acceptable method to look for sex differences. CIHR stated, "Sex considerations [can] be taken into account by performing analyses in males and females separately;" in CIHR's quizzes, approaches with separate analyses were marked as "correct, " e.g., a proper sex comparison can be achieved by "separating the data into two groups and then running the analyses separately for each group. ...

Sex-Inclusive Biomedicine: Are New Policies Increasing Rigor and Reproducibility?
  • Citing Article
  • April 2023

Women s Health Issues

... For example, in a recent study of abdominal obesity in children (AO), the authors missed a large sex difference in the association between AO and a measure of lipoprotein particle number because their within-sex p-values showed nonsignificant associations in both girls and boys (Akiyama et al., 2022). In fact, the interaction between sex and AO was highly significant (P=0.001); the association in girls was positive and among boys, negative (Vorland et al., 2023). The significant interaction serves as strong evidence that the association between AO and this measure of lipoprotein does depend on sex -a potentially important finding that was masked by a DISS approach. ...

Determination of sex differences requires formal test for differences: Comment on ‘Lipoprotein‐subclass particle numbers in children with abdominal obesity.’
  • Citing Article
  • February 2023

Pediatrics International

... To investigate this, traditional behavioral paradigms that rely on operant conditioning, such as key-pressing assays [19][20][21] or operant discrimination tasks [22][23][24][25][26] are insufficient. While these methods are valuable for many behavioral studies, they do not require the birds to use their vocalizations, which is essential for studying the flexibility and contextual use of calls. ...

Song preferences predict the quality of vocal learning in zebra finches