Diane N. Ruble’s research while affiliated with New York University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (146)


Correlation matrix for ethnic/racial, gender, and age diversity. Note: E/R = Ethnicity/Race. Colored numbers represent children's ages. Plots represent distributions of responses for each variable. * p < .05; ** p < .001.
Proportion of other–ethnicity/race peers by age and ethnicity. Note: E/R = Ethnicity/Race. Bolded horizontal black lines in box plots represent the sample median; red lines represent the sample mean.
Proportion of other–gender peers by age and ethnicity. Note: Bolded horizontal black lines in box plots represent the sample median; red lines represent the sample mean.
Age diversity by age (in years). Note: Each circle represents one child. Purple lines mark the average peer age range at each age.
Proportion of total peers from each social context by age (in years).

+1

A 6‐year longitudinal exploration of diversity in ethnically/racially minoritized children's early peer circles
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

April 2024

·

22 Reads

·

2 Citations

·

·

May Ling D. Halim

·

[...]

·

Diane Ruble

Exposure to diverse peers can expand children's experiences and skillsets, and these positive effects linger beyond childhood. Yet, little is known about the ethnic/racial, gender, and age diversity in children's peer groups and how it may shift over time. Even less is known about these patterns among US nonwhite children. In the present study, we thus explored how diversity (with regard to ethnicity/race, gender, and age) in ethnically minoritized children's peer groups change from infancy through early childhood and tested whether the diversity of early peer groups remained stable across time. Over a 6‐year period we followed 234 children (ages 1–6; 115 girls) from three large ethnic/racial minority groups in the United States: African American, Dominican American, and Mexican American. With age, children's peer groups increased in ethnic/racial diversity but decreased in gender and age diversity. Moreover, children's early peer diversity (at/around age 2–4) positively predicted the diversity of their later peer groups (at/around age 6) across all three types of diversity. This study provides novel insights into how children's peer groups change and grow in early development, particularly focusing on children from backgrounds that have been historically underrepresented in psychological science.

View access options


The Influence of Siblings on Ethnically Diverse Children’s Gender Typing Across Early Development

May 2021

·

179 Reads

·

8 Citations

Most U.S. children grow up with siblings. Theory and prior work suggest that older siblings are important sources of gender-related information and socialization. However, few studies have investigated the patterns of these associations longitudinally across early childhood. The present study examines the influence of sibling presence and gender composition on the trajectory of early gender-typed behavior and appearance in children from age 2 through 6 in a diverse sample of Dominican American (36%), African American (33%), and Mexican American (31%) mother-child dyads (N = 232; 112 girls, 120 boys) from low-income households in New York City (M = $20,459, SD = 14,632). Results found that children without older siblings spent more time playing with counterstereotypical toys and their mothers' reports indicated similar behavior over the past month (e.g., a girl playing with toy vehicles and balls; a boy playing with toy kitchen sets and dolls) than children with older siblings. Further, children with at least one other-gender sibling (e.g., a girl with an older brother) played more frequently with counterstereotypical toys compared with children with only same-gender siblings (e.g., a girl with only older sisters). Results on the relation between siblings and gender appearance were mixed. Older siblings may thus influence early trajectories of important gender domains (e.g., toy play), which can have various long-term implications for developing skills and interests. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Enjoying Each Other's Company: Gaining Other-Gender Friendships Promotes Positive Gender Attitudes Among Ethnically Diverse Children

February 2021

·

72 Reads

·

27 Citations

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

Gender segregation is ubiquitous and may lead to increased bias against other-gender peers. In this study, we examined whether individual differences in friendships with other-gender children reduce gender bias, and whether these patterns vary by gender or ethnicity. Using a 1-year longitudinal design (N = 408 second graders [Mage = 7.56 years] and fourth graders [Mage = 9.48 years]), we found that, across groups, gaining more other-gender friendships over the year led to (a) increased positive cognitive-based attitudes toward the other gender and (b) increased positive and decreased negative affect when with the other gender. We also tested the reverse pattern and found support for a bidirectional link. Girls and Latinx children often showed more gender bias than did boys and European American children. Implications for promoting positive relationships between girls and boys are discussed.


The influence of center-based care on young children's gender development

July 2020

·

120 Reads

·

8 Citations

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

Many U.S. children spend a significant amount of time in center-based care prior to entering preschool. Previous theory and research would suggest center-based care settings offer important opportunities for gender socialization as children here are surrounded by multiple sources of gender-typing information (e.g. peers, adults, toys and activities). The present longitudinal study examined whether center-based care enrollment status influences level and timing of children's gender-typed behaviors (same-gender friendships, play and appearance), and knowledge (self-categorization and stereotyping) between the ages of 2–5. Participants were children and their mothers of low-income, urban backgrounds (N = 232; African American, Mexican American, and Dominican American). Overall, children enrolled in center-based care at ages 2 and 3 showed higher gender-typing patterns than children enrolled later or not at all. Associations were strongest for same-gender-friendships and gender-typed play, domains that might affect children's subsequent engagement in and learning of certain tasks, skillsets, and activities.


The Roles of Self-Socialization and Parent Socialization in Toddlers’ Gender-Typed Appearance

November 2018

·

393 Reads

·

25 Citations

Children's gender-stereotypical dress and appearance might be one of the first representations of children's emerging sense of gender identity. Gender self-socialization theories posit that as children become more aware of gender categories, they become motivated to adhere to gender stereotypes, such as by expressing interest in dressing in feminine or masculine ways. Socialization theories predict that children's gender-typed appearance reflects parents' choices. For example, gender-traditional parents might dress their children in gender-stereotypical ways. At the same time, dressing in gender-stereotypical ways might contribute to children's growing awareness of gender categories. The current study investigated the factors associated with gender-typed appearance among 175 (87 girls, 88 boys) Mexican American, Dominican American, and African American 2-year-olds. We examined both child and parent contributions to early gender-typed appearance. To measure children's early conceptual understanding of gender categories, we assessed children's use and recognition of gender verbal labels. To examine the influence of parent socialization, we assessed mothers' gender-role attitudes. Children's gender-typed appearance was observed and coded during an assessment. Surprisingly, mothers' gender-role attitudes were not significantly associated with toddlers' gender-typed appearance. However, toddlers' gender labeling was associated with their gender-typed appearance, suggesting that self-socialization processes can be found as early as 24 months of age.



Immigrant Chinese Mothers' Socialization of Achievement in Children: A Strategic Adaptation to the Host Society

December 2016

·

70 Reads

·

23 Citations

Academic socialization by low-income immigrant mothers from Mainland China was investigated in two studies. Immigrant Chinese mothers of first graders (n = 52; Mage = 38.69) in the United States (Study 1) and kindergartners (n = 86; Mage = 36.81) in Hong Kong (Study 2) tell stories that emphasized achieving the best grade through effort more than did African American (n = 39; Mage = 31.44) and native Hong Kong (n = 76; Mage = 36.64) mothers, respectively. The emphasis on achievement was associated with mothers' heightened discussion on discrimination (Study 1) and beliefs that education promotes upward mobility (Study 2), as well as children's expectations that a story protagonist would receive maternal criticism for being nonpersistent in learning (Study 2).


Gender Attitudes in Early Childhood: Behavioral Consequences and Cognitive Antecedents

October 2016

·

346 Reads

·

69 Citations

This study examined factors that predicted children's gender intergroup attitudes at age 5 and the implications of these attitudes for intergroup behavior. Ethnically diverse children from low-income backgrounds (N = 246; Mexican-, Chinese-, Dominican-, and African American) were assessed at ages 4 and 5. On average, children reported positive same-gender and negative other-gender attitudes. Positive same-gender attitudes were associated with knowledge of gender stereotypes. In contrast, positive other-gender attitudes were associated with flexibility in gender cognitions (stereotype flexibility, gender consistency). Other-gender attitudes predicted gender-biased behavior. These patterns were observed in all ethnic groups. These findings suggest that early learning about gender categories shape young children's gender attitudes and that these gender attitudes already have consequences for children's intergroup behavior at age 5.


A Dual Identity Approach for Conceptualizing and Measuring Children's Gender Identity

June 2016

·

345 Reads

·

152 Citations

The goal was to test a new dual identity perspective on gender identity by asking children (n = 467) in three grades (Mage = 5.7, 7.6, 9.5) to consider the relation of the self to both boys and girls. This change shifted the conceptualization of gender identity from one to two dimensions, provided insights into the meaning and measurement of gender identity, and allowed for revisiting ideas about the roles of gender identity in adjustment. Using a graphical measure to allow assessment of identity in young children and cluster analyses to determine types of identity, it was found that individual and developmental differences in how similar children feel to both genders, and these variations matter for many important personal and social outcomes.


Citations (98)


... Indeed, researchers have shown that early peer diversity affects later peer diversity (Bennet et al., 2024). Thus, reduced prejudice in middle childhood has the potential to set children up for flourishing during adolescence and beyond. ...

Reference:

A Longitudinal Examination of Children's Friendships Across Racial Status and Gender and Their Intergroup Prosocial Behavior
A 6‐year longitudinal exploration of diversity in ethnically/racially minoritized children's early peer circles

... Interestingly, however, quite a substantial body of research suggests that only at around 7 years do children draw upon social comparison information in order to make judgments about the self (e.g. Pomerantz, Ruble, Frey, & Greulich, 1995;Ruble, 1983;Ruble & Frey, 1991). A further developmental consideration is that young children's representations about the self are quite isolated from one another; that is, young children have relatively compartmentalized notions about the self, reflecting 'a rudimentary ability to intercoordinate concepts' about the self (Harter, 1999, p. 41). ...

Changing Patterns of Comparative Behavior as Skills Are Acquired: A Functional Model of Self-Evaluation
  • Citing Chapter
  • February 2024

... And for children learning their own and others' places in the world, the marks of these systems are visible. Given what appears to be an innate tendency in humans to notice and interpret social group differences (Arthur et al., 2008;Bigler & Liben, 2006, 2007, as children observe their worlds, they craft inferential explanations for these manifest inequalities. In the United States, where racial and socioeconomic segregation persist in communities and schools (Owens, 2020), and contact across racial and class identities is often fleeting and superficial, there is ample space in children's inferences for the growth of stereotypes and prejudice. ...

Gender Stereotyping and Prejudice in Young Children A Developmental Intergroup Perspective
  • Citing Chapter
  • February 2008

... A dearth of empirical evidence exists on the extent to which prepubertal TGD children understand their gender. Gender constancy refers to a child's ability to report their gender identity stably and consistently over time, irrespective of changes in gender-typed activities or appearances [30]. Based on developmental research to date (and samples presumed to consist of non-TGD children), gender constancy is considered to be achieved by age 4-7 years, and it is foundational to the formation of knowledge structures (ie, gender schemas) that inform how children learn gender-role norms and mores [31]. ...

“God Made Me a Girl”: Sex-Category Constancy Judgments and Explanations Revisited

... have been used to explain differences between men and women, based mainly on the role of evolutionary (Buss, 1988) and social factors (Eagly & Wood, 1991). A more recent view explains gender differences as a product of diverging self-construal -the core of one's self-definition (Baumeister & Sommer, 1997;Cross & Madson, 1997;Krasnova et al., 2017;C. L. Martin & Ruble, 1997). Costa et al. (2001) stated that these explanations are not mutually exclusive, and social roles may modify a biologically based pattern. Lippa (2010) added that many studies have been inconclusive in distinguishing between social-environmental and biological explanations. In summary, in-born differences, socially-endorsed-views and str ...

A Developmental Perspective of Self-Construals and Sex Differences: Comment on

... The presence of children of both genders in a family tends to have a "gender-neutralizing" effect, leading to less gender stereotypes and less gendered communication in Dutch parents (Endendijk et al., 2013;2014). Similarly, having an opposite-gender sibling is associated with less gender-typical behavior in children (Kuchirko et al., 2021) and adolescents (Samek et al., 2015), particularly in girls (Farkas & Leaper, 2013). These findings, obtained in families from diverse ethnic and socio-economic backgrounds, can be explained by the other-gender sibling being a role model for genderatypical behavior. ...

The Influence of Siblings on Ethnically Diverse Children’s Gender Typing Across Early Development

... Compared to their connections with same-gender peers, children are less likely to spontaneously form connections with other-gender peers. Decades of social development research demonstrate that young children's friendships are highly gender-segregated (Halim et al., 2021;Maccoby, 1990;Rose et al., 2022;Shrum et al., 1988), and many children miss out on the unique benefits of cross-gender friendships (Fabes et al., 2019;Rose et al., 2022). Formal structures that encourage intergroup interactions are thought to be helpful in facilitating meaningful connections across group boundaries (Allport, 1954). ...

Enjoying Each Other's Company: Gaining Other-Gender Friendships Promotes Positive Gender Attitudes Among Ethnically Diverse Children
  • Citing Article
  • February 2021

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

... We sought to explore exposure to diverse peers in the understudied developmental period of early childhood for several reasons. First, this is a time of significant cognitive and social development (Rubin et al., 2008) that coincides with shifts in children's social environments (Bennet et al., 2020). From birth, children encounter peers primarily in the family context, but around 2-3 years of age many of them transition into center-based care and school (Rubin et al., 2008). ...

The influence of center-based care on young children's gender development
  • Citing Article
  • July 2020

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology