David Duemler’s research while affiliated with University of California, Santa Barbara and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (4)


Empirical Constraints on Theories of Scientific Reasoning: Reply to Baron (1990)
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

June 1990

·

9 Reads

Journal of Educational Psychology

·

David Duemler

J. Baron (see record 1990-27432-001) makes two assertions concerning our research on reflectiveness in scientific reasoning (D. Duemler and R. E. Mayer; see record 1989-14366-001): (a) His theory, particularly as stated in Baron (1988), predicts our findings, and (b) we have misnamed the independent variable in our study as reflectiveness. In this essay, we briefly respond to each assertion and call for three improvements in theories of scientific reasoning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

View access options

Empirical constraints on theories of scientific reasoning: Reply to Baron (1990).

June 1990

·

6 Reads

Journal of Educational Psychology

J. Baron (see record 1990-27432-001) makes two assertions concerning our research on reflectiveness in scientific reasoning (D. Duemler and R. E. Mayer; see record 1989-14366-001): (a) His theory, particularly as stated in Baron (1988), predicts our findings, and (b) we have misnamed the independent variable in our study as reflectiveness. In this essay, we briefly respond to each assertion and call for three improvements in theories of scientific reasoning.


Hidden Costs of Reflectiveness: Aspects of Successful Scientific Reasoning

December 1988

·

13 Reads

·

14 Citations

Journal of Educational Psychology

We tested a prediction of Baron's (1985) rationality theory: that reflectiveness is directly related to creative problem-solving performance. In two experiments, subjects solved a series of four induction problems involving conventional rules, followed by an induction problem involving an unconventional rule. Subjects who had been extremely reflective or extremely impulsive on successfully solving conventional problems performed more poorly on solving the unconventional problem than did subjects who had been moderately reflective or moderately impulsive on conventional problems. This U-shaped relationship between degree of reflectiveness/impulsiveness on conventional problems and problem-solving performance on an unconventional problem conflicts with predictions of rationality theory. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)


Hidden costs of reflectiveness: Aspects of successful scientific reasoning.

December 1988

·

13 Reads

·

5 Citations

Journal of Educational Psychology

We tested a prediction of Baron's (1985) rationality theory: that reflectiveness is directly related to creative problem-solving performance. In two experiments, subjects solved a series of four induction problems involving conventional rules, followed by an induction problem involving an unconventional rule. Subjects who had been extremely reflective or extremely impulsive on successfully solving conventional problems performed more poorly on solving the unconventional problem than did subjects who had been moderately reflective or moderately impulsive on conventional problems. This U-shaped relationship between degree of reflectiveness/impulsiveness on conventional problems and problem-solving performance on an unconventional problem conflicts with predictions of rationality theory.

Citations (1)


... Brainstorming-"saying whatever comes to mind without evaluation"-is considered an aspect of "non-critical thinking" as is "putting facts, concepts and principles together in new and original ways" (Huitt 1998). However, when students use techniques associated with reason and logic as well as creativity and divergence, they are more successful in problem-solving (Duemler and Mayer 1988) suggesting that a careful balance needs to be maintained in this phase between diverging and converging (Manning 2016), or more broadly between design (creative) and critical thinking. Conscious, rational, and logical reasoning can lead to formalized sets of rules being applied to solution generation and selection, while design thinking approaches leave open the possibility of iterative development of options over time as experience-based feedback is garnered (Glen et al. 2015). ...

Reference:

The Road to Becoming a Design Thinking Business Professional: Expected Bumps Along the Way
Hidden Costs of Reflectiveness: Aspects of Successful Scientific Reasoning

Journal of Educational Psychology