David Andersson’s research while affiliated with Linköping University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (52)


Comprehension in economic games
  • Article
  • Full-text available

May 2025

·

75 Reads

Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization

·

David Andersson

·

·

[...]

·

Many disciplines rely on economic games to measure prosocial behavior. However, there is a concern that participants may misunderstand these games, complicating interpretation of results. This study combines online and laboratory data (total n = 1568) to assess subject comprehension of five standard economic games: the Dictator Game, Ultimatum Game, Trust Game, Public Goods Game, and Prisoner's Dilemma. The online and lab data collections are carried out separately and for the online data collection we collect data for two separate platforms (Prolific and Clou-dResearch's MTurk Toolkit). Within each data collection participants carry out all five games, and are randomized to comprehension questions with or without incentives for correct answers. Results indicate that misunderstanding is common: the proportion of participants who misunderstood ranged from 22 % (Dictator Game) to 70 % (Trust Game) in the online samples and from 22 % (Dictator Game) to 53 % (Public Goods Game) in the lab sample. Incentivizing the comprehension questions had no significant impact on misunderstanding, but numeracy was associated with lower misunderstanding. Misunderstanding also predicted increased prosocial behavior in several of the games. Our findings suggest that misunderstanding may be important in explaining prosocial behavior, making it more complicated to draw clear inferences about social preferences from experimental data.

Download

Fig. 1 | First and last call each shift. Substantial overlap in the afternoon between morning and afternoon shifts gives a quasi-experimental setting. AM shift indicated in light purple, PM shift in purple, and Night shift in dark purple [n = 15,810].
Fig. 2 | Timing and duration of breaks. Each point in the figure represents an individual call indexed by the time it ended (x-axis) and duration in minutes to next call for the same nurse on the same day (y-axis) [n = 145,991].
Fig. 3 | Separation in cumulative workload for observations included in the AM-PM shift overlap subsample. Left: Distribution of time spent at work by type of shift (AM vs. PM) for calls handled during the overlap time window. [n = 9736] Right: Distribution of number of previous calls on the same day by type of shift (AM vs. PM) for calls handled during the overlap time window. AM shift indicated in light purple, PM shift in purple. [n = 9736]. See "Data inclusion and exclusion" subsection for details on how the AM-PM shift overlap subsample was selected.
Fig. 4 | Time of day and assigned urgency. Each bar shows the distribution of urgency ratings for calls during that hour. Higher urgency ratings later in the day would be expected under decision fatigue. This analysis is not part of our main test battery [n = 125,587].
Fig. 5 | Outcomes for morning vs. afternoon shift in the overlap subsample. Left: Data and distribution for Urgency, showing the average (for each nurse) urgency rating assigned for calls during the overlap time window, separated by AM and PM shifts. We hypothesized higher urgency ratings on average for workers on the AM shift. [n = 9648]. Right: Data and distribution for Default, showing the proportion of calls a particular nurse chose the most common urgency rating (personal modal) for that type of call during the overlap time window, separated by AM and PM shift. We hypothesized higher proportion Default for workers on the AM shift. Vertical lines denote mean values. AM shift indicated in light purple, PM shift in purple [n = 6158].

+5

No evidence for decision fatigue using large-scale field data from healthcare

February 2025

·

75 Reads

Communications Psychology

Decision fatigue is the idea that making decisions is mentally demanding and eventually leads to deteriorated decision quality. Many studies report results that appear consistent with decision fatigue. However, most of this evidence comes from observed sequential patterns using retrospective designs, without preregistration or external validation and with low precision in how decision fatigue is operationalized. Here we conducted an empirical test of decision fatigue using large-scale, high-resolution data on healthcare professionals’ medical judgments at a national telephone triage and medical advice service. This is a suitable setting for testing decision fatigue because the work is both hard and repetitive, yet qualified, and the variation in scheduling produced a setting where level of fatigue could be regarded as near random for some segments of the data. We hypothesized increased use of heuristics, more specifically convergence toward personal defaults in case judgments, and higher assigned urgency ratings with fatigue. We tested these hypotheses using one-sided Bayes Factors computed from underlying Bayesian generalized mixed models with random intercepts. The results consistently showed relative support for the statistical null hypothesis of no difference in decision-making depending on fatigue (BF 0+ > 22 for all main tests). We thus found no evidence for decision fatigue. Whereas these results don’t preclude the existence of a weaker or more nuanced version of decision fatigue or more context-specific effects, they cast serious doubt on the empirical relevance of decision fatigue as a domain general effect for sequential decisions in healthcare and elsewhere.


Figure 1 Share of participants viewing rationing as acceptable (versus unacceptable) across experimental conditions The error bars display 95% confidence intervals.
Regression Analysis on Acceptance for Limiting Patients' Access to Treatments
Withdrawing versus Withholding Treatments in Medical Reimbursement Decisions: A Study on Public Attitudes

June 2024

·

43 Reads

·

1 Citation

Medical Decision Making

Background The use of policies in medical treatment reimbursement decisions, in which only future patients are affected, prompts a moral dilemma: is there an ethical difference between withdrawing and withholding treatment? Design Through a preregistered behavioral experiment involving 1,067 participants, we tested variations in public attitudes concerning withdrawing and withholding treatments at both the bedside and policy levels. Results In line with our first hypothesis, participants were more supportive of rationing decisions presented as withholding treatments compared with withdrawing treatments. Contrary to our second prestated hypothesis, participants were more supportive of decisions to withdraw treatment made at the bedside level compared with similar decisions made at the policy level. Implications Our findings provide behavioral insights that help explain the common use of policies affecting only future patients in medical reimbursement decisions, despite normative concerns of such policies. In addition, our results may have implications for communication strategies when making decisions regarding treatment reimbursement. Highlights We explore public’ attitudes toward withdrawing and withholding treatments and how the decision level (bedside or policy level) matters. People were more supportive of withholding medical treatment than of withdrawing equivalent treatment. People were more supportive of treatment withdrawal made at the bedside than at the policy level. Our findings help clarify why common-use policies, which impact only future patients in medical reimbursement decision, are implemented despite the normative concerns associted with thesepolicies.


Flowchart of the translation process according to the ISPOR-technique
CFS-9 2.0, English (a) and Swedish (b) versions
Cross-cultural adaption and inter-rater reliability of the Swedish version of the updated clinical frailty scale 2.0

December 2023

·

58 Reads

·

3 Citations

BMC Geriatrics

Background Worldwide, there is a large and growing group of older adults. Frailty is known as an important dis‑criminatory factor for poor outcomes. The Clinical Frailty Scale (CFS) has become a frequently used frailty instrument in diferent clinical settings and health care sectors, and it has shown good predictive validity. The aims of this study were to describe and validate the translation and cultural adaptation of the CFS into Swedish (CFS-SWE), and to test the inter-rater reliability (IRR) for registered nurses using the CFS-SWE. Methods An observational study design was employed. The ISPOR principles were used for the translation, linguis‑tic validation and cultural adaptation of the scale. To test the IRR, 12 participants were asked to rate 10 clinical case vignettes using the CFS-SWE. The IRR was assessed using intraclass correlation and Krippendorf’s alpha agreement coefcient test. Results The Clinical Frailty Scale was translated and culturally adapted into Swedish and is presented in its final form. The IRR for all raters, measured by an intraclass correlation test, resulted in an absolute agreement value among the raters of 0.969 (95% CI: 0.929–0.991) and a consistency value of 0.979 (95% CI: 0.953–0.994), which indicates excellent reliability. Krippendorf’s alpha agreement coefcient for all raters was 0.969 (95% CI: 0.917–0.988), indicating near-perfect agreement. The sensitivity of the reliability was examined by separately testing the IRR of the group of specialised registered nurses and non-specialised registered nurses respectively, with consistent and similar results. Conclusion The Clinical Frailty Scale was translated, linguistically validated and culturally adapted into Swedish following a well-established standard technique. The IRR was excellent, judged by two established, separately used, reliability tests. The reliability test results did not difer between non-specialised and specialised registered nurses. However, the use of case vignettes might reduce the generalisability of the reliability fndings to real-life settings. The CFS has the potential to be a common reference tool, especially when older adults are treated and rehabilitated in different care sectors.


Differences and similarities between samples in original study and replication
Classification of the replication, based on LeBel et al.
Proportion of participants in each category
Summary of statistical tests for the Likert scale
We are all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers: Successful replication and extension of Svenson (1981)

May 2023

·

336 Reads

·

4 Citations

Meta-Psychology

The better-than-average effect refers to the tendency to rate oneself as better than the average person on desirable traits and skills. In a classic study, Svenson (1981) asked participants to rate their driving safety and skill compared to other participants in the experiment. Results showed that the majority of participants rated themselves as far above the median, despite the statistical impossibility of more than 50% of participants being above the median. We report a preregistered, well-powered (total N = 1,203), very close replication and extension of the Svenson (1981) study. Our results indicate that the majority of participants rated their driving skill and safety as above average. We added different response scales as an extension and findings were stable across all three measures. Thus, our findings are consistent with the original findings by Svenson (1981). Materials, data, and code are available at https://osf.io/fxpwb/


Figure 1. Percent misunderstanding participants for each game, separated by platform (CloudResearch or Prolific) and whether the comprehension check involved monetary incentives. DG = Dictator Game; UG = Ultimatum Game; TG = Trust Game, PGG = Public Goods Game; PD = Prisoner's Dilemma. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. N = 1067.
Sample characteristics for data collected on CloudResearch and Prolific
Results from logistic regressions
Results from OLS regressions of behavior in the games
Comprehension in Economic Games

September 2022

·

293 Reads

Most disciplines rely on economic games to measure prosocial behavior in controlled experimental settings. However, participants’ comprehension of these games might be lower than desirable, which complicates interpretation of results. We here assess subject comprehension of the payoff structure of five standard economic games commonly used to study prosocial behavior: the Dictator Game, Ultimatum Game, Trust Game, Public Goods Game, and Prisoner’s Dilemma. Participants were recruited from two online platforms: Prolific (n = 528) and CloudResearch (using the CloudResearch MTurk toolkit; n = 540). The Trust Game had the highest level of misunderstanding (70%), followed by the Public Goods Game and the Prisoner’s Dilemma (each at 52%), the Ultimatum Game (27%), and the Dictator Game (24%). Study platform was a significant predictor of misunderstanding in the Dictator Game, Ultimatum Game, and Public Goods Game, with greater misunderstanding on Prolific than CloudResearch. Incentivizing the comprehension questions had no significant impact on misunderstanding in any of the games. The only variable that significantly predicted misunderstanding across all games was numeracy, which was associated with lower misunderstanding. Finally, we found suggestive evidence in exploratory analyses that misunderstanding predicts greater contributions in the Public Goods Game (in line with previous studies) and in the Dictator Game, increased the likelihood to choose the option that maximizes total payoff in the Prisoner’s Dilemma and reduced back transfers in the Trust Game. These findings suggest that misunderstanding may be an important factor in explaining prosocial behavior and that reliance on standard one-shot games may lead researchers to overestimate the importance of social preferences.


We are all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers: Successful replication and extension of Svenson (1981)

April 2022

·

134 Reads

The better-than-average effect refers to the tendency to rate oneself as better than the average person on desirable traits and skills. In a classic study, Svenson (1981) asked participants to rate their driving safety and skill compared to other participants in the experiment. Results showed that the majority of participants rated themselves as far above the median, despite the statistical impossibility of more than 50% of participants being above the median. We report a preregistered, well-powered (total N = 1,203), very close replication and extension of the Svenson (1981) study. Our results indicate that the majority of participants rated their driving skill and safety as above average. We added different response scales as an extension and findings were stable across all three measures. Thus, our findings are consistent with the original findings by Svenson (1981). Materials, data, and code are available at https://osf.io/fxpwb/.


figure shows scatter plot of the level prosocial behavior for processing different styles. -4 indicate maximum reliance on deliberation. 4 indicate maximum reliance on intuition.
Affect and prosocial behavior: The role of decision mode and individual processing style

January 2022

·

672 Reads

·

10 Citations

Judgment and Decision Making

We study the effects of experimental manipulation of decision mode (rational "brain" vs. affective "heart") and individual difference in processing styles (intuition vs. deliberation) on prosocial behavior. In a survey experiment with a diverse sample of the Swedish population (n = 1,828), we elicited the individuals' processing style and we experimentally manipulated reliance on affect or reason, regardless of subjects' preferred mode. Prosocial behavior was measured across a series of commonly used and incentivized games (prisoner's dilemma game, public goods game, trust game, dictator game). Our results show that prosocial behavior increased for the affective ("heart") decision mode. Further, individual differences in processing style did not predict prosocial behavior and did not interact with the experimental manipulation.


Figure 1. Experimental scenarios.
Figure 5. The impact of ideological view on the likelihood to correctly assess respectively scenario conditioned on time pressure (A,B) and numeracy (C,D), respectively. Note: The error bars and dashed lines indicate 95% confidence intervals. (A,C) The results of the immigration scenario, where the correct answer is that more immigration decreases the crime rates. (B,D) The results of the gender quota scenario, where the correct answer is that companies that have implemented genre quotas perform worse.
Figure 6. The impact of ideological view on the likelihood to correctly assess respectively scenario, conditioned on numeracy. Note: The dashed lines indicate 95% confidence intervals. The sample is divided by treatment. (A,C) The results of the participants in the time pressure treatment, and (B,D) the results of the participants in the control group.
Motivated reasoning, fast and slow

December 2021

·

296 Reads

·

14 Citations

Behavioural Public Policy

Are people more likely to (mis)interpret information so that it aligns with their ideological identity when relying on feelings compared to when engaging in analytical thinking? Or is it the other way around: Does deliberation increase the propensity to (mis)interpret information to confirm existing political views? In a behavioral experiment, participants ( n = 1207, Swedish sample) assessed numerical information concerning the effects of gender quotas and immigration either under time pressure or under no time pressure. To measure trait differences in cognitive sophistication, we also collected data on numeric ability. We found clear evidence of motivated reasoning when assessing both the effects of gender quotas on companies’ financial results and the effect of refugee intake on crime rates. Subjects who prioritized equality over liberty on the labor market were 13 percentage points less likely to correctly assess numerical information depicting that companies that used gender quotas when hiring made less profit. Subjects who classified themselves as ‘Swedes’ rather than ‘World citizens’ were 14 percentage points less likely to correctly assess numerical information depicting that crime rates decreased following immigration. Time pressure did not affect the likelihood to engage in motivated reasoning, while subjects with higher numeric ability were less likely to engage in motivated reasoning when analyzing information concerning refugee intake, but more likely to engage in motivated reasoning when analyzing information regarding the effect of gender quotas. Together these results indicate that motivated reasoning is primarily driven by individual differences in analytical thinking at the trait level and not by situational factors such as time pressure, and that whether motivated reasoning is primarily driven by analysis or feelings depends on the topic at hand.


Fig. 1. Distribution of numeracy scores in the full sample (left panel, n = 3154) and by political orientation (right panel). Political orientation is a composite measure of two underlying variables measuring ideology and party self-identification. Cons./Rep. = score above zero on the composite indicator for political orientation (n = 1615); Lib./Dem. = score below zero on the composite indicator for political orientation (n = 1539).
Fig. 2. Proportion correct interpretation for crime scenarios as a function of numeracy, plotted by scenario and political orientation. Predicted probabilities based on the logistic regressions (which are displayed in Table 3), evaluated at +/ 1 SD for political orientation (Cons./Rep. and Lib./Dem., respectively). The stimuli (scenario) in Crime increase is congruent with conservative/Republican ideology and the stimuli in Crime decrease is congruent with liberal/Democrat ideology. Sample sizes are n = 756 for Crime increase and n = 768 for Crime decrease.
Fig. 3. Proportion correct interpretation for rash scenarios as a function of numeracy, plotted by scenario and political orientation. Predicted probabilities based on logistic regressions (which are displayed in Table 3), evaluated at +/ 1 SD for political orientation (Cons./Rep. and Lib./Dem., respectively). None of the stimuli (scenarios) is congruent with a specific political ideology. Sample sizes are n = 769 for Rash got worse and n = 780 for Rash got better. These two plots were not listed as confirmatory analyses in the preregistration.
Fig. 4. Summary of main findings in original study (left panel) and replication (right panel). The left panel is based on results reported in Fig. 8 in Kahan et al. (2017). For the replication, the differences in predicted probabilities are calculated from the predictions shown in Figs. 2-3, at numeracy equal to three (low num.) and seven (high num.) correct answers, respectively. Results for the underlying logistic regressions can be found in Table 3.
Analyses for motivated reasoning.
A preregistered replication of motivated numeracy

September 2021

·

183 Reads

·

50 Citations

Cognition

Motivated numeracy refers to the idea that people with high reasoning capacity will use that capacity selectively to process information in a manner that protects their own valued beliefs. This concept was introduced in a now classic article by Kahan, Peters, Dawson, & Slovic [2017, Behavioral Public Policy 1, 54–86], who used numeracy to index reasoning capacity, and demonstrated that the tendency to engage in ideologically congruent interpretation of facts increased substantially with people's numeracy. Despite the importance of this finding, both from a theoretical and practical point of view, there is yet no consensus in the literature about the factual strength of motivated numeracy. We therefore conducted a large-scale replication of Kahan, Peters, Dawson, and Slovic (2017), using a pre-specified analysis plan with strict evaluation criteria. We did not find good evidence for motivated numeracy; there are distinct patterns in our data at odds with the core predictions of the theory, most notably (i) there is ideologically congruent responding that is not moderated by numeracy, and (ii) when there is moderation, ideologically congruent responding occurs only at the highest levels of numeracy. Our findings suggest that the cumulative evidence for motivated numeracy is weaker than previously thought, and that caution is warranted when this feature of human cognition is leveraged to improve science communication on contested topics such as climate change or immigration.


Citations (34)


... The early identification of frail persons can initiate appropriate actions and support prognostication and risk stratification [11,18,19,[26][27][28]. Information on frailty status can promote the identification of persons in need of treatments across different health care sectors and with transdisciplinary acceptance [29][30][31]. ...

Reference:

Clinical Frailty Scale score is a predictor of short-, mid- and long-term mortality in critically ill older adults (≥ 70 years) admitted to the emergency department: an observational study
Cross-cultural adaption and inter-rater reliability of the Swedish version of the updated clinical frailty scale 2.0

BMC Geriatrics

... Replications allow reassessing, validating, and adjusting the original's measurement, examining appropriateness and alignment with theory, and retesting using better validated measures. For example, in our replications of studies with scale predictors we often run the original's alongside different types of measurement or more comprehensive scales in random order and then test them all with comparisons against each other (e.g., Koppel et al., 2023;Zhu and Feldman, 2023). ...

We are all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers: Successful replication and extension of Svenson (1981)

Meta-Psychology

... According to Sun [20] converges among e-participants who are less inclined to participate is minimal. The way people process the supplied information influences their involvement competence [43]. supports people in processing information in ways that enhance their involvement competence In the knowledge-based e-participation, the silent group is perpetually in a conundrum, even though the thinkers' group protects its place by organizing and administering actions. ...

Affect and prosocial behavior: The role of decision mode and individual processing style

Judgment and Decision Making

... However, recent studies have challenged this "motivated reflection" hypothesis. First, the relationship between cognitive control processes and motivated reasoning varies across topics (Strömbäck et al., 2024), and the aforementioned seminal finding has proven difficult to replicate Persson et al., 2021;Stagnaro et al., 2023). Furthermore, when accounting for differences in the prior beliefs between opposing partisans, the "motivated reflection" effect is not well-evidenced (Tappin et al., 2021). ...

Motivated reasoning, fast and slow

Behavioural Public Policy

... If an effect is real, and a study is well-powered enough to be able to detect this effect at a high threshold of likelihood, then a preregistered study would be expected to replicate. There have been many efforts to conduct preregistered replications for a variety of psychological phenomena, and these have had varying degrees of success (e.g., Garrison et al., 2016;Persson et al., 2021). ...

A preregistered replication of motivated numeracy

Cognition

... In Studies 2 and 3, volunteer rates were generally higher in the first round of the games (Figures 3 and 4; see also Supplemental Figures S2-2 and S3-2). This pattern may relate to the intuitive cooperation hypothesis, which suggests that people are more cooperative when making decisions intuitively rather than deliberately, as cooperation is often advantageous in everyday life (Rand et al., 2014; see also Kvarven et al., 2020 for a recent meta-analysis). It is possible that our participants initially volunteered intuitively but later adjusted their behavior more deliberately to better align with equity. ...

The Intuitive Cooperation Hypothesis Revisited: A Meta-analytic Examination of Effect-size and Between-study Heterogeneity

Journal of the Economic Science Association

... Also, the distress variable showed a floor effect. This might be due to the neutral faces without context in the blocks, which are less likely to evoke feelings of distress in comparison to, e.g., an identified victim described to be in need of help (Kogut and Ritov, 2005b;Moche et al., 2020Moche et al., , 2024Västfjäll et al., 2014). ...

Opportunity Cost in Monetary Donation Decisions to Non-identified and Identified Victims

... To address this question, the present study manipulated the cognitive resources directly rather than utilizing individual differences to test the impact of depletion effects on ERC. Specifically, we used the classic word/color Stroop task as a resource depletion task, which is frequently used for depletion research, requiring significant effort to complete (Hagger et al. 2010;Koppel et al. 2019;Dang et al. 2021;Lin et al. 2020;Singh and Göritz 2018). This approach aligns with methodological recommendations for inducing genuine self-control demands through validated cognitive conflict tasks (Forestier et al. 2022). ...

No Effect of Ego Depletion on Risk Taking

... Moreover, emotional reactions associated with both channels activate the amygdala (Ledoux, 2002), the region of the brain responsible for social cognition (Adolphs, 2010), and is more likely to make treated individuals prone to recognize trustworthy signals in the controlled environment or in their own community, thus favoring cooperation (Declerk and Boone, 2015). From a neuroeconomic point of view, it is also easier to explain heterogeneous results in an urban environment, where other cues are more salient, or where intuition provides less guidance (see the debate in Kvarven et al., 2019, Rand, 2019, Alós-Ferrer and Garagnani, 2018, Krajbich et al., 2015. ...

The Intuitive Cooperation Hypothesis Revisited: A Meta-analytic Examination of Effect-size and Between-study Heterogeneity

... People do not always act selfishly or in their best interest; nor are behaviors in every instance purposeful [5]. Combined, these postulates highlight that actions cannot be explained by personal preferences alone and underscore the importance of social interactions in shaping behavior [6]. While those are important avenues of study, this analysis focuses instead on the role of social relationships. ...

Donations to Outgroup Charities, but Not Ingroup Charities, Predict Helping Intentions Toward Street-Beggars in Sweden
  • Citing Article
  • December 2018

Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly