Daniel R. Stalder’s research while affiliated with University of Wisconsin–Whitewater and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (11)


FIGURE 1  Mean scores on a measure of the fundamental attribution error (FAE) as a function of manipulated mood (via video clip, sad or happy) and self-reported type of day (bad or good). FAE scores can range from –6.00 to 6.00 in which zero represents no FAE. Standard errors are represented by the error bars attached to each column.
On Being Happy and Mistaken on a Good Day: Revisiting Forgas's (1998) Mood-Bias Result
  • Article
  • Full-text available

September 2014

·

533 Reads

·

2 Citations

Daniel R Stalder

·

Forgas (1998) reported "on being happy and mistaken," finding that a happy mood increased the fundamental attribution error (FAE) compared to a sad mood. However, the standard attitude-attribution paradigm used by Forgas might contain demand characteristics, to which happy people might be especially susceptible. In addition, Goldenberg and Forgas (2012) showed that a happy mood decreased a form of the FAE. Using the questioner-contestant paradigm, we replicated Forgas's result, but only when participants were already having a "good day."

Download

A Role for Social Psychology Instruction in Reducing Bias and Conflict

June 2012

·

41 Reads

·

7 Citations

Psychology Learning & Teaching

Two studies investigated the potential for social psychology instruction to improve interpersonal understanding and to reduce conflict among American undergraduates. In Study 1, in comparison with general-education social science students who received fewer lectures and exercises on the fundamental attribution error (FAE), social psychology students showed: (a) greater openness to their own FAE; (b) less FAE in explaining anti-American terrorism; and (c) less negative feeling and judgment in response to negative behaviour in general. Self-reported understanding of the FAE concept predicted all three of these outcome measures across courses. In Study 2, from pre- to post-instruction periods, social psychology students showed: (a) an increase in openness to their own biases; (b) a decrease in the FAE in judging three of five targets (driver, employee, terrorist); (c) a decrease in negative feelings toward one of four targets (driver); but (d) a slight increase in a self-serving bias. All but one result in Study 2 remained after covarying for social desirability.


Reasons for divorce provided in Studies 1 and 2 for acceptability measure
Correlations between three marital variables and each of acceptability and comfort
Acceptability and comfort means (and standard deviations) as a function of vow reminder and divorce-rate information (Study 1)
Acceptability, comfort, and trivialization means (and standard deviations) as a function of marital status
Acceptability and comfort means (and standard deviations) as a function of vow reminder and divorce-rate information (Study 2)
The role of dissonance, social comparison, and marital status in thinking about divorce

May 2012

·

78 Reads

·

7 Citations

Using a cognitive dissonance framework, this research tried to identify predictors of openness toward divorce and trivialization of wedding vows. Using single undergraduates, Study 1 showed that those reminded of traditional (divorce-inconsistent) wedding vows reported less openness toward divorce than those not reminded (possibly indicative of dissonance-induced attitude change). Study 1 also showed that those who received social-comparison (divorce-rate) information were more likely to trivialize the vows than those who did not receive such information. Study 2 showed that marital status moderated these two primary effects: married individuals showed a stronger vow-reminder effect than divorced individuals, and divorced individuals showed a stronger social-comparison effect. Study 2 also demonstrated pluralistic ignorance in that participants took divorce more seriously than they thought “other people” did. Other results, implications, and counseling applications are discussed.


Investigation of the Two-Factor Model for the English Version of the Need for Closure Scale

April 2012

·

35 Reads

·

3 Citations

Psychological Reports

Using the English version of the Need for Closure Scale, this research investigated previous, though largely ignored, recommendations to exclude the close-mindedness subscale and to treat the Need for Closure Scale as a two-factor (vs single-factor) scale. Reliability and correlational analyses supported the recommendation to exclude close-mindedness. The two-factor model without close-mindedness yielded better fit indexes than a single-factor model or a two-factor model with close-mindedness. Despite the strong cross-cultural use of the scale, no investigation had yet tested the two-factor model without close-mindedness using any language other than Dutch. The present results and discussion can improve the study of need for closure by encouraging more researchers to consider the supported recommendations. Roets and Van Hiel's proposed modification to make the Need for Closure Scale a single-factor scale was also discussed.


t for Two: Using Mnemonics to Teach Statistics

October 2011

·

287 Reads

·

11 Citations

Teaching of Psychology

This article provides a list of statistical mnemonics for instructor use. This article also reports on the potential for such mnemonics to help students learn, enjoy, and become less apprehensive about statistics. Undergraduates from two sections of a psychology statistics course rated 8 of 11 mnemonics as significantly memorable and helpful in learning statistics. Undergraduates rated the 3 remaining mnemonics as helpful after excluding students who did not recall those mnemonics (beyond scale midpoint). Other measures indicated a relatively high regard for the overall use of statistical mnemonics. In particular, mnemonics were rated as motivating and fun. Students also reported moderate belief that mnemonics could reduce statistics anxiety. The variety of positive findings suggests strong promise for statistical mnemonics.


Competing roles for the subfactors of need for closure in moderating dissonance-produced attitude change

April 2010

·

57 Reads

·

14 Citations

Personality and Individual Differences

Stalder and Baron (1998) found no relation between need for closure (NFC) and dissonance, despite a prediction by Kruglanski and Webster (1996) for a positive relation. However, Stalder and Baron did not consider the two orthogonal subfactors of NFC, decisiveness and need for structure (Neuberg, Judice, & West, 1997). A reanalysis of the original data showed that need for structure predicted dissonance-produced attitude change whereas decisiveness attenuated it. In addition, only decisiveness related to two other modes of dissonance reduction (trivialization and external justification). Results underscore the importance of considering both NFC subfactors and support further investigation of dissonance-NFC connections.


The power of proverbs: Dissonance reduction through common sayings

January 2010

·

71 Reads

·

4 Citations

Current Research in Social Psychology

After reading a detailed account of a serious mistake in which a similar-age other went against personal values or a prior commitment, undergraduates rated their feelings of dissonance (regret, hypocrisy, and stupidity) had they been in the actor's place. Relative to a control condition, reading relevant proverbs such as "everybody makes mistakes" or "live and learn" significantly reduced feelings of dissonance in men but not women. For men but not women, perceived applicability of the proverbs also predicted trivialization of the mistake.


Competing roles for the subfactors of need for closure in committing the fundamental attribution error

November 2009

·

66 Reads

·

8 Citations

Personality and Individual Differences

Webster’s (1993) finding that the need for closure (NFC) trait predicts the fundamental attribution error (FAE) is well-cited but has mixed support. After detailing failed replications and contradictory findings, this article reports an attempt to verify the positive NFC–FAE relation using the questioner–contestant paradigm. Rarely investigated but potentially vital to the field, this research also considered the two orthogonal subfactors of NFC, decisiveness and need for structure (Neuberg, Judice, & West, 1997). Results showed that need for structure predicted, overall NFC partially predicted, but decisiveness attenuated the FAE. Thus, Webster’s finding was both replicated and reversed by different aspects of NFC. Decisiveness adds to a short list of trait moderators of the FAE. Implications are discussed for how to measure NFC.


Revisiting the issue of safety in numbers: The likelihood of receiving help from a group

March 2008

·

125 Reads

·

10 Citations

Social Influence

This article re‐examines Latané and Nida's (198112. Latané , B. and Nida , S. 1981 . Ten years of research on group size and helping. . Psychological Bulletin , 89 : 308 – 324 . [CrossRef], [Web of Science ®], [CSA]View all references) meta‐analysis on the inhibiting effect of groups on helping behavior and highlights the distinction between social inhibition and a victim's likelihood of receiving help. Correcting a minor miscalculation in one of Latané and Nida's analyses indicated that, under restricted communication among bystanders, victims were significantly more likely to receive help from a group than from an individual. New correlational analyses indicated that as group size increased, the likelihood of receiving help increased under restricted communication and did not change under full communication. I address several implications of these new findings, including those regarding the case of Kitty Genovese and the role of communication type in bystander intervention. I also briefly report on recent studies on online requests for help.



Citations (11)


... Psychological literacy plays a key role in reducing the FAE as comprehensive FAE education can help to overcome this FAE blind spot. Stalder (2012) found FAE reductions in social psychology students, both compared to general education students and across instruction periods (i.e., before and after completing a social psychology course on biases and the FAE). Enhanced class activities, such as a real-life case study with classroom discussion (Riggio & Garcia, 2009) or an interactive FAE demonstration (Howell & Shepperd, 2011) can reduce the FAE. ...

Reference:

Informal psychology education and the fundamental attribution error: Testing the effectiveness of accessible online passages
A Role for Social Psychology Instruction in Reducing Bias and Conflict
  • Citing Article
  • June 2012

Psychology Learning & Teaching

... Mnemonics may also help students perform better and/or with less anxiety on exams. Also, as Stalder and Olson (2011) noted, mnemonics are easily added to existing material and thus require less instructor effort relative to other interventions. Stalder (2005) also noted motivational effects for his introductory psychology students: "Students generally reported that acronyms increased their motivation to begin studying, that it was helpful to solve review sheet acronyms [i.e., to review their notes to fill in what each letter stood for] on their own, and that they would try to use acronyms or other mnemonics on their own in future courses" (p. ...

t for Two: Using Mnemonics to Teach Statistics
  • Citing Article
  • October 2011

Teaching of Psychology

... The common sense thinking and memory processes Many authors in social sciences (Bartlett, 1932(Bartlett, /1997Brockmeier, 2012;Bruner, 2000Bruner, , 2015Gavillet, 2003;Guscetti, 2011;Jodelet, 2006Jodelet, , 2015Mieder, 1982;Stalder, 2010;Veyne, 1992;Wertsch, 2002) highlight the fact that in society we shape and assign meanings to our experiences using existing cultural materials. The work of F.C. Bartlett have been invested by many studies interested in the cultural anchoring of psychological processes (Bangerter & Lehmann, 1997;Bruner, 2002;Edwards & Middleton, 1987;Johnston, 2001;Kalampalikis, 2002;Roediger III, Bergman, & Meade, 2003;Wertsch, 2002). ...

The power of proverbs: Dissonance reduction through common sayings
  • Citing Article
  • January 2010

Current Research in Social Psychology

... Several researchers have shown that moods can affect social adjustment; physical health; socializing, due to the fundamental attribution error; and problem-solving (Bisquerra, 2009(Bisquerra, , 2012Fredrickson, 2001;Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002;Górriz et al., 2013Górriz et al., , 2015Rieffe et al., 2004;Stalder & Cook, 2014). ...

On Being Happy and Mistaken on a Good Day: Revisiting Forgas's (1998) Mood-Bias Result

... As SNS seem to be filled with attractive individuals who have a pleasant personality and attractive lifestyle, alternative partners on SNS may be perceived as rather ideal and their traits may be compared in an upward fashion to the traits of one's own partner (Steers, Wickham, & Acitelli, 2014). The outcome of this romantic comparison may influence general relational attitudes (Stalder, 2012) and perceptions about their current partner (Surra & Hughes, 1997) in a negative direction. Such romantic comparison processes can lead to the impression that the current relationship brings more costs than benefits. ...

The role of dissonance, social comparison, and marital status in thinking about divorce

... Takođe, upotrebom mnemonika postiže se i održava dugoročna memorija, gde se strategijom upotrebe slikovnih reprezentacija to i dokazuje u istraživanju Karneja i Levina (Carney & Levin, 2000a;Carney & Levin, 2000b). Predlog upotrebe mnemonika jeste prvenstveno kao pomoćna olakšica za podsećanje učenika nakon savladanog gradiva (Stalder, 2005), a korisno je i da se mnemotehnike koriste sa drugim strategijama učenja (Belleza, 1983). ...

Learning and Motivational Benefits of Acronym Use in Introductory Psychology
  • Citing Article
  • October 2005

Teaching of Psychology

... These situational and individual levels of analysis serve an important methodological purpose of avoiding ecological and atomistic fallacies-i.e., associations found at either the situational and individual level of analysis may not generalize to the other level (Diez Roux, 2002). In the field of bystander studies, the lack of distinction between situational and individual-level associations has led to misconceptions about helping behavior, confusing the distinction between "whether someone intervenes" versus "whether a specific individual intervenes" (Stalder, 2008). As such, we aim to provide insights into bystander GENDER COMPOSITION AND BYSTANDER INTERVENTION 5 intervention across gender compositions from various perspectives, while avoiding ecological and atomistic fallacies. ...

Revisiting the issue of safety in numbers: The likelihood of receiving help from a group
  • Citing Article
  • March 2008

Social Influence

... Need for structure was measured using a French-adapted version of the sixth item of the Need for closure scale (Kruglanski, Webster, & Klem, 1993;Salama-Younes, Guingouain, Floch, & Somat, 2014), which in turn was borrowed from the Personal Need for structure scale (Webster & Kruglanski, 1994). The Need for Closure scale was developed by proposing five facets (preference for order and structure, affective discomfort caused by ambiguity, decisiveness of judgments and decisions, desire for predictability, and closed-mindedness; Mannetti, Pierro, Kruglanski, Taris, & Bezinovic, 2002) as manifestations of one or two latent variables: one representing a preference for structure, including order, and one related to decisiveness (Mannetti et al., 2002;Salama-Younes et al., 2014;Stalder, 2012). The item retained in the present study specifically measures the need for order in the daily life of the respondents (Webster & Kruglanski, 1994). ...

Investigation of the Two-Factor Model for the English Version of the Need for Closure Scale
  • Citing Article
  • April 2012

Psychological Reports

... These additional findings are consistent with the inference account. They do not help adjudicate the applicability of the dissonance account because perceived closure may increase or decrease dissonance (e.g., Carmon et al., 2003;Stalder, 2010). ...

Competing roles for the subfactors of need for closure in moderating dissonance-produced attitude change
  • Citing Article
  • April 2010

Personality and Individual Differences

... In this section, I will briefly present some of the empirical literature on outcome biasroughly, the effect of knowledge of an outcome on the evaluation of the decision or procedure leading to it (Baron and Hershey 1988;Mazzocco et al. 2004). I will also briskly note the closely related phenomenon of hindsight bias (Fishhoff 1975, Hawkins andHastie 1990;Guilbault et al. 2004) and, even more briskly, the phenomenon of fundamental error attribution (Ross 1977;Stalder 2009). ...

Competing roles for the subfactors of need for closure in committing the fundamental attribution error
  • Citing Article
  • November 2009

Personality and Individual Differences