September 2021
·
16 Reads
·
11 Citations
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
September 2021
·
16 Reads
·
11 Citations
January 2007
·
131 Reads
·
30 Citations
Sociological Inquiry
January 1996
September 1993
·
8 Reads
·
7 Citations
October 1992
·
7 Reads
·
21 Citations
American Psychologist
The experiences of 3 social psychologists asked to evaluate (and improve) a series of large-scale programs designed to change energy-related attitudes and behavior are described. The research generated significant change but also substantial conflict involving both the utility companies being evaluated and the state agency that commissioned the evaluation. This experience is reviewed with an eye to maximizing the usefulness of future efforts to apply social psychology in complex and potentially conflicted public policy arenas. The difference between influence and power models of applied research is discussed, and possible remedies are examined for social psychologists undertaking future research similar in scale, policy consequences, or potential controversy. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
December 1991
·
36 Reads
·
11 Citations
Teaching of Psychology
A method for teaching about verbal and nonverbal communication is described. The Interpersonal Perception Task (IPT) consists of a videotape of 30 brief scenes. Viewers answer interpretive questions by decoding the verbal and nonverbal cues in the scenes. Information is presented in all communication channels; several categories of interaction are represented; and for each scene, there is an objective criterion of accurate judgment. Instructional techniques using the IPT highlight the subtlety and complexity of communication cues, teach about specific cues to accuracy, demonstrate the relative importance of communication channels, and help students understand the process of interpretation.
March 1991
·
54 Reads
·
43 Citations
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior
Person perception tasks involving nonverbal communication have a mystified reputation. It is frequently argued that nonverbal cues are accurately, but only unconsciously, perceived. This may explain the frequent response of judges who, when asked to decode a sample of nonverbal behavior, reply that it was just a hunch—i.e., show little or no awareness of how they arrived at a judgment, even a correct one. Two alternative models, the Unconsciousness hypothesis and the Inarticulation hypothesis are posited to describe the possible relationship between accuracy and awareness. Two studies are reported on the relationship between accuracy and awareness, both using the Interpersonal Perception Task (IPT). In the first study, the IPT was given to a sample of 476 undergraduates. Accuracy was measured by how many of the 30 IPT questions these judges answered correctly. Global awareness was indexed by having each judge guess how many of the 30 scenes they had answered correctly. In this coarse analysis, the accuracy-awareness relationship was positive but weak. A second experiment was conducted to produce more fine-grained tests of the accuracy-awareness relationship. Different versions of the IPT were presented to 134 undergraduates. Judges given each version of the IPT were asked to indicate their degree of confidence in each one of their answers. This second experiment found a positive, substantial relationship between accuracy and awareness, and this relationship was particularly strong when analyzed across experimental conditions as a whole. These findings support the Inarticulation hypothesis, and cast doubt on the Unconsciousness hypothesis. Implications for an emerging understanding of how nonverbal communication is processed are discussed.
January 1990
·
16 Reads
The encoding and decoding of verbal and nonverbal cues is basic to the process of social interaction. A method of teaching about verbal and nonverbal communication--the Interpersonal Perception Task (IPT)--consists of a videotape divided into 30 brief scenes. After each scene viewers answer an interpretive question by decoding the verbal and nonverbal cues present in the scene. Five categories of interaction are represented: deception, kinship, status, competition, and intimacy. Information is presented in all communication channels, and for each scene there is an objective criterion of accurate judgment. Several instructional uses of the IPT are designed to highlight the subtlety and complexity of communication cues, teach about specific cues to accuracy for the five types of interaction depicted, demonstrate the relative importance of communication channels, and help students understand the process of interpretation. (Eighteen references are attached.) (Author/SR)
January 1989
·
79 Reads
·
282 Citations
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior
This paper describes the validation of the Interpersonal Perception Task (IPT), a new method for studying the process of social perception. The IPT is a videotape consisting of 30 scenes. Each scene is paired with a multiple-choice question about the interaction depicted in the scene. All scenes contain full-channel sequences of unscripted behavior and employ an objective criterion of accurate judgment. Five common types of social interaction are represented: status, intimacy, kinship, competition, and deception. In the first study the IPT was administered to 438 subjects. Results indicated that subjects performed better than chance for 28 of the 30 scenes and that females performed better than males. A second study investigated the possibility that the people who appear in the IPT display idiosyncratic or unrepresentative behaviors. Three coders performed a scene-by-scene content analysis of the IPT, noting the presence or absence of behaviors which previous researchers have found to be associated with the five areas represented in the IPT. In all but one scene, coders found enough behavioral information to enable correct interpretation. A third study employed a peer nomination procedure to explore the construct validity of the IPT. Subjects obtaining higher scores on the IPT were perceived by their friends as more socially skilled. Finally, in an investigation of the convergent and discriminant validity of the IPT, we found no relationship with a visual acuity task or the Machiavellian scale, a significant positive correlation with the Self-Monitoring Scale, a significant positive correlation with the Social Interpretations Task (SIT), and an even stronger positive correlation with those SIT items which measure the same areas as the IPT. Uses of the IPT to investigate the process and accuracy of interpersonal perception are discussed.
January 1987
·
21 Reads
·
36 Citations
... Nonverbal interaction features have a prominent role in human communication [15]. Nonverbal signals are deployed to portray personality traits, convey attitudes, express emotions or modulate a verbal message [2,5]. Companion robots (especially humanoid companion robots) have a physical entity to which the user applies an expectation of certain social skills [9]. ...
September 2021
... The assumption that nonverbal cues such as proximity, body posture, facial signals, eye contact, and vocalics provide the means through which a perceiver's expectations are conveyed to the target is one shared by many theorists (Archer et al., 1993;Buck, 1993;Darley & Oleson, 1993;DePaulo, 1993;Hall & Briton, 1993). Many have noted that nonverbal communication behaviors are particularly powerful for " prompting unambiguous meanings and evaluations " (Burgoon & LePoire, 1993, p. 71) because they are more " spontaneous " (Darly & Oleson, 1993) and function under less cognitive control than verbal behaviors (Buck, 1993). ...
September 1993
... In general, high uptakes of residential rooftop PV are mainly supported by government support such as subsidies, as suggested by Chaianong and Pharino (Chaianong and Pharino 2015). In addition to that, several factors also affect people installing rooftop PV, such as expanded awareness of energy use (Truffer et al. 2001), bill thrift (Herring et al. 2007), perceivability of the home exterior as a societal position symbol (Archer et al. 1987), and environmental advantages of diminishing nearby contamination (Luque 2001;Pearce 2002). One interesting finding in Sri Lanka suggests that middle-aged inhabitants who are educated as well as retirees also have the tendency to install rooftop PV at their households (Jayaweera et al. 2018). ...
January 1987
... Following this trend, the authors intend to look into the usage of nonverbal communication strategy use among learners of English in Malaysian setting so as to bridge the gap on the study on nonverbal mode of communication strategies of Malaysian learners. It has been found that little attention has been given in language teaching to non-verbal communication as a complement to spoken language [20], [21]. ...
December 1991
Teaching of Psychology
... Unfortunately this attitudebehaviour approach is often an ineffective way of sparking behaviour change, and when evaluated, they repeatedly fall short of achieving their goal (McKenzie-Mohr, 2013). Large scale information and advertising campaigns also tend to be very expensive In one rather extreme example, utility companies in American state of California spent yearly about 200-milliondollar on advertising campaigns promoting the installation of energy-efficient devices in households, along with suggestions on behaviour changes that could save energy (like closing windows on sunny days; Archer et al., 1983). However, when evaluated only mixed results could be shown, at best, and audits suggested that there is a weak linkage between consumers receiving conservation information and actually acting on that information (Coltrane et al., 1986). ...
April 1984
Evaluation Review
... Thus, the use of a single encoder may be problematic (Bänziger et al., 2011a;Riggio, 2006). Second, the test is likely culture specific (Archer et al., 2001;Hall, 2001;Nowicki & Duke, 2001). Initial research included 20 nations outside of the United States and showed that every culture scored better than chance; the cultures most similar to the United States scored the best (Rosenthal et al., 2013). ...
Reference:
Profile of Nonverbal Sensitivity (PONS)
... Stern's model, as detailed in [38], integrates individual elements (such as attitude, habit, and routine) alongside contextual factors (comprising external conditions and personal capabilities) to construct a multifaceted framework. In [41], a comprehensive socialpsychological model of energy use behavior is presented, which includes two sets of factors (psychological and positional) that interact in a complex manner to prompt users to make proactive choices that either facilitate or hinder their energy-related actions. At the same time, Ref. [42] notes that both micro-level factors (such as preferences, values, attitudes, and opportunities) and macro-level factors (including socio-cultural changes, technological advances, economic and demographic trends, regulations, and policies) have a substantial influence on household energy consumption. ...
May 1986
American Psychologist
... va R. Tamari tomonidan berilgan emoji taʼrifida, emoji hissiyotlarni ifodalash yoki biror kishini jiddiy boʻlmagan tarzda belgilash niyatida ishlatiladigan ramzlar sifatida izohlanadi [1]. Emoji tovush tonini, ifoda yoki jismoniy harakat yoki holatni yozma aloqa orqali yetkazishdagi oʻrinni qoplash maqsadida yaratilgan. ...
June 1977
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
... Since the days of Stern, other calls for a broadening of energy research have come from a variety of studies [2][3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10]. While varied in the gravity of their critique, these calls have generally posited that energy research has downplayed the role of choice and the human dimensions of energy use and environmental change. ...
October 1992
American Psychologist
... In contrast to cognitive load theory, additive theory postulates that accuracy accumulates as a linear function of available information (Archer & Akert, 1980). This implies that video representations of a situation are expected to be more authentic than written text transcripts. ...
January 2007
Sociological Inquiry