Dana Turjeman’s research while affiliated with Reichman University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (9)


Partisan gaps in the shares of devices engaging in different types of activity
Notes: This figure plots the estimated coefficient βw(t) in Eq (1) and the corresponding 95% confidence interval where the dependent variable is indicated at the top of each panel. The week of February 1, 2020, is taken as the baseline t = 0. The y-axis indicates the beginning date of the week for which the coefficients are reported. Observations are weighted by the number of candidate devices in the county, and standard errors are clustered at the county level.
Partisan gaps in actions, attitudes, worries, beliefs
Notes: This figure plots the estimated Democrat—Republican partisan gaps obtained from the estimates of ατ in Eq (2) and the corresponding 95% confidence intervals. The x-axis indicates the period τ. In Panel A, a positive estimate means that, ceteris paribus, Democratic respondents are more likely than Republican respondents to have taken an action indicated in the legend. The actions studied are “Wash Hands”–wash hands more often; “Wear Mask”– wear a mask when out and about; “Not See Friends”– do not meet any friends or extended family; “Avoid Gatherings”– avoid public transportation and large gatherings. In Panel B, a positive estimate means that, ceteris paribus, Democratic respondents are more likely than Republican respondents to feel comfortable with an activity indicated in the legend. Activities studied are “Restaurant, In”– eat in a restaurant with indoor seating; “Restaurant, Out”– eat in a restaurant with outdoor seating; “10 + ppl”– be part of a gathering with more than 10 people; “Cafe”– go to a coffee shop; “Bar”– go to a bar; “Gym”– go to a gym; “Shopping”– go shopping for non-grocery items; “Grocery”– go grocery shopping. In Panel C, a positive estimate means that, ceteris paribus, Democratic respondents worry more about the health well-being of the group of people indicated in the legend. The groups of people are “Self”– respondent herself; “Partner”– respondent’s partner; “Children”– respondent’s kids; “Ext. Family”– respondent’s extended family; “Comm.”– members of the respondent’s community; “U.S.”– all people in the U.S. In Panel D, a positive estimate means that, ceteris paribus, Democratic respondents predict a larger number on the outcomes indicated in the legend. Outcomes over which expectations are elicited are “U.S. Deaths”– total number of deaths in the U.S. by a target date; “Chance of Infection”– chances that the respondent will get infected with the coronavirus in the next three months; “Chance of Serious Illness”– chances that the respondent will have serious symptoms should she get infected.
Partisan gap heterogeneity across high-risk and low-risk individuals
Notes: This figure plots the estimated Democrat—Republican partisan gaps for low-risk and high-risk respondents. In each panel, a dashed line and the marker on it give the confidence interval and the estimate for ατ in Eq (3), which is the partisan gap among low-risk respondents, and a solid line and the marker on it give the confidence interval and the estimate for ατ + γτ, which is the partisan gap among high-risk respondents. The x-axis indicates the period τ. The legends are the same as those in Fig 2.
Partisan gap heterogeneity across those who consume news across the political line vs. those who do not
Notes: This figure plots the estimated Democrat—Republican partisan gaps for respondents who consume news across the political line and for those who do not. In each panel, a dashed line and the marker on it give the confidence interval and the estimate for ατ in Eq (3), which is the partisan gap among consumers of narrow news, and a solid line and the marker on it give the confidence interval and the estimate for ατ + γτ, which is the partisan gap among consumers of news across the political line. The x-axis indicates the period τ. The legends are the same as those in Fig 2.
Partisan gap heterogeneity across those who pay attention to other information sources vs. those who do not
Notes: This figure plots the estimated Democrat—Republican partisan gaps for respondents whose average attention score on a 1–5 scale across other information sources (Friends, Family, Scientists, Pastor, Facebook or Twitter, CDC, Governor, President) is greater than 3.5, and those whose average attention score is 3.5 or lower. In each panel, a dashed line and the marker on it give the confidence interval and the estimate for ατ in Eq (3), which is the partisan gap among those who do not pay a lot of attention to other information sources, and a solid line and the marker on it give the confidence interval and the estimate for ατ + γτ, which is the partisan gap among those who pay high attention overall. The x-axis indicates the period τ. The legends are the same as those in Fig 2.
A tale of two pandemics: The enduring partisan differences in actions, attitudes, and beliefs during the coronavirus pandemic
  • Article
  • Full-text available

October 2023

·

17 Reads

Ying Fan

·

A. Yeşim Orhun

·

Dana Turjeman

Early in the new coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic, scholars and journalists noted partisan differences in behaviors, attitudes, and beliefs. Based on location data from a large sample of smartphones, as well as 13,334 responses to a proprietary survey spanning 10 months from April 1, 2020 to February 15, 2021, we document that the partisan gap has persisted over time and that the lack of convergence occurs even among individuals who were at heightened risk of death. Our results point to the existence and persistence of the interaction of partisanship and information acquisition and highlight the need for mandates and targeted informational campaigns towards those with high health risks.

Download




Field Evidence of the Effects of Pro-sociality and Transparency on COVID-19 App Attractiveness

July 2021

·

7 Reads

·

1 Citation

COVID-19 exposure-notification apps have struggled to gain adoption. Existing literature posits as potential causes of this low adoption: privacy concerns, insufficient data transparency, and the type of appeal used to pitch the pro-social behavior of installing the app. In a field experiment,we advertised CovidDefense, Louisiana's COVID-19 exposure-notification app, at the time it was released. We find that all three hypothesized factors -- privacy, data transparency, and appeals framing -- relate to app adoption, even when controlling for age, gender, and community density. Specifically, we find that collective-good appeals are effective in fostering pro-social COVID-19 app behavior in the field. Our results empirically support existing policy guidance on the use of collective-good appeals and offer real-world evidence in the on-going debate on the efficacy of such appeals. Further, we offer nuanced findings regarding the efficacy of transparency -- about both privacy and data collection -- in encouraging health technology adoption and pro-social COVID-19 behavior. Our results may aid in fostering pro-social public-health-related behavior and for the broader debate regarding privacy and data transparency in digital healthcare.


Essays on Privacy

January 2021

·

50 Reads

We create troves of data with nearly every step we take, every button we click, and every query we submit. These data can be used to cater to us with services that better align with our desires. They can help us locate restaurants matching our tastes, build up social networks with individuals sharing similar characteristics, find a soul mate or distant relative, attain financial goals, detect our health conditions, and potentially assist in developing individualized medicine. However, misuse of the data can induce us to buy things we don't need, offer us things that might harm our health, lead to an addiction, or even imprison us in the absence of wrongdoing. These data might also be breached, causing harm to us and our loved ones with revelations we might have never shared with the world. In this dissertation, in a series of three chapters, I detect opportunities and propose approaches to reduce the potential risks and leverage the benefits of data collection and data usage. I first analyze users' reactions to the data breach in a matchmaking website, exploring their engagement changes and potentially insufficient behaviors in privacy protection following the breach. I then plot how years of data collection in the Marketing realm and other business domains have led to great improvements to our lives, but have also introduced harms -- some of which are are still likely awaiting revelation. I discuss potential avenues for improving the benefits of the vast data we all create, while reducing the risks associated with those data. Finally, I explicitly develop one of these solutions -- a privacy preserving data fusion methodology -- intended to securely combine datasets while reducing the risks of de-identification. This dissertation, I hope, will serve as a steppingstone towards making the Marketing domain a safer zone in terms of privacy preservation. Marketing efforts were a major driver towards vast data collection and the associated benefits and harms; the marketing domain can now drive the efforts to further improve the benefits and reduce those harms.




Citations (5)


... Beyond this intentional data misuse, there are concerns about data leakage caused by hackers or unintentional mishandling [55], [114]. People are also reluctant to interact with entities with any history of data breaches, doubting their ability to safeguard sensitive information [108]. ...

Reference:

The Role of Privacy Guarantees in Voluntary Donation of Private Data for Altruistic Goals
When the Data Are Out: Measuring Behavioral Changes Following a Data Breach
  • Citing Article
  • August 2023

Marketing Science

... Prior research on COVID-19 tracing apps demonstrated that expected app performance, social influence, attitude, subjective norms, self-efficacy, privacy, transparency in data usage, and trust in the app provider positively influence the intention to install or use a tracing app (Dooley et al., 2020;Kaptchuk et al., 2020;Kozyreva et al., 2021;Lohar et al., 2021;Munzert et al., 2021;S. Sharma et al., 2020;Utz et al., 2021;Walrave et al., 2020). ...

Field Evidence of the Effects of Pro-sociality and Transparency on COVID-19 App Attractiveness
  • Citing Preprint
  • July 2021

... Likewise, privacy risks negatively indirectly influenced the actual use of contact-tracing apps [36]. Dooley et al. suggested that clearly communicating how individual data were protected and being transparent about privacy in a collective setting may reduce concerns regarding personal privacy risk [37]. Contrary to the above findings, a study conducted in Saudi Arabia established that privacy risk was not a significant factor influencing behavioural intention to use the contact-tracing app [38]. ...

Field Evidence of the Effects of Privacy, Data Transparency, and Pro-social Appeals on COVID-19 App Attractiveness
  • Citing Conference Paper
  • April 2022

... Zahlreiche Studien theoretischer (siehe unter anderem Ellison, 2005;Heidhues et al., 2021;2017), laborexperimenteller (siehe unter anderem Rasch et al., 2020;Robbert, 2014;Robbert & Roth, 2014;Santana et al., 2020) sowie feldexperimenteller (siehe unter anderem Blake et al., 2021;Bradley & Feldman, 2020;Brown et al., 2010;Chetty et al., 2009;Hossain & Morgan, 2006) (Bian et al., 2023;Kraft et al., 2024). Zudem reagieren Verbraucher:innen auf offenbarte Datenschutzverletzungen, indem sie sich von betroffenen Diensten abwenden, wenn sie einen (potenziellen) Verlust ihrer Privatsphäre befürchten (Turjeman & Feinberg, 2024). Gleichzeitig besteht die Herausforderung, dass Verbraucher:innen nicht immer über die notwendigen Informationen oder Mittel verfügen, um informierte Entscheidungen im Hinblick auf die Nutzung ihrer Daten zu treffen (Acquisti et al., 2016). ...

When the Data Are Out: Measuring Behavioral Changes Following a Data Breach
  • Citing Article
  • January 2019

SSRN Electronic Journal