Cynthia L. Pickett's research while affiliated with University of California, Davis and other places
What is this page?
This page lists the scientific contributions of an author, who either does not have a ResearchGate profile, or has not yet added these contributions to their profile.
It was automatically created by ResearchGate to create a record of this author's body of work. We create such pages to advance our goal of creating and maintaining the most comprehensive scientific repository possible. In doing so, we process publicly available (personal) data relating to the author as a member of the scientific community.
If you're a ResearchGate member, you can follow this page to keep up with this author's work.
If you are this author, and you don't want us to display this page anymore, please let us know.
It was automatically created by ResearchGate to create a record of this author's body of work. We create such pages to advance our goal of creating and maintaining the most comprehensive scientific repository possible. In doing so, we process publicly available (personal) data relating to the author as a member of the scientific community.
If you're a ResearchGate member, you can follow this page to keep up with this author's work.
If you are this author, and you don't want us to display this page anymore, please let us know.
Publications (23)
Many social phenomena do not result solely from intentional actions by isolated individuals, but rather emerge as the result of repeated interactions among multiple individuals over time. However, such phenomena are often poorly captured by traditional empirical techniques. Moreover, complex adaptive systems are insufficiently described by verbal m...
According to optimal distinctiveness theory (ODT), individuals prefer social groups that are relatively distinct compared to other groups in the individuals' social environment. Distinctive groups (i.e., groups of moderate relative size) are deemed "optimal" because they allow for feelings of inclusion and social connection while simultaneously pro...
Leonardelli, Pickett, Joseph and Hess integrate Brewer’s (1991) optimal distinctiveness theory with the nested categorical context typically descriptive of an organization’s internal structure, where individuals belong to groups (subgroups) nested within more inclusive superordinate groups (such as functional units nested within an organization). I...
Optimal distinctiveness theory [Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: on being the same and different at the same time. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(5), 475–482] proposes that individuals have two fundamental and competing human needs—the need for inclusion and the need for differentiation—that can be met by membership in moderatel...
Previous research indicates that rejection by a group causes aggressive responses. However, in these previous studies, rejected participants were led to believe that they were liked and accepted before the rejection; likely, this rejection was highly unanticipated. Sociometer theory (Leary et al., 1995) proposes the existence of a psychological mec...
Recent research (Twenge, Catanese, & Baumeister, 2003) demonstrated decreased self-awareness among socially-rejected individuals as a defensive strategy designed to buffer the self from the acute distress of rejection. In the present study, we sought to demonstrate that this decreased self-awareness among socially-rejected individuals is: (a) prima...
Optimal distinctiveness theory [Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: on being the same and different at the same time. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(5), 475–482] proposes that individuals have two fundamental and competing human needs—the need for inclusion and the need for differentiation—that can be met by membership in moderatel...
David Galenson's research on creativity has identified two unique creative methods: conceptual and experimental. These methods have different processes, goals, and purposes. To determine whether (a) college students use one method more than the other, and (b) if one method is superior to the other, the authors randomly assigned 115 college students...
Humans are driven by a variety of needs, motives, and goals. Dating back to the early part of the twentieth century, researchers have attempted to understand human behavior by linking behavior to underlying motiva-tions(e.g., Hull, 1943; Spence, 1956). In line with this tradition of exam-ining human behavior within the framework of individual goals...
The skill-deficit view of loneliness posits that unskilled social interactions block lonely individuals from social inclusion. The current studies examine loneliness in relation to social attention and perception processes thought to be important for socially skilled behavior. Two studies investigate the association between social monitoring (atten...
Despite the obvious benefits of social connectedness, one of the barriers to its achievement is social exclusion and rejection by others. Most societies, including non-human societies (Lancaster, 1986; Raleigh & McGuire, 1986), engage in routine rejection of some of their group members. This rejection is often the result of an individual failing to...
To successfully establish and maintain social relationships, individuals need to be sensitive to the thoughts and feelings of others. In the current studies, the authors predicted that individuals who are especially concerned with social connectedness--individuals high in the need to belong--would be particularly attentive to and accurate in decodi...
A robust finding in the psychological literature is that objects belonging to the same category invite comparison more readily than objects belonging to different categories. However, little attention has been given to whether the type of shared category matters for comparison processes. In this paper, we predicted that the ease with which comparis...
This research was conducted to explore the impact of assimilation and differentiation needs on content-specific self-stereotyping. According to optimal distinctiveness theory (M. B. Brewer, 1991), social identities serve the function of satisfying individuals' need for assimilation (in-group inclusion) and their need for differentiation (distinctiv...
At the heart of optimal distinctiveness theory is the idea that a group’s level of inclusiveness is a significant determinant of how well that group can meet members’ needs for assimilation and differentiation. In two studies, this principle was demonstrated by experimentally manipulating both needs and examining their effects on perceptions of ing...
This research was conducted to explore the impact of assimilation and differentiation needs on content-specific self-stereotyping. According to optimal distinctiveness theory (M. B. Brewer, 1991), social identities serve the function of satisfying individuals' need for assimilation (in-group inclusion) and their need for differentiation (distinctiv...
The goal of the present research was to demonstrate the influence of perceiver motivations on perceptions of in-group and out-group homogeneity. Based on Optimal Distinctiveness Theory (Brewer, 1991), it was predicted that arousal of assimilation and differentiation needs (through threats to intragroup standing and intergroup distinctiveness) would...
The present research indicates that perceivers’ beliefs about a group’s level of entitativity can affect the extent to which group members are implicitly compared with one another. To find evidence for these implicit comparisons, a variation of the Ebbinghaus illusion was used. Experiment 1 demonstrated that an identical set of faces produced a gre...
The need to belong has been forwarded as a pervasive human motive, influencing a range of cognitive, emotional, and behavioural responses. The current research explored the influence of belongingness needs on the selective retention of social information. Just as physical hunger results in selective memory for food-relevant stimuli, it was hypothes...
Citations
... Thus, the authors conclude that virtual agents with human-like qualities have the potential to serve as a temporary substitute for social interaction when real-life social interactions are unavailable. This idea is referred to as a "social snack" [36]. Similarly, Christoforakos and Diefenbach [37] stated that even though technology cannot truly satisfy social needs in the same way as human interaction, it can alleviate negative emotional states such as loneliness or boredom. ...
... Based on the SIP-model, prior victimization experiences likely affect how victims encode, interpret, and respond to social cues as their past experiences color the interpretation of new social situations. First, victims may have a positive social-cognitive style and focus on positive social cues and interpretations to facilitate reaffiliation (Bernstein, 2003;Pickett & Gardner, 2005), called the reaffiliation hypothesis in this review. In line with this hypothesis, socially excluded people tend to focus more on positive social cues (Buckner et al., 2010) and have better memory recall for positive events (DeWall et al., 2011). ...
... Moreover, I showed the existence of a hierarchical three-level structure, according to which the eleven shopping motives may be aggregated in five main components, associable to the Big Five factors of human personality, and, through these latter, in two meta-dimensions (cf. Digman 1997;Mooradian and Olver 1996), associable to the two main values pursued by people during shopping (Guido 2006b): i.e., a Hedonic meta-dimension, linked to the Open- ness to Experience, the Agreeableness, and the Extroversion factors; and a Utilitarian meta-dimension, linked to the Emotional Stability and the Conscientiousness factors. Together with shopping motives, we considered levels of materialism to profile consumers' religious- like behaviours. ...
... The upward-mobility hypothesis assumes that naturalized citizens are motivated to reach central positions within the national majority group (Barreto & Ellemers, 2009;Pickett, Bonner, & Coleman, 2002). Naturalized citizens who wish to get as close as possible to the national prototype may endorse acculturation orientations which are prototypically associated with the national majority group, and derogate immigrants in order to publicize their new national allegiance (Branscombe, Ellemers, et al., 1999;Ellemers & Jetten, 2013;Jetten, Branscombe, Spears, & McKimmie, 2003). ...
... However, another promising method for studying emergent processes is agentbased modeling (for reviews, see Smaldino, Calanchini, & Pickett, 2015). Agent-based modeling is a technique that reproduces behaviors of entities, such as employees or organizations, in a way that mimics ongoing, real-world interactions in an artificial environment (Fioretti, 2013). ...
... Recent research has shown that firms may have different propensities to conform and varying ways of doing so depending on whether they are more likely to see conformity as a way to avoid the threat of social disapproval and delegitimation or pursue the opportunity to reap the social gains associated with quality recognition (Durand & Kremp, 2016;Kennedy & Fiss, 2009). These motivational drivers for conformity depend on firm membership in one or multiple social categories (Cattani, Porac, & Thomas, 2017;Vergne & Wry, 2014) that segment and order the social structure of a field or industry, as well as firm owners' and managers' concerns for their own and their firm's social identity (Leonardelli et al., 2011;Vergne & Wry, 2014). For example, Zhao et al. (2016) have recently suggested that conformity and distinctiveness are not necessarily mutually exclusive and optimal distinctiveness rests on the constant interplay between managerial agency and stakeholder evaluation. ...
... From the perspective of ODT, a social identity of this kind fulfills the need for distinctiveness in addition to the need for inclusion. Thus, such a valued group membership may be more likely to be affirmed and defended through expressions of in-group loyalty and favoritism (Brewer, 1991;Leonardelli et al., 2010;Pickett & Leonardelli, 2006). ...
Reference: Subjective belonging and in-group favoritism
... Because participants had transitioned to so many different high schools, it was not possible for us to assemble their class schedules and objectively determine how many students of their racial/ethnic group were present in any specific course. Although there may be instances of overestimation or underestimation of group size compared to actual size (e.g., Pickett et al., 2002), subjective perceptions of group size are important in their own right and have explanatory power because they tell us how each person uniquely perceives their context (Syed et al., 2018). Thus, subjectively perceived group size is likely to be more related to feelings of belonging than objective measures. ...
... First, the Belonging Regulation Model [7][8][9] assumes that a threat to the need to belong, e.g., by means of loneliness or social rejection, activates a social monitoring system, followed by an attempt to restore this need by increasing attention to social cues (e.g., by checking the affective state of our interaction partners). Increased attention to social cues represents a potential prerequisite to social success because this information helps us behave in a socially acceptable manner [10]. Additionally, research on social reconnection strategies showed that social exclusion increased the motivation to build a new social bond with so-far-unknown other individuals [11]. ...
... According to optimal distinctiveness theory, individuals evaluate a given behavior more positively when they perceive that it will help them achieve their goals related to inclusion and distinctiveness (Leonardelli et al., 2010). For example, individuals had positive perceptions toward purchasing and wearing a team sweatshirt featuring in-group symbols when they perceived that it would satisfy their need for inclusion (Leonardelli et al., 2010). ...