Constantijn De Baecke’s research while affiliated with Ghent University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (3)


Table 3 : animate concrete NP1 showed a 66%
Relative clause attachment in Dutch: On-line comprehension corresponds to corpus frequencies when lexical variables are taken into account
  • Article
  • Full-text available

June 2006

·

210 Reads

·

67 Citations

·

Constantijn De Baecke

·

Timothy Desmet

·

Desmet, Brysbaert, and De Baecke (2002a) showed that the production of relative clauses following two potential attachment hosts (e.g., `Someone shot the servant of the actress who was on the balcony') was influenced by the animacy of the first host. These results were important because they refuted evidence from Dutch against experience-based accounts of syntactic ambiguity resolution, such as the tuning hypothesis. However, Desmet et al. did not provide direct evidence in favour of tuning, because their study focused on production and did not include reading experiments. In the present paper this line of research was extended. A corpus analysis and an eye-tracking experiment revealed that when taking into account lexical properties of the NP host sites (i.e., animacy and concreteness) the frequency pattern and the on-line comprehension of the relative clause attachment ambiguity do correspond. The implications for exposure-based accounts of sentence processing are discussed.

Download

Figure 2. Probability of word recognition as a function of presentation duration and word position relative to fixation location. Empirical data and best fitting Gaussian distributions (reprinted from Brysbaert et al., 1996). 
TABLE 3 Skipping probability and fixation time measures (in ms) on the target word as function of contextual constraint and target word length 
TABLE 4 Mean launch site in function of target type and skipping 
sentence for a two-letter predictable word Preceding context: Hanneke begon haar stage als verpleegster. Na een lange en zware opleiding was ze blij dat ze eindelijk eens kon ervaren hoe het er allemaal praktisch aan toe gaat. Een vroedvrouw begeleidde haar bij de eerste taak. Hanneke maakte het bed 
Word skipping in reading: On the interplay of linguistic and visual factors

April 2004

·

1,395 Reads

·

101 Citations

European Journal of Cognitive Psychology

An eye movement experiment is reported in which target words of two and four letters were presented in sentences that strongly raised the expectation of a par-ticular word. There were three possible conditions: The expected word was present in the sentence, an unexpected word of the same length was present, or an unexpected word of a different length was present (all continuations were accep-table, but the latter two were difficult to predict). Our first purpose was to test one of the core assumptions of the Extended Optimal Viewing Position model of eye guidance in reading (Brysbaert & Vitu, 1998). This model states that word skip-ping is primarily a function of the length of the upcoming word. It leads to the prediction that an unpredicted two-letter word will be skipped more often than a predicted four-letter word, which is indeed what we observed. Our second aim was to determine if we could obtain an interaction between context predictability and parafoveal word length, by looking at what happens when the length of the parafoveal word does not agree with the length of the expected word. No such interaction was observed although the effects of both word length and predict-ability were substantial. These findings are interpreted as evidence for the hypothesis that visual and language-related factors independently affect word skipping. Denis Drieghe and Timothy Desmet are research assistants of the Fund for Scientific Research (Flanders, Belgium). Due to a software error which resulted in an incorrect calculation of the skipping data, this research has previously been presented with slightly different data (Drieghe & Brysbaert, 2001). We thank Keith Rayner, Franc Ëoise Vitu, Sarah White, and an anonymous reviewer for the many helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper.


TABLE 2 Number of NP1 and NP2 attachments as a function of head type, based on the clear instances from Table 1 
of NP1 and NP2 completions as a function of head type in the second completion study 
The correspondence between sentence production and corpus frequencies in modifier attachment

August 2002

·

341 Reads

·

67 Citations

The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A

We examined the production of relative clauses in sentences with a complex noun phrase containing two possible attachment sites for the relative clause (e.g., "Someone shot the servant of the actress who was on the balcony."). On the basis of two corpus analyses and two sentence continuation tasks, we conclude that much research about this specific syntactic ambiguity has used complex noun phrases that are quite uncommon. These noun phrases involve the relationship between two humans and, at least in Dutch, induce a different attachment preference from noun phrases referring to non-human entities. We provide evidence that the use of this type of complex noun phrase may have distorted the conclusions about the processes underlying relative clause attachment. In addition, it is shown that, notwithstanding some notable differences between sentence production in the continuation task and in coherent text writing, there seems to be a remarkable correspondence between the attachment patterns obtained with both modes of production.

Citations (3)


... Some researchers have suggested that eye movement decisions, especially very early decisions such as word skipping, are not closely tied to language processing (McConkie & Yang, 2003a, 2003bVitu, 2003). However, there is overwhelming evidence that lexical and contextual variables do influence fixation durations during reading (Rayner, 1998(Rayner, , 2009Staub, 2015), and that they are processed early enough to affect the majority of fixations (Reingold et al., 2012;Sheridan & Reingold, 2012) and word skipping decisions (see Brysbaert & Vitu, 1998, for meta-analysis;Drieghe et al., 2004Drieghe et al., , 2005. Schotter (2018) highlighted two premises that could account for how eye movement decisions precede the completion of word identification: (1) readers can engage in parafoveal preprocessing of upcoming words that lie outside of central vision (i.e., the upcoming text starting from 1° of visual angle away from the point of gaze), and (2) eye-movement behavior in reading represents a "hedged bet" that enough lexical processing will be complete by the time the eyes move to successfully identify the word. ...

Reference:

Parafoveal N400 effects reveal that word skipping is associated with deeper lexical processing in the presence of context-driven expectations
Word skipping in reading: On the interplay of linguistic and visual factors

European Journal of Cognitive Psychology

... While the animacy manipulation employed in Frazier et al. (2015) is a valid operationalization of blameworthiness, targeting the agency component of blame attribution (e.g., Guglielmo & Malle, 2017;Lagnado & Channon, 2008), and the authors conducted a norming study showing that animate referents are perceived as more causally responsible than inanimate referents, it does not convincingly demonstrate on its own that the observed effect is due to changes in blameworthiness resulting from different levels of animacy. This is partly due to a well-established cross-linguistic processing bias favoring animate referents across various linguistic phenomena, such as relative clause attachment (e.g., Desmet et al., 2006;Mak et al., 2002), anaphora resolution and production (e.g., Fukumura & Van Gompel, 2011;Vogels et al., 2014) and differential object marking (e.g., Aissen, 2003;Naess, 2007) due to increased bottom-up salience 4 (stimulus-driven salience that draws attention to a referent). ...

Relative clause attachment in Dutch: On-line comprehension corresponds to corpus frequencies when lexical variables are taken into account

... Previous work has found that expectationbased models provide a good fit against human data: across a variety of different structural contexts, expectation-based models correctly predict in which sentences, and where in those sentences, comprehenders will experience processing difficulty (e.g., Demberg and Keller, 2008;Levy, 2008;Boston et al., 2011;Frank and Bod, 2011;Frank et al., 2015). This includes-sometimes complex-interactions between cues that require additional explanations under the linguistic account (we provide examples in Section "Previous Work on Argument interpretation"), as well as qualitative differences in the effects of the same cue across languages (MacWhinney et al., 1984;MacWhinney and Bates, 1989;Desmet et al., 2002Desmet et al., , 2006Acuña-Fariña et al., 2009). This ability to correctly predict the data is particularly noteworthy since the expectationbased account is more parsimonious than the linguistic account: the expectation-based account allows linguistic cues to affect argument interpretation only to the extent that these cues affect the relative probability of different argument interpretations. ...

The correspondence between sentence production and corpus frequencies in modifier attachment

The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A