Colin G. Tredoux’s research while affiliated with University of Cape Town and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (7)


Identification parades in South Africa — Time for a change?
  • Article

January 2024

·

51 Reads

·

1 Citation

South African Law Journal

Colin G Tredoux

·

·

Aldred Allan

·

Identification parades are essential when obtaining evidence of identity from eyewitnesses. Eyewitnesses are shown a line of people containing the suspect(s) and innocent fillers, and witnesses are asked to point out the perpetrator(s) of the crime, noting that the perpetrator(s) might not be present. Corporeal (‘live’) parades are required in South Africa unless there is a good reason not to use them, in which case the police may use photograph parades. We review the rules for conducting parades in South Africa and compare these to those in several other countries, many of which no longer use corporeal parades. We consider evidence from empirical studies that have tested the ‘live superiority’ hypothesis and conclude that there is no clear evidence in its favour, notwithstanding that there are benefits to augmenting static views of faces with additional cues to identity. We then consider the logistical and financial cost of conducting live parades, which we find to be considerable. We conclude that it may well be time to reconsider the use of live identification parades in South Africa but caution that this should coincide with a review of the law regulating the use of alternative methods to ensure that accused persons receive fair trials.


Estimation of Eyewitness Error Rates in Fair and Biased Lineups
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

August 2023

·

58 Reads

·

10 Citations

Objective: The risk of mistaken identification for innocent suspects in lineups can be estimated by correcting the overall error rate by the number of people in the lineup. We compared this nominal size correction to a new effective size correction, which adjusts the error rate for the number of plausible lineup members. Hypotheses: We hypothesized that (a) increasing lineup bias would increase misidentifications of a designated innocent suspect; (b) with the effective size correction, increasing lineup bias would also increase the estimate of innocent-suspect misidentifications; and (c) with the nominal size correction, lineup bias would have no effect on the estimate of innocent-suspect misidentifications. Method: In a reanalysis of previous literature, we obtained 10 data sets from Open Science Framework. In three new experiments (Ns = 686, 405, and 1,531, respectively), participants observed a staged crime and completed a fair or biased lineup. Results: In the reanalysis of previous literature, less than four of six lineup members were identified frequently enough to be classified as plausible, M = 3.78, 95% confidence interval [CI: 2.20, 5.36]. In the new experiments, increasing lineup bias increased mistaken identifications of a designated innocent suspect, odds ratio (OR) = 5.50, 95% CI [2.77, 10.95] and also increased the effective size-corrected estimate of innocent-suspect misidentifications, OR = 3.04, 95% CI [2.13, 4.33]. With the nominal size correction, lineup bias had no effect on the estimate of innocent-suspect misidentifications, OR = 0.84, 95% CI [0.60, 1.18]. Conclusions: Most lineups include a combination of plausible and implausible lineup members. Contrary to the nominal size correction, which ignores implausible lineup members, the effective size correction is sensitive to implausible lineup members and accounts for lineup bias when estimating the risk of innocent suspect misidentifications.

View access options

Police trainees versus laypeople: Identification performance and confidence–accuracy relationship for facial and body lineups

May 2023

·

45 Reads

·

3 Citations

Applied Cognitive Psychology

·

Anna K. Geisendörfer

·

·

[...]

·

Melanie Sauerland

Court instructions and public perception endorse that eyewitness evidence provided by police should weight more heavily than laypeople's in court. Evidence is inconsistent. The current experiment provides a nuanced analysis of identification performance of police and laypeople at different levels of confidence. Laypeople and advanced police trainees ( N = 192) viewed portrait, profile, and body‐only lineups for central and peripheral targets. Police trainees displayed higher hit and correct rejection rates than laypeople for portrait lineups, and higher correct rejection rates in profile lineups for central targets. Calibration was similar for both groups, although police trainees had an advantage at low target presence base rates. Calibration was best for central targets' portrait and profile lineups. Participants displayed poor calibration and strong overconfidence for body‐only lineups and peripheral target lineups. We conclude that experience and specialization of police might be important when investigating a possible superiority of police who serve as eyewitness.


Social contact, own‐group recognition bias and visual attention to faces

October 2022

·

73 Reads

·

8 Citations

British Journal of Psychology

The own‐group recognition bias (OGRB) has been consistently linked to social contact in theoretical accounts. Indeed, social contact is assumed by most authors to underlie the perceptual expertise of out‐groups' faces. However, little is known empirically about how it might impact face‐processing strategies. We tested the proposition that social interaction would improve the face recognition performance of another group by modulating visual strategies for different face areas. In Experiment 1, we studied visual processes using an eye tracker during a person's first live encounter with a particular member of their own group (European) or an outgroup (African) to explore how increasing familiarity during a first interaction influences face‐processing strategies. In Experiments 2 and 3, we explored the effect of simulated intergroup contact on face recognition accuracy, while simultaneously studying the impact of contact on visual attention strategies that occur during recognition (Experiment 2) and encoding (Experiment 3). The results showed a strong OGRB and a difference in visual processes based on the ethnic group of the targets. Although a single interaction is not sufficient to reduce the OGRB, familiarization during a live interaction (Experiment 1) and virtual social contact (Experiment 2) had an impact on the visual strategies employed.


Automatic recognition, elimination strategy and familiarity feeling: Cognitive processes predict accuracy from lineup identifications

February 2022

·

126 Reads

·

3 Citations

Consciousness and Cognition

Identifying a suspect is critical for successful criminal investigations. Research focused on two decision processes during lineup identification, namely ‘automatic recognition’ and ‘elimination’ strategy, and their relation to identification accuracy. In this article, we report two experiments conducted in France and South Africa, which further examine strategies in eyewitness decision-making. We first used a modified-RSA (Retrospective Self Awareness; Kassin, 1985) interview method to construct questionnaires to have finer-grained measures of cognitive processes involved in lineup identification. Studies 1 and 2 tested the relevance of the questionnaire in each of the countries, and factor analysis yielded three common factors among the countries, namely the expected ‘automatic recognition’, and ‘elimination’ strategies, as well as an additional familiarity related factor. Logistic regressions showed that witnesses who reported using a familiarity feeling and/or an ‘elimination strategy’ to conduct their decision, were less likely to make correct decisions than witnesses who reported using ‘automatic recognition’.



Citations (5)


... Given such obstacles, I am hesitant to attempt to be precise about what makes an explanation 'good'. 7 For reviews, see, for example, Dubelaar (2014); Ross et al., (2014);Wise et al., (2014). ...

Reference:

Eyewitness evaluation through inference to the best explanation
Evaluating Eyewitness Testimony of Adults

... Although confidence might imply accuracy, it certainly does not guarantee it. In fact, some recent work estimates high-confidence accuracy to be lower than what was observed by Wixted and Wells (2017) (Fitzgerald et al., 2023) especially when viewing conditions are poor (Giacona et al., 2021). Furthermore, only a fraction of witnesses who encounter culprit-present lineups identify the culprit with high confidence (e.g., Palmer et al., 2013;Quigley-McBride & Wells, 2023;Wells, Yang et al., 2015). ...

Estimation of Eyewitness Error Rates in Fair and Biased Lineups

... We aimed for these numbers, but the nature of our ad hoc sample also determined the number of people that we could test at different ages. Following earlier work (Sauerland et al., 2018;Tupper et al., 2023), we later decided to conduct Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE), instead of chi-square analyses because this regression model is a more appropriate method for examining the relationship between a binary outcome and one or more within-subject variables as predictors (Hanley et al., 2003). Because the assumptions that need to be made for a credible power analysis for a GEE model are hard to make in an objectively justifiable way, we decided to base our sample size on the power analysis for the simpler chi-square model, outlined above. ...

Police trainees versus laypeople: Identification performance and confidence–accuracy relationship for facial and body lineups
  • Citing Article
  • May 2023

Applied Cognitive Psychology

... Typically, participants are less likely to recognize previously seen otherrace faces, and more likely to falsely indicate recognition for previously unseen other-race faces (Meissner & Brigham, 2001). This decreased recognition performance of other-race faces reflects that observers use different face processing strategies when processing out-group vs. in-group faces (Anzures et al., 2023;Brunet et al., 2023;Ficco et al., 2023), probably as a consequence of expertise and social categorization (Chen et al., 2023;Richler & Gauthier, 2014). Therefore, it is no surprise that cross-race bias appears to be greater for the majority than the minority members of a community (Hancock & Rhodes, 2008;Walker & Hewstone, 2006). ...

Social contact, own‐group recognition bias and visual attention to faces

British Journal of Psychology

... Eyewitness accuracy has also been reliably associated with how long an eyewitness takes to make their decision (Brewer et al., 2006;Dunning & Peretta, 2002;Nyman et al., 2019;Quigley-McBride & Wells, 2023;Seale-Carlisle et al., 2019;Sporer, 1992;Weber, et al., 2004) and their decision process (Dunning & Stern, 1994;Kneller, Memon, & Stevenage, 2001;Ross et al., 2007;Smith, Stinson, & Prosser, 2004;Wittwer et al., 2022). Other potential reflector variables are less well established: memory for fillers (Charman & Cahill, 2012), metamemory (Saraiva et al., 2020a;2020b), response bias (Baldassari, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2019), and (perhaps) measurements of individual differences such as face identification ability (Bindemann et al., 2012) 1 . ...

Automatic recognition, elimination strategy and familiarity feeling: Cognitive processes predict accuracy from lineup identifications
  • Citing Article
  • February 2022

Consciousness and Cognition