Claire N. Spottiswoode’s research while affiliated with University of Cambridge and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (143)


Highly virulent avian brood-parasitic species show elevated embryonic metabolic rates at specific incubation stages compared to less virulent and non- parasitic species
  • Article
  • Full-text available

September 2024

·

42 Reads

Biology Letters

·

Jess Lund

·

·

[...]

·

As the avian embryo grows and develops within the egg, its metabolic rate gradually increases. Obligate avian brood-parasitic birds lay their eggs in the nests of other species to avoid the costs of parental care, and all but one of these brood-parasitic species are altricial at hatching. Yet the chicks of some altricial brood-parasitic species perform the physically demanding task of evicting, stabbing or otherwise killing host progeny within days of hatching. This implies a need for high metabolic rates in the embryo, just as precocial species require. Using flow-through respirometry in situ, we investigated embryonic metabolic rates in diverse avian brood parasite lineages which either kill host offspring (high virulence) or share the nest with host young (low virulence). High-virulence brood parasite embryos exhibited higher overall metabolic rates than both non-parasitic (parental) species and low-virulence parasites. This was driven by significantly elevated metabolic rates around the halfway point of incubation. Additionally, a fine-scale analysis of the embryos of a host-parasitic pair showed faster increases in metabolic rates in the parasite. Together these results suggest that the metabolic patterns of the embryos of high-virulence parasites facilitate their early-life demands.

Download

Location and extent of the South East Africa Montane Archipelago (SEAMA) showing core sites in red, and an outline boundary of the convex hull of the ecoregion (created using QGIS version 3.28.12 LTR https://qgis.org/en/site/).
Contemporary macro-climatology of the South East Africa Montane Archipelago. Also labelled is the Eastern Arc Mountains (EAM) ecoregion to the north, and the Eastern Highlands of Zimbabwe to the southwest (EHZ). Annual rainfall (a) is measured in mm. Rainfall seasonality (b) is the coefficient of variation across months⁶⁵. Aridity index (c) is the ratio of annual rainfall to potential evapotranspiration⁶⁶ with values above 0.65 considered humid⁶⁷. Maximum water deficit (d) is a measure of water stress defined across the most arid months of the year, with lower values conducive of potentially dense, evergreen canopy structure⁶⁸. Bioclimatic layers were extracted from WorldClim version2 (https://www.worldclim.com/version2) and presented in QGIS version 3.28.12 LTR (https://qgis.org/en/site/).
Comparative ecoregion endemism according to the species-area function. Within each taxonomic group and ecoregion, we plot the number of endemics divided by Az, where A is the core area (ha) of the ecoregion and z = 0.25 is taken as a representative value of the slope of the species-area curve. This yields a metric of endemism to estimate the number of endemic species per hectare, which diminishes the impact of larger areas.
Relative survey effort across core sites in the SEAMA. Bars plot a comparative measure of sampling intensity, relative to the area of each site⁷⁵. Score varies between 0 and 1, where 0 is the minimum (no sampling at all) while 1 is the maximum effort possible to obtain.
Examples of SEAMA endemics. (a) Rhinolophus mabuensis (AM), (b) Chamaetylas choloensis (JB), (c) Nothophryne inagoensis (WC), (d) Atheris mabuensis (WRB), (e) Epamera malaikae (TCEC), f) Rhampholeon maspictus (JB), (g) Nadzikambia baylissi (WRB), (h) Maritonautes namuliensis (JB), (i) Euphorbia mlanjeana (ID), (j) Widdringtonia whytei (JB), (k) Encephalartos gratus (JB).
A biogeographical appraisal of the threatened South East Africa Montane Archipelago ecoregion

March 2024

·

683 Reads

·

6 Citations

Recent biological surveys of ancient inselbergs in southern Malawi and northern Mozambique have led to the discovery and description of many species new to science, and overlapping centres of endemism across multiple taxa. Combining these endemic taxa with data on geology and climate, we propose the ‘South East Africa Montane Archipelago’ (SEAMA) as a distinct ecoregion of global biological importance. The ecoregion encompasses 30 granitic inselbergs reaching > 1000 m above sea level, hosting the largest (Mt Mabu) and smallest (Mt Lico) mid-elevation rainforests in southern Africa, as well as biologically unique montane grasslands. Endemic taxa include 127 plants, 45 vertebrates (amphibians, reptiles, birds, mammals) and 45 invertebrate species (butterflies, freshwater crabs), and two endemic genera of plants and reptiles. Existing dated phylogenies of endemic animal lineages suggests this endemism arose from divergence events coinciding with repeated isolation of these mountains from the pan-African forests, together with the mountains’ great age and relative climatic stability. Since 2000, the SEAMA has lost 18% of its primary humid forest cover (up to 43% in some sites)—one of the highest deforestation rates in Africa. Urgently rectifying this situation, while addressing the resource needs of local communities, is a global priority for biodiversity conservation.


Culturally determined interspecies communication between humans and honeyguides

December 2023

·

128 Reads

·

5 Citations

Science

Species interactions that vary across environments can create geographical mosaics of genetic coevolution. However, traits mediating species interactions are sometimes culturally inherited. Here we show that traditions of interspecies communication between people and wild birds vary in a culturally determined geographical mosaic. Honey hunters in different parts of Africa use different calls to communicate with greater honeyguides ( Indicator indicator ) that lead them to bees’ nests. We show experimentally that honeyguides in Tanzania and Mozambique discriminate among honey hunters’ calls, responding more readily to local than to foreign calls. This was not explained by variation in sound transmission and instead suggests that honeyguides learn local human signals. We discuss the forces stabilizing and diversifying interspecies communication traditions, and the potential for cultural coevolution between species.


Guides and cheats: producer–scrounger dynamics in the human–honeyguide mutualism

November 2023

·

43 Reads

·

2 Citations

Foraging animals commonly choose whether to find new food (as ‘producers’) or scavenge from others (as ‘scroungers’), and this decision has ecological and evolutionary consequences. Understanding these tactic decisions is particularly vital for naturally occurring producer–scrounger systems of economic importance, because they determine the system's productivity and resilience. Here, we investigate how individuals' traits predict tactic decisions, and the consistency and pay-offs of these decisions, in the remarkable mutualism between humans (Homo sapiens) and greater honeyguides (Indicator indicator). Honeyguides can either guide people to bees’ nests and eat the resulting beeswax (producing), or scavenge beeswax (scrounging). Our results suggest that honeyguides flexibly switched tactics, and that guiding yielded greater access to the beeswax. Birds with longer tarsi scrounged more, perhaps because they are more competitive. The lightest females rarely guided, possibly to avoid aggression, or because genetic matrilines may affect female body mass and behaviour in this species. Overall, aspects of this producer–scrounger system probably increase the productivity and resilience of the associated human–honeyguide mutualism, because the pay-offs incentivize producing, and tactic-switching increases the pool of potential producers. Broadly, our findings suggest that even where tactic-switching is prevalent and producing yields greater pay-offs, certain phenotypes may be predisposed to one tactic.


Changes in complexity across 50 years in cuckoo finches and prinias
Change in complexity over time in (a) cuckoo finches and (b) prinias. Years are defined as number of years after 1970 (such that Year 0 corresponds to 1970 and Year 50 to 2020).
Changes in egg pattern complexity over time
a, Randomly selected host (above) and parasitic (below) eggs, from the historical (left) and current (right) samples. b, Changes in egg pattern complexity (log-transformed) over time in (1) parasites (n = 162 biologically independent eggs) and (2) hosts (n = 414 biologically independent eggs). Boxes range from the 25th to the 75th percentile and horizontal lines represent medians. Minima and maxima are defined by the smallest datapoint no lower than the 25th percentile minus 1.5× interquartile range (IQR) and the largest datapoint no greater than the 75th percentile plus 1.5× IQR, respectively. All datapoints are shown as dots. c, Mimetic fidelity through time: no significant change in differences in log(complexity) between all historical (n = 2,788) and current (n = 42,496) pairs of parasites and hosts These pairs were generated from the 162 parasitic eggs and 414 host eggs photographed. Boxes range from the 25th to the 75th percentile and horizontal lines represent medians. Minima and maxima are defined by the smallest datapoint no lower than the 25th percentile minus 1.5× IQR and the largest datapoint no greater than the 75th percentile plus 1.5× IQR, respectively. Outliers are shown as dots. Individual points are excluded as they obscure the boxplot. Bird illustrations reproduced with permission from faansiepeacock.com.
Chase-away evolution maintains imperfect mimicry in a brood parasite–host system despite rapid evolution of mimics

October 2023

·

70 Reads

·

1 Citation

Nature Ecology & Evolution

We studied a brood parasite–host system (the cuckoo finch Anomalospiza imberbis and its host, the tawny-flanked prinia Prinia subflava) to test (1) the fundamental hypothesis that deceptive mimics evolve to resemble models, selecting in turn for models to evolve away from mimics (‘chase-away evolution’) and (2) whether such reciprocal evolution maintains imperfect mimicry over time. Over only 50 years, parasites evolved towards hosts and hosts evolved away from parasites, resulting in no detectible increase in mimetic fidelity. Our results reflect rapid adaptive evolution in wild populations of models and mimics and show that chase-away evolution in models can counteract even rapid evolution of mimics, resulting in the persistence of imperfect mimicry.


When perfection isn't enough: host egg signatures are an effective defence against high-fidelity African cuckoo mimicry

July 2023

·

76 Reads

·

3 Citations

Most mimicry systems involve imperfect mimicry, whereas perfect and high-fidelity mimicry are rare. When the fidelity of mimicry is high, mimics might be expected to have the upper hand against their antagonists. However, in coevolving systems, diversification of model phenotypes may provide an evolutionary escape, because mimics cannot simultaneously match all model individuals in the population. Here we investigate high-fidelity mimicry in a highly specialized, Afrotropical brood parasite–host system: the African cuckoo and fork-tailed drongo. Specifically, we test whether host egg polymorphisms are an effective defence against such mimicry. We show, using a combination of image analysis, field experiments and simulations, that: (1) egg colour and pattern mimicry of fork-tailed drongo eggs by African cuckoos is near-perfect on average; (2) drongos show fine-tuned rejection of foreign eggs, exploiting unpredictable pattern differences between parasitic eggs and their own; and (3) the high degree of interclutch variation (polymorphic egg ‘signatures’) exhibited by drongos gives them the upper hand in the arms race, with 93.7% of cuckoo eggs predicted to be rejected, despite cuckoos mimicking the full range of drongo egg phenotypes. These results demonstrate that model diversification is a highly effective defence against mimics, even when mimicry is highly accurate.


Figure 1. (a) Three tawny-flanked prinia eggs laid by the same female, illustrating consistency in
Figure 3. Consistency within clutches is negatively correlated with distinctiveness between clutches
Repeatable randomness, invariant properties, and the design of biological signatures of identity

July 2023

·

118 Reads

·

1 Citation

Evolution

What makes a perfect signature? Optimal signatures should be consistent within individuals and distinctive between individuals. In defence against avian brood parasitism, some host species have evolved 'signatures' of identity on their eggs, comprising inter-individual variation in colour and pattern. Tawny-flanked prinia (Prinia subflava) egg signatures facilitate recognition and rejection of parasitic cuckoo finch (Anomalospiza imberbis) eggs. Here, we show that consistency and distinctiveness of pattern are negatively correlated in prinia eggs, perhaps because non-random, repeatable pattern generation mechanisms increase consistency but limit distinctiveness. We hypothesise that pattern properties which are repeatable within individuals but random between individuals ('invariant properties') allow hosts to circumvent this trade-off. To find invariant properties, we develop a method to quantify entire egg phenotypes from images taken from different perspectives. We find that marking scale (a fine-grained measure of size), but not marking orientation or position, is an invariant property in prinias. Hosts should therefore use differences in marking scale in egg recognition, but instead field experiments show that these differences do not predict rejection of conspecific eggs by prinias. Overall, we show that invariant properties allow consistency and distinctiveness to coexist, yet receiver behaviour is not optimally tuned to make use of this information.


(a) Locations of 11 communities in which we interviewed honey‐hunters. (b) A plot indicating, per community: the percentage (%) of interviewees that believed in badger‐honeyguide cooperation; the percentage (%) of interviewees that reported having seen badgers and honeyguides interact; a balloon plot indicating the relative frequency of interviewees that had never seen the species interacting, that had only seen them interacting at a bees' nest, or had (also) seen them interacting away from a bees' nest. Balloon sizes indicate the proportion of interviewees from each group that provided each response. Sample sizes are given next to each community name.
The six key steps of the hypothetical badger‐honeyguide cooperation. Beneath each step, we summarize the likelihood that a given step ever occurs in the wild based on available evidence and feasibility. For citations and interview responses that support these summaries, see the corresponding sections 1–6 of the main text. Illustration by Emma Wood.
Maps of the African ranges of (a) Honeybees (Apis mellifera; Requier et al., 2019), (b) honey badgers (Mellivora capensis; Skinner & Smithers, 1990), (c) greater honeyguides (Indicator indicator; BirdLife International, 2022), and (d) locations of cultural groups in which honey‐hunting with greater honeyguides has been the subject of scientific research. Guiding behaviour is commonplace in these areas and could extend to badgers as well as humans. (i) Boran community in Kenya (Isack & Reyer, 1989); (ii) Hadzabe, Maasai, “Ndorobo” and Sonjo communities in Tanzania (Laltaika, 2021; Wood et al., 2014), (iii) Awer community in Kenya (van der Wal, Gedi, et al., 2022) and (iv) Yao community in Mozambique (Spottiswoode et al., 2016). Photo credits: honeybees and honeyguide: D.L.C.; honey badger: C.M.B.; honey‐hunter: C.N.S.
Do honey badgers and greater honeyguide birds cooperate to access bees' nests? Ecological evidence and honey‐hunter accounts

June 2023

·

348 Reads

In parts of Africa, greater honeyguides (Indicator indicator) lead people to bees' nests, after which people harvest the honey, and make beeswax and larvae accessible to the honeyguide. In scientific and popular literature, a similar cooperative relationship is frequently described between honeyguides and honey badgers (Mellivora capensis), yet the evidence that this occurs is unclear. Such a partnership may have implications for the origins of our own species' cooperation with honeyguides and for the ecology and conservation of both honey badgers and honeyguides. Here, we review the evidence that honey badgers and honeyguides cooperate to access bees' nests, drawing from the published literature, from our own observations whilst studying both species, and by conducting 394 interviews with honey‐hunters in 11 communities across nine African countries. We find that the scientific evidence relies on incomplete and second‐hand accounts and does not convincingly indicate that the two species cooperate. The majority of honey‐hunters we interviewed were similarly doubtful about the interaction, but many interviewees in the Hadzabe, Maasai, and mixed culture communities in Tanzania reported having seen honey badgers and honeyguides interact, and think that they do cooperate. This complementary approach suggests that the most likely scenario is that the interaction does occur but is highly localized or extremely difficult to observe, or both. With substantial uncertainty remaining, we outline empirical studies that would clarify whether and where honeyguides and honey badgers cooperate, and emphasize the value of integrating scientific and cultural knowledge in ecology.


Eggshell composition and surface properties of avian brood-parasitic species compared with non-parasitic species

May 2023

·

114 Reads

·

3 Citations

The eggs of avian obligate brood-parasitic species have multiple adaptations to deceive hosts and optimize development in host nests. While the structure and composition of the eggshell in all birds is essential for embryo growth and protection from external threats, parasitic eggs may face specific challenges such as high microbial loads, rapid laying and ejection by the host parents. We set out to assess whether eggshells of avian brood-parasitic species have either (i) specialized structural properties, to meet the demands of a brood-parasitic strategy or (ii) similar structural properties to eggs of their hosts, due to the similar nest environment. We measured the surface topography (roughness), wettability (how well surfaces repel water) and calcium content of eggshells of a phylogenetically and geographically diverse range of brood-parasitic species (representing four of the seven independent lineages of avian brood-parasitic species), their hosts and close relatives of the parasites. These components of the eggshell structure have been demonstrated previously to influence such factors as the risk of microbial infection and overall shell strength. Within a phylogenetically controlled framework, we found no overall significant differences in eggshell roughness, wettability and calcium content between (i) parasitic and non-parasitic species, or (ii) parasitic species and their hosts. Both the wettability and calcium content of the eggs from brood-parasitic species were not more similar to those of their hosts' eggs than expected by chance. By contrast, the mean surface roughness of the eggs of brood-parasitic species was more similar to that of their hosts’ eggs than expected by chance, suggesting brood-parasitic species may have evolved to lay eggs that match the host nest environment for this trait. The lack of significant overall differences between parasitic and non-parasitic species, including hosts, in the traits we measured, suggests that phylogenetic signal, as well as general adaptations to the nest environment and for embryo development, outweigh any influence of a parasitic lifestyle on these eggshell properties.


Combined measures of mimetic fidelity explain imperfect mimicry in a brood parasite-host system

February 2023

·

64 Reads

·

5 Citations

The persistence of imperfect mimicry in nature presents a challenge to mimicry theory. Some hypotheses for the existence of imperfect mimicry make differing predictions depending on how mimetic fidelity is measured. Here, we measure mimetic fidelity in a brood parasite-host system using both trait-based and response-based measures of mimetic fidelity. Cuckoo finches Anomalospiza imberbis lay imperfectly mimetic eggs that lack the fine scribbling characteristic of eggs of the tawny-flanked prinia Prinia subflava, a common host species. A trait-based discriminant analysis based on Minkowski functionals-that use geometric and topological morphometric methods related to egg pattern shape and coverage-reflects this consistent difference between host and parasite eggs. These methods could be applied to quantify other phenotypes including stripes and waved patterns. Furthermore, by painting scribbles onto cuckoo finch eggs and testing their rate of rejection compared to control eggs (i.e. a response-based approach to quantify mimetic fidelity), we show that prinias do not discriminate between eggs based on the absence of scribbles. Overall, our results support relaxed selection on cuckoo finches to mimic scribbles, since prinias do not respond differently to eggs with and without scribbles, despite the existence of this consistent trait difference.


Citations (73)


... Among the most exciting developments in recent ecology are the recognition of a new ecoregion of global biological importance, the 'South East Africa Montane Archipelago' (SEAMA, Bayliss et al. 2024). This area, distinguished by its mid-elevation rainforests and biologically unique montane grasslands, is home to at least 127 endemic plants, 45 endemic vertebrates and 45 endemic invertebrates, including two endemic genera of plants and reptiles (Bayliss et al. 2024). ...

Reference:

How to Make Your African Ecology Paper Stand Out to a Global Audience
A biogeographical appraisal of the threatened South East Africa Montane Archipelago ecoregion

... In this unique interaction, honeyguides lead humans to honeybee (Apis mellifera) nests, with humans harvesting honey and bee larvae while the birds feed on beeswax. This relationship is facilitated by specialised communication sounds, with recent studies showing honeyguides responding preferentially to local signals, suggesting cultural coevolution with cultural process on both sides [18]. Another example is the common raven (Corvus corax) strategic association with grey wolves (Canis lupus), particularly in winter [19]. ...

Culturally determined interspecies communication between humans and honeyguides
  • Citing Article
  • December 2023

Science

... Examples Typically antagonistic or mutualistic References i. Species/populations Predator-prey coevolution Antagonistic (Nair et al., 2019) Parasite-host coevolution Antagonistic (Paterson et al., 2010) Brood parasite-host coevolution Antagonistic (Spottiswoode & Stevens, 2012;Dixit et al., 2023) Pollinator-plant coevolution Host-symbiont coevolution Mutualistic Either (Anderson & Johnson, 2008) (Charlat et al., 2007;Smith & Moran, 2020) ii. Classes of individual within a species/population Chase-away sexual selection Antagonistic (Arnqvist & Rowe, 2002;Holland & Rice, 1998) Parent-offspring coevolution Antagonistic (Kilner & Hinde, 2012) iii. ...

Chase-away evolution maintains imperfect mimicry in a brood parasite–host system despite rapid evolution of mimics

Nature Ecology & Evolution

... For example, Chrysococcyx spp. are often specialists on a single passerine genus ; whereas African cuckoo Cuculus gularis parasitises the nests of fork-tailed drongo Dicrurus adsimilis almost exclusively (Lund et al. 2023). On the other side of the spectrum, some generalist parasites can lay their eggs in the nest of several different host species: for instance M. ater can parasitise more than 40 species (Friedmann and Kiff 1985); and C. canorus uses up to 184 species as potential hosts (Yang et al. 2020a). ...

When perfection isn't enough: host egg signatures are an effective defence against high-fidelity African cuckoo mimicry

... The eggshell strength of avian obligate brood parasites is higher than that of their respective hosts (López et al., 2021), which have focused on eggshell thickness, egg shape or heteromorphic deviations (Spaw and Rohwer 1987;Brooker and Brooker 1991;Picman and Pribil 1997;Spottiswoode and Colebrook-Robjent 2007;Spottiswoode, 2010;McClelland et al., 2023), microhardness (Igic et al., 2011), or microstructure (Soler et al., 2019;López et al., 2023). Furthermore, Spottiswoode (2010) reported on the eggshell strength of common cuckoo eggs compared to that of other host eggs. ...

Eggshell composition and surface properties of avian brood-parasitic species compared with non-parasitic species

... palatable species that mimic the appearance of unpalatable model species to deter predation attempts on themselves. The presence of similar noticeable bright colours between models and mimics is widespread [4][5][6][7][8] and found in a wide range of organisms, including, but not limited to, fish, poison frogs, and snakes [9][10][11][12][13][14]. Stevens and Ruxton [15], however, highlight a gap in the understanding of how pattern per se, or the spatial positioning of colour patches across a colour pattern, affects mimicry and why mimics do not always perfectly resemble their models [13,[16][17][18][19][20]. ...

Combined measures of mimetic fidelity explain imperfect mimicry in a brood parasite-host system

... Indeed, studies by Welbergen & Davies [42] and Davies & Welbergen [43] indicated that the artificial dummy of the cuckoo grey morph, which mimics the sparrowhawk, experienced reduced mobbing by the reed warbler hosts. However, experimental research on GRWs and other more aggressive hosts suggested that this trait offers cuckoos minimal protection from host contact attacks because the hosts are able to distinguish cuckoos from sparrowhawks [65,66] (see also [41,67] for similar results in Oriental reed warblers (Acrocephalus orientalis) and fork-tailed drongos (Dicrurus adsimilis), respectively) but GRWs cannot discriminate between kestrel and rufous cuckoo [66]. The hypothesis of rufous cuckoo mimicry of kestrels has been questioned by several studies, primarily owing to significant morphological differences between the two species [68]. ...

Aggressive hosts are undeterred by a cuckoo's hawk mimicry, but probably make good foster parents

... During the honey-hunt and subsequent harvest, the behaviour of both the human and honeyguide are conspicuous (typically involving reciprocal calling, the felling or opening of a tree trunk with an axe, and the use of fire and smoke to subdue the bees [30,31]), and the resulting resource can be large and difficult to monopolize. Consequently, scrounging honeyguides arrive after the human has left and scavenge on beeswax without having invested in locating the bees' nest and guiding the human [32]. The productivity of the human-honeyguide mutualism, which has significant ecological, economic and cultural importance [29,30,[32][33][34] is therefore strongly influenced by the outcome of the producer-scrounger game among honeyguides. ...

When wax wanes: competitors for beeswax stabilize rather than jeopardize the honeyguide-human mutualism

... Individual prinias lay eggs with distinct colour and pattern phenotypes (egg signatures; Fig. 1a), such that a given cuckoo finch egg will be a poor match to most prinia clutches in the population 6 . Cuckoo finch eggs (mimics) exhibit simpler patterns than prinia eggs (models) and differences in pattern complexity predict egg rejection by prinias 7 . Egg rejection therefore has fitness consequences for both hosts and parasites and this implies that selection should favour parasites evolving towards hosts (that is, evolving increased complexity) and hosts evolving away from parasites (that is, also evolving increased complexity). ...

Visual complexity of egg patterns predicts egg rejection according to Weber's law

... In recent years, there has been increased emphasis on mutualistic interactions between humans and wild (nondomestic) species [1,2]. This is an important area of investigation; however, as currently defined, the concept excludes a number of important examples of possible mutualistic interactions while also ignoring the historical component of relationships between humans and more-than-human animals that involve niche construction and its impacts on species dynamics [3]. ...

The ecology and evolution of human‐wildlife cooperation