Christopher H. Parker’s research while affiliated with University of Utah and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (12)


Fire's impact on threat detection and risk perception among vervet monkeys: Implications for hominin evolution
  • Article

August 2020

·

63 Reads

·

6 Citations

Journal of Human Evolution

·

Christopher Parker

·

Earl Keefe

·

Kristen Hawkes

The spatial behavior of primates is shaped by many factors including predation risk, the distribution of food sources, and access to water. In fire-prone settings, burning is a catalyst of change, altering the distribution of both plants and animals. Recent research has shown that primates alter their behavior in response to this change. Here, we study primates' perceived threat of predation in fire-modified landscapes. We focus on the predator-related behaviors of vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) after controlled burning events. We compare the occurrence of vigilance and predator-deterrent behaviors, including alarm calls, scanning, and flight across different habitats and burn conditions to test the hypothesis that subjects exhibit fewer predator-specific vigilance and predator-deterrent behaviors in burned areas. The results demonstrate that predator-related behaviors occur less often in burned habitats, suggesting that predators are less common in these areas. These results provide foundations for examining hypotheses about the use of fire-altered landscapes among extinct hominins. We set these data in the context of increasing aridity, changes in burning regimes, and the emergence of pyrophilia in the human lineage.


Optimal Foraging Theory

September 2018

·

156 Reads

·

1 Citation

Optimal foraging theory is a set of concepts and models that aim to explain the wide variation in foraging strategies among individuals and between species, guided by the assumption that living things are designed by a history of natural selection. The use of foraging models in anthropology was prompted by the geographic and temporal variation in resource use among people who live on wild foods. Foraging models provide a theoretical basis for linking the variation both among and within populations to features of local ecology—with initially unexpected implications for what has happened in human evolution.


When to defend? Optimal territoriality across the Numic homeland

May 2018

·

34 Reads

·

15 Citations

Quaternary International

Explanations for the complex human decisions that lead to territoriality have focused on models of economic defendability. While powerful, these models fail to explain variation in territorial behavior in all cases. To help overcome this limitation, here we offer an approach that synthesizes the logic of economic defendability with a general optimality model from behavioral ecology: the marginal value theorem (MVT). Through the MVT we add considerations of how resource profitability, in addition to abundance, alters the amount of time individuals should spend foraging in order to meet caloric requirements, and how far they should travel between foraging locations. Where and when individuals rely on abundant, but lower profitability resources, they should spend more time foraging and less time traveling, which leads to smaller territories that are easier to defend, and wherein each unit of land has a greater economic value per capita thereby increasing the benefits of economic boundary defense. Numic foragers of the Great Basin and surrounding areas provide an illustrative case study to validate this general ecological model. The results show that individuals exploiting high density but low profitability resources live in smaller territories and have higher rates of resource privatization, thereby explaining societal differences between Northern Paiute and Shoshone bands. Importantly, because defended resource patches are the target of women's foraging, the payoffs to territoriality are likely structured by women's risk averse foraging decisions. We suggest this model is capable of explaining variation in hunter-gatherer territoriality worldwide.


Territorial Behavior in Foraging Populations
  • Poster
  • File available

March 2017

·

51 Reads

We utilized the Dyson-Hudson and Smith model of economic defensibility, combined with the Charnov patch choice in an effort to help explain the threshold for territorial behavior within foraging populations. We found that as foraging becomes more intensive and populations become more dense, there is a greater likelihood of both ownership of goods and defensive behavior.

Download

Fig. 2. Areas surrounding Parnngurr within Martu Native Title area (inset).  
Fig. 5. Plots and model fits summarizing results from the four main predictions. (a, P1) Number of site occupants as a function of site size ( ). (b, P2) Duration of active site occupation as a function of size sorting. (c, P3) Median monitor lizard harvest (kcals) as a function of site size and (d, P4) as a function of size sorting. Confidence intervals show the standard error (95%) of the model fit. See Table 1 for details.
of bivariate model results testing each of the four predictions.
Martu ethnoarchaeology: Foraging ecology and the marginal value of site structure

July 2016

·

369 Reads

·

19 Citations

Journal of Anthropological Archaeology

Brian F. Codding

·

·

·

[...]

·

Archaeological investigations of hunter-gatherer site structure have remained largely descriptive, despite significant explanatory advancements by evolutionary approaches to foraging behavior and ecology. To date, calls to incorporate site structure studies within this behavioral ecological framework have largely been ignored. We suggest there is a clear explanation for this. At large spatial extents, human behavior is constrained by patterned ecological variability, as such, a general theory of behavior is likely to characterize key aspects of human decisions. At small spatial extents, human behavior is not constrained by patterned ecological variability, therefore, the human decisions that produce site structure should be driven by mechanical constraints or random variation. However, variation in site structure may be ecologically relevant inasmuch as it informs on landscape level variation in human-environment interactions. Drawing on ethnoarchaeological data collected in collaboration with Martu, Aboriginal foragers in Western Australia, here we test empirically-derived, mechanistic predictions on site size and material size sorting to show how these can inform theoretically-derived, adaptive predictions from the Marginal Value Theorem. Results show that site size increases with the number of occupants and hence, the amount of in-patch foraging competition, while size sorting increases with the duration of occupation and hence, in-patch residence time. Combined, these attributes of site structure can be used as proxies of foraging behavior to explain variability in overall foraging yields. With this approach, site structure can provide insights into foraging decisions that can be examined through a general theory of behavior.


The pyrophilic primate hypothesis

March 2016

·

8,966 Reads

·

67 Citations

Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews

Members of genus Homo are the only animals known to create and control fire. The adaptive significance of this unique behavior is broadly recognized, but the steps by which our ancestors evolved pyrotechnic abilities remain unknown. Many hypotheses attempting to answer this question attribute hominin fire to serendipitous, even accidental, discovery. Using recent paleoenvironmental reconstructions, we present an alternative scenario in which, 2 to 3 million years ago in tropical Africa, human fire dependence was the result of adapting to progressively fire-prone environments. The extreme and rapid fluctuations between closed canopy forests, woodland, and grasslands that occurred in tropical Africa during that time, in conjunction with reductions in atmospheric carbon dioxide levels, changed the fire regime of the region, increasing the occurrence of natural fires. We use models from optimal foraging theory to hypothesize benefits that this fire-altered landscape provided to ancestral hominins and link these benefits to steps that transformed our ancestors into a genus of active pyrophiles whose dependence on fire for survival contributed to its rapid expansion out of Africa.


What's burning got to do with it? Primate foraging opportunities in fire-modified landscapes

October 2015

·

369 Reads

·

18 Citations

American Journal of Physical Anthropology

Objectives: Anecdotal and formal evidence indicate that primates take advantage of burned landscapes. However, little work has been done to quantify the costs and benefits of this behavior. Using systematic behavioral observations from a population of South African vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops pygerythrus), we evaluate differences in food availability and energetics before and after controlled burns altered vegetation near their home range. We aim to determine whether burned habitats offer improved foraging opportunities. Methods: We collected feeding data from foraging individuals and analyzed common plant foods for their energetic content. We then used the feeding and energetic data to calculate postencounter profitabilities and encounter rates for food types. Using negative binomial and mixed linear regression models we compared data from burned and unburned habitats. Results: Our results show significantly improved encounter rates in burned landscapes for two prey items, invertebrates and grasses. However, postencounter profitabilities in burned areas were not significantly different than those achieved in unburned areas. Conclusions: Results suggest that improved encounters alone can motivate changes in foraging behavior. These foraging benefits enable the exploitation of burned savanna habitats, likely driving postburn range expansions observed among populations of vervet monkeys. Thus quantified, these results may serve as a foundation for hypotheses regarding the evolution of fire-use in our own lineage. Am J Phys Anthropol, 2015. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Burning the Land: An Ethnographic Study of Off-Site Fire Use by Current and Historically Documented Foragers and Implications for the Interpretation of Past Fire Practices in the Landscape Author(s): Fulco Scherjon, Corrie Bakels, Katharine MacDonald, and Wil Roebroeks Source: Current Anthropology, Vol. 56, No. 3 (June 2015), pp. 299-326 Published by: The University of Chicago Press on behalf of Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research

June 2015

·

840 Reads

Current Anthropology


Burning the Land: An Ethnographic Study of Off-Site Fire Use by Current and Historically Documented Foragers and Implications for the Interpretation of Past Fire Practices in the Landscape Author(s): Fulco Scherjon, Corrie Bakels, Katharine MacDonald, and Wil Roebroeks Source: Current Anthropology, Vol. 56, No. 3 (June 2015), pp. 299-326 Published by: The University of Chicago Press on behalf of Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research

June 2015

·

143 Reads

Current Anthropology


Fire and Home Range Expansion: A Behavioral Response to Burning Among Savanna Dwelling Vervet Monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops)

August 2014

·

345 Reads

·

34 Citations

American Journal of Physical Anthropology

The behavioral adaptations of primates to fire-modified landscapes are of considerable interest to anthropologists because fire is fundamental to life in the African savanna-the setting in which genus Homo evolved. Here we report the behavioral responses of a savanna-dwelling primate, vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops), to fire-induced ecological change. Using behavioral and spatial data to characterize ranging patterns prior to and postburn and between burn and nonburn years, we show that these primates inhabiting small, spatially bound, riverine habitats take advantage of newly burned savanna landscapes. When subjects encountered controlled fires, they did not flee but instead avoided the path of the fire seemingly unbothered by its approach. After fire, the primates' home range expanded into newly burned but previously unused areas. These results contribute to understanding the response of non-human primates to fire-modified landscapes and can shed light on the nature and scope of opportunities and constraints posed by the emergence of fire-affected landscapes in the past. Results also expose deficiencies in our knowledge of fire-related behavioral responses in the primate lineage and highlight the need for further investigation of these responses as they relate to foraging opportunities, migration, resource use, and especially fire-centric adaptations in our own genus. Am J Phys Anthropol, 2014. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Citations (8)


... Human groups living near the Fongoli study area report burning for safety (improving ability to detect dangerous animals and insects as well as to create firebreaks), accessibility and visibility, and to reduce parasite loads (Mbow et al., 2000). Similarly, Fongoli chimpanzees and other savanna-dwelling primates appear to find burned areas easier and possibly safer to move through (Jaffe and Isbell 2009;Pruetz and Herzog 2017;Herzog et al., 2020). Profitabilities, as reflected in patch residence times, do appear to be impacted by burning, but the relationships between rates of encounter and profitability are complex. ...

Reference:

Investigating foundations for hominin fire exploitation: Savanna-dwelling chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in fire-altered landscapes
Fire's impact on threat detection and risk perception among vervet monkeys: Implications for hominin evolution
  • Citing Article
  • August 2020

Journal of Human Evolution

... This suggests that the sexual difference in net energy gain also occurred in the sandperch, with males gathering lower net energy than females. The phenomenon that the diet of males (foragingtime minimizers) was fairly close to that of females (foraging-time maximizers) can be explained using optimal foraging theory (Schoener 1971, Pyke et al. 1977, Pyke 1984, Stephens & Krebs 1986). Based on this theory, any individual should obtain optimal diet by maximizing the ratio of net energy input to foraging time. ...

Optimal Foraging Theory
  • Citing Article
  • September 2018

... Here we follow recent work suggesting territoriality and inequality may be correlated within human populations [30] and explore key resource characteristics that scholars have hypothesized (individually or through interaction) should promote territoriality and/or inequality through pay-offs for controlling access to resources. Specifically, we investigate resource predictability [1,16], abundance [1,31], heterogeneity [5,15,32,33], the economy of scale or Allee effect of resources [32, [34][35][36] and the monopolizability (or ability to control access) of resources patches [5,35,[37][38][39] which we summarize below. ...

When to defend? Optimal territoriality across the Numic homeland
  • Citing Article
  • May 2018

Quaternary International

... As an example of the predictive utility of this model, we first turn to an ethnographic case drawing on quantitative foraging data collected by the authors in collaboration with Martu, an Australian Aboriginal population who live in and have Native Title over their ancestral estates in Western Australia (Fig. 6.2) , 2010Bird et al., , 2009Bird et al., , 2013Bird et al., , 2016a, 2012aCodding et al., 2010Codding et al., , 2011Codding et al., , 2014Codding et al., , 2016aScelza et al., 2014;Scelza & Bliege Bird, 2008; Zeanah et al., 2015Zeanah et al., , 2017. We then briefly contrast these patterns with those reported by Jim O'Connell and Kristen Hawkes (1981Hawkes ( , 1984 who worked with Alyawarre populations in Central Australia (Fig. 6.2). ...

Martu ethnoarchaeology: Foraging ecology and the marginal value of site structure

Journal of Anthropological Archaeology

... There is currently much debate over the origin of hominin fire use and the definition of "control," with some arguing that taking advantage of naturally occurring landscape fires (e.g., from lightning strikes) may have occurred prior to other types of "control" (e.g., Chazan, 2017;Parker et al., 2016;Pruetz & Herzog, 2017). Archaeological evidence for hominin fire use suggests deep roots for the practice, as early as 1 million years ago (Ma) (Berna et al., 2012) or even 1.5 Ma (Barbetti, 1986;Bellomo, 1994;Brain, 1993;Brain & Sillent, 1988;Glikson, 2013;Gowlett et al., 1981;Rowlett, 2000). ...

The pyrophilic primate hypothesis

Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews

... Recently burned areas are attractive for many herbivores because the increased visibility makes it easier to avoid predators and the new vegetation cover contains a higher nutrient level; these freshly burned areas also support hunting opportunities for some birds and insects (e.g. Allred et al., 2011;Bird et al., 2008;Eby et al., 2014;Herzog et al., 2016;Komarek, 1969;Mellars, 1976;Reid, 2012). People were then able to hunt prey animals attracted to recently burned areas (Scherjon et al., 2015). ...

What's burning got to do with it? Primate foraging opportunities in fire-modified landscapes

American Journal of Physical Anthropology

... Fire is fundamental for the living organisms in the African savannah (Herzog et al., 2014). These extremely dynamic, coupled socioecological ecosystems are shaped by a long history of interaction with humans, fire, climate and wildlife (Marchant, 2010). ...

Fire and Home Range Expansion: A Behavioral Response to Burning Among Savanna Dwelling Vervet Monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops)

American Journal of Physical Anthropology

... If so, are there gender biases associated with burning activities as there are in other aspects of the Hadza foraging economy (e.g., Crittenden et al., 2013;Lew-Levy et al., 2020;Wood et al., 2014)? If gender biases in landscape burning do exist, are they consistent with reports from other hunter-gatherer groups (e.g., Bird et al., 2005;? Are the motivations for intentionally burning the landscape consistent with published accounts of landscape burning among other groups of hunter-gatherers? ...

Aboriginal Burning Regimes and Hunting Strategies in Australia’s Western Desert

Human Ecology