Chip Heath’s research while affiliated with Stanford University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (31)


Reflections on Self-Reflection: Contemplating Flawed Self-Judgments in the Clinic, Classroom, and Office Cubicle
  • Article

March 2018

·

143 Reads

·

13 Citations

Perspectives on Psychological Science

·

Chip Heath

·

Jerry M. Suls

We reflect back on our 2004 monograph reviewing the implications of faulty self-judgment for health, education, and the workplace. The review proved popular, no doubt because the importance of accurate self-assessment is best reflected in just how broad the literature is that touches on this topic. We discuss opportunities and challenges to be found in the future study of self-judgment accuracy and error, and suggest that designing interventions aimed at improving self-judgments may prove to be a worthwhile but complex and nuanced task.


The Repetition-Break Plot Structure Makes Effective Television Advertisements

September 2011

·

202 Reads

·

22 Citations

Journal of Marketing

The plot structure in television advertisements can enhance consumers' brand attitudes and foster increasing consumer and industry recognition. A corpus analysis of contemporary television advertisements shows that advertisements using the repetition-break plot structure are a small percentage of television advertisements but a large percentage of Clio and Effie award-winning advertisements. They are also likely to attain postings and views on YouTube. Three experiments using television advertisements from contemporary brands show that repetitionbreak advertisements are persuasive, leading to more favorable brand attitudes and greater purchase intentions than similar plot structures and that this effect is attributable in part to the advertisements being more engaging. Thus, a theoretically explainable and generic plot structure yields effective advertisements. The result is a new and flexible tool for marketing professionals to use to generate advertisements, with guidelines for when and why it should and should not be effective.


Common Ground and Cultural Prominence How Conversation Reinforces Culture

July 2009

·

291 Reads

·

70 Citations

Psychological Science

Why do well-known ideas, practices, and people maintain their cultural prominence in the presence of equally good or better alternatives? This article suggests that a social-psychological process whereby people seek to establish common ground with their conversation partners causes familiar elements of culture to increase in prominence, independently of performance or quality. Two studies tested this hypothesis in the context of professional baseball, showing that common ground predicted the cultural prominence of baseball players better than their performance, even though clear performance metrics are available in this domain. Regardless of performance, familiar players, who represented common ground, were discussed more often than lesser-known players, both in a dyadic experiment (Study 1) and in natural discussions on the Internet (Study 2). Moreover, these conversations mediated the positive link between familiarity and a more institutionalized measure of prominence: All-Star votes (Study 2). Implications for research on the psychological foundations of culture are discussed.


FIGURE 1. A typical Prospect Theory value function  
TablE 1 . Summary Statistics for agreement in round 1, No agreement, round of agreement, and Pay- ment for dyad by goal Condition (all measures are dyad level) goal Condition (dyad)
FIGURE 2. A Prospect Theory value function for goals of 0 and 30 units.
TablE 2 . gamble Options
FIGURE 3. Distribution of requests by proposers (top panel) and responders (bottom panel) in an ultimatum game (study 1).  

+1

Goal-Induced Risk Taking in Negotiation and Decision Making
  • Article
  • Full-text available

June 2009

·

4,420 Reads

·

114 Citations

Social Cognition

Three experiments test whether specific, challenging goals increase risk taking. We propose that goals serve as reference points, creating a region of perceived losses for outcomes below a goal (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Tversky & Kahneman, 1992). According to the Prospect Theory value function, decision makers become more risk seeking in the domain of losses. In all three experiments we compared a "do your best" condition with a "specific, challenging goal" condition. The goal condition consistently increased risky behavior in both negotiation and decision making tasks. The discussion considers how goals influence expectations, strategy choice, and unethical behavior.

Download

Fig. 1. Mean choices in the Weak Link Coordination Exercise in Study 2, as a function of round. Results are plotted separately for the four conditions: synchronous singing and moving; synchronous singing; asynchronous singing and moving; and no singing, no moving (control). Error bars indicate AE1 SEM. 
Fig. 2. Contributions to the public account in Study 3, as a function of round, plotted separately for the four conditions: synchronous singing and moving; synchronous singing; asynchronous singing and moving; and no singing, no moving (control). Error bars indicate AE SEM. 
Payoff Grid for the Weak Link Coordination Exercise Used in Studies 1 and 2
Synchrony and Cooperation

February 2009

·

6,453 Reads

·

1,342 Citations

Psychological Science

Armies, churches, organizations, and communities often engage in activities-for example, marching, singing, and dancing-that lead group members to act in synchrony with each other. Anthropologists and sociologists have speculated that rituals involving synchronous activity may produce positive emotions that weaken the psychological boundaries between the self and the group. This article explores whether synchronous activity may serve as a partial solution to the free-rider problem facing groups that need to motivate their members to contribute toward the collective good. Across three experiments, people acting in synchrony with others cooperated more in subsequent group economic exercises, even in situations requiring personal sacrifice. Our results also showed that positive emotions need not be generated for synchrony to foster cooperation. In total, the results suggest that acting in synchrony with others can increase cooperation by strengthening social attachment among group members.


The Repetition‐Break Plot Structure: A Cognitive Influence on Selection in the Marketplace of Ideas

January 2009

·

317 Reads

·

54 Citations

Cognitive Science A Multidisciplinary Journal

Using research into learning from sequences of examples, we generate predictions about what cultural products become widely distributed in the social marketplace of ideas. We investigate what we term the Repetition-Break plot structure: the use of repetition among obviously similar items to establish a pattern, and then a final contrasting item that breaks with the pattern to generate surprise. Two corpus studies show that this structure arises in about a third of folktales and story jokes. An experiment shows that jokes with this structure are more interesting than those without the initial repetition. Thus, we document evidence for how a cognitive factor influences the cultural products that are selected in the marketplace of ideas.


Who Drives Divergence? Identity Signaling, Outgroup Dissimilarity, and the Abandonment of Cultural Tastes

September 2008

·

477 Reads

·

472 Citations

People often diverge from members of other social groups: They select cultural tastes (e.g., possessions, attitudes, or behaviors) that distinguish them from outsiders and abandon tastes when outsiders adopt them. But while divergence is pervasive, most research on the propagation of culture is based on conformity. Consequently, it is less useful in explaining why people might abandon tastes when others adopt them. The 7 studies described in this article showed that people diverge to avoid signaling undesired identities. A field study, for example, found that undergraduates stopped wearing a particular wristband when members of the "geeky" academically focused dormitory next door started wearing them. Consistent with an identity-signaling perspective, the studies further showed that people often diverge from dissimilar outgroups to avoid the costs of misidentification. Implications for social influence, identity signaling, and the popularity and diffusion of culture are discussed.


Divergence in Cultural Practices: Tastes as Signals of Identity

January 2008

·

209 Reads

·

10 Citations

Divergence is a fact of social life; people select different tastes to distinguish themselves from others and they abandon tastes when others adopt them. But while we know a great deal about conformity, it predicts convergence, and thus is less equipped to explain why people diverge. We suggest people diverge to maintain clear signals of identity. Our approach emphasizes that the meaning of signals is set at a social rather than individual level. Tastes gain signal value through association with groups or types of individuals, but become diluted when members of more than one type hold them. Thus different types of people will diverge in the tastes they select, and they will abandon tastes they previously liked when they are adopted by members of other social types. (127 words)


Where Consumers Diverge from Others: Identity Signaling and Product Domains

August 2007

·

2,956 Reads

·

1,225 Citations

Journal of Consumer Research

We propose that consumers often make choices that diverge from those of others to ensure that they effectively communicate desired identities. Consistent with this identity-signaling perspective, four studies illustrate that consumers are more likely to diverge from majorities, or members of other social groups, in product domains that are seen as symbolic of identity (e.g., music or hairstyles, rather than backpacks or stereos). In identity domains, participants avoided options preferred by majorities and abandoned preferences shared with majorities. The social group associated with a product influenced choice more in identity domains and when a given product was framed as identity relevant. People diverge, in part, to avoid communicating undesired identities. (c) 2007 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH, Inc..



Citations (26)


... In a mediation, this often involves a retreat to positional bargaining, or responses and behaviours that are reactive, rigid and lacking in consideration of another's perspective. Further, when mentalizing is impaired, any judgements that parties make about their own motivations are likely to be prone to inaccuracy and self-benefiting cognitive biases (Dunning et al., 2018;Luyten, 2019;Luyten & Fonagy, 2015). ...

Reference:

Mediation 2.0: a mentalizing-informed framework for renewed purpose and practice
Reflections on Self-Reflection: Contemplating Flawed Self-Judgments in the Clinic, Classroom, and Office Cubicle
  • Citing Article
  • March 2018

Perspectives on Psychological Science

... However, they may be able to improve decision-makers' attention to feedback, by enhanced record-keeping or through activities explicitly aimed at learning from the past (Cyert & March, 1963, Friedman, 1998. Another approach is the use of policies to restrict decision-makers' freedom (Heath et al., 1998) with the goal of avoiding "noise chasing" (i.e., over-reacting to inconsistent feedback). Both of these approaches-learning programs and policy-based decisions-are ways to improve institutional memory, an adaptive response to environments with inconsistent feedback. ...

Cognitive repairs: How organizational practices can compensate for individual shortcomings
  • Citing Article
  • January 1998

Research in Organizational Behavior

... Consumers are inherently attracted to stories ( Van Laer et al. 2014), as they are easy to follow, entertaining, and engaging (Hansen and Kahnweiler 1993). They can generate positive affect and attitudes (Escalas, Moore, and Britton 2004), and they can enhance consumer engagement with brand communications (Loewenstein, Raghunathan, and Heath 2011). Further, when the characters are well-known and specified, as they are with rivals, consumers become more interested (Escalas, Moore, and Britton 2004;Van Laer et al. 2014), and the conflictcentered rivalry plot may be particularly compelling (Lengauer, Esser, and Berganza 2012). ...

The Repetition-Break Plot Structure Makes Effective Television Advertisements
  • Citing Article
  • September 2011

Journal of Marketing

... In the process whereby tastes gain 'signaling value' through their distinctiveness and costly adoption, cultural practices endowed with 'a functional component … [are] harder to interpret as [social] signals'. 118 Foodstuffs associated with such utilitarian objectives as nutrition or health are thus less likely to be coopted aesthetically in the contexts of cultural globalization and democratization, which have now deprived past exotic items, like the onceprestigious pineapple, of their 'social cachet'. 119 Culinary products of terroir, which can be detached from nutritional needs, asserted as art forms, and consumed aesthetically as haute cuisine, are therefore best coded as exclusionist consumption goods. ...

Divergence in Cultural Practices: Tastes as Signals of Identity
  • Citing Article
  • January 2008

... Very few works studied linguistic memes, which are "units of language that encapsulate a core idea and that is transmitted, largely intact, among individuals-examples include slogans, sayings, metaphors and stories" [29]. Existing findings show that linguistic memes can help facilitate team management and coordination [18,39,69]. ...

Language as a Coordinating Mechanism: How Linguistic Memes Help Direct Appropriate Action
  • Citing Article

... It was found that setting clear, challenging goals is likely to increase output on these tasks (Locke and Latham, 2002). According to empirical research, goals may significantly affect a person's ability to take risks (Larrick et al ., 2009). West et al . ...

Goal-Induced Risk Taking in Negotiation and Decision Making

Social Cognition

... To incorporate that model, the agents in this chapter (as will be explained) alter their trust judgments via direct experience only, but can situationally alter their behavior based on gossip. Direct experience (repeated cooperation in this model) increases the likelihood of trust (Burt, 1999a). However, gossip information is discounted structurally via the trust model. ...

Evidence of Density, Distrust, and Character Assassination
  • Citing Article
Ronald S. Burt

·

Boulevard de Constance

·

Russell Sage

·

[...]

·

John Lott

... There are genuine jointly developed goals only when the employer is professional and willing to integrate the interests of its agent with hers (Mosey, 2009). Moreover, according to Wu et al. (2008), goals can be divided into losses (failure to achieve the goal) and gains (success in achieving the goal). Setting more challenging goals implies a greater chance of loss. ...

A prospect theory model of goal behavior

... Studies on the attribution theory have revealed that while emphasising that the most fundamental reason for their relative success is themselves, individuals believe that their failures are due to reasons originating from the outside world. Moreover, it has become a scientific fact that individuals tend to exaggerate their positive aspects [27]. Since attribution theory deals with the causes of success and failure in achievement-oriented environments [28] and focuses on interpersonal behaviours and attitudes from the perspective of social psychology, sport provides a suitable environment for qualitative and quantitative analyses of causal attributions. ...

Flawed Self-Assessment Implications for Health, Education, and the Workplace
  • Citing Article
  • December 2004

Psychological Science in the Public Interest

... A common outcome of shared characteristics is friendship (Marmaros and Sacerdote 2006;Reagans 2011). In evaluation, friends tend to refrain from judging each other (Goffman 1959), emphasize the positive (Blumberg 1972), and "echo" common beliefs in discourse, which increases the odds of favorable evaluation of friends (Burt 2001). ...

Bandwidth and Echo: Trust, Information, and Gossip in Social Networks
  • Citing Article
  • May 2001