Chetan C. Gandhi’s research while affiliated with Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (18)


Figure 1. 
Table 1 . Summary of task variables
Figure 2. Top, Latency across trials to find food in the Lashley maze. Bottom, Errors (turns in wrong direction, retracing) across trials.
Figure 3. 
Table 3 . Correlations (r values) of individuals' (n 56) performance across tasks

+6

Individual Differences in the Expression of a “General” Learning Ability in Mice
  • Article
  • Full-text available

July 2003

·

131 Reads

·

239 Citations

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

·

Yu Ray Han

·

Henya Grossman

·

[...]

·

Chetan C Gandhi

Human performance on diverse tests of intellect are impacted by a "general" regulatory factor that accounts for up to 50% of the variance between individuals on intelligence tests. Neurobiological determinants of general cognitive abilities are essentially unknown, owing in part to the paucity of animal research wherein neurobiological analyses are possible. We report a methodology with which we have assessed individual differences in the general learning abilities of laboratory mice. Abilities of mice on tests of associative fear conditioning, operant avoidance, path integration, discrimination, and spatial navigation were assessed. Tasks were designed so that each made unique sensory, motor, motivational, and information processing demands on the animals. A sample of 56 genetically diverse outbred mice (CD-1) was used to assess individuals' acquisition on each task. Indicative of a common source of variance, positive correlations were found between individuals' performance on all tasks. When tested on multiple test batteries, the overall performance ranks of individuals were found to be highly reliable and were "normally" distributed. Factor analysis of learning performance variables determined that a single factor accounted for 38% of the total variance across animals. Animals' levels of native activity and body weights accounted for little of the variability in learning, although animals' propensity for exploration loaded strongly (and was positively correlated) with learning abilities. These results indicate that diverse learning abilities of laboratory mice are influenced by a common source of variance and, moreover, that the general learning abilities of individual mice can be specified relative to a sample of peers. Matzel,L.D.,Han YR,Grossman H,Karnik MS,Patel D,Scott N,Specht SM,Gandhi CC.

Download

Fig. 2. Intracellular recordings were made from the somatic membrane of hair cells before and 60 min after the introduction of the L-channel agonist 6 BAY K 8644 (7 m M) into the intracellular bath. (A) Summary data from hair cells ( n 5 5) indicates that 6 BAY K 8644 had no effect on the resting membrane potential, spike amplitude, or spike duration. However, 6 BAY K 8644 reduced the rate of spike discharge (top panel), and this effect was reversed following washout of the agonist. (B) Representative voltage records obtained during a depolarizing voltage step before and after introduction of 6 BAY K 8644 are provided. (C) 6 BAY K 8644 increased the amplitude of the post-spike AHP, an effect that likely underlies the reduction in rate of discharge during spike trains. Summary data on the effect of 6 BAY K 8644 on the AHP are provided in the bottom panel of Fig. 1C. 
Fig. 3. Paired intracellular recordings from the hair cell and a postsynaptic B photoreceptor ( n 5 4) during 3 s of hair cell depolarization. (A) Summary data indicate that the absolute amplitude of the compound IPSP in the B cell is positively related to the firing frequency in the presynaptic hair cell. (B) Representative voltage records illustrating the compound IPSP recorded in the B cell (upper records) in response to 12- or 25-Hz spike trains induced in the presynaptic hair cell. 
Fig. 4. Pseudocolor Fura-2 images of a hair cell exposed to 6 BAY K 8644 (7 m M). After baseline recording, 6 BAY K 8644 was introduced into the bath and images were acquired at 2, 40, and 60 min. A significant 2 1 increase in somatic Ca was observed 2 min after 6 BAY K 8644 
Fig. 6. (A) Paired intracellular recordings were made from a hair cell (lower records) and a postsynaptic B photoreceptor (upper records) before, 2 and 50 min after bath application of 6 BAY K 8644, and again after a 10-min washout of the agonist. (B) Summary data obtained from four pairs of recordings (means 6 S.E.) illustrating the IPSP generated in the B photoreceptor as described in (A). 
Calcium 'leak' through somatic L-type channels has multiple deleterious effects on regulated transmitter release from an invertebrate hair cell

March 2003

·

86 Reads

Brain Research

Using an identified synapse in the nervous system of the mollusc Hermissenda, the influence of somatic calcium accumulation on regulated synaptic transmission was investigated. Hair cells in Hermissenda project onto postsynaptic B photoreceptors where they mediate inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs). Intracellular recordings in combination with bath perfusion of calcium channel modulators indicated that L-type channels were present on the hair cell soma but not on the terminal branches. In contrast, P/Q and an unidentified channel type (similar to N-type channels) contributed additively to transmitter release from the hair cell. Antibodies raised against rat brain channel proteins detected L- (alpha1


FIGURE 1. 
FIGURE 2. 
FIGURE 3. 
Hippocampal function during behaviorally silent associative learning: Dissociation of memory storage and expression

October 2002

·

103 Reads

·

39 Citations

Hippocampus

In laboratory studies, the assessment of memory is typically associated with overt behavioral responses. Thus, it has been difficult to determine whether the enhancement of hippocampal sensory-evoked potentials that often accompany memory formation are the neurophysiological manifestation of a memory "trace" or are a secondary product of the behavioral expression of the memory. We addressed this issue by examining changes in evoked hippocampal field potentials during sensory preconditioning, a form of behaviorally silent relational learning that requires an intact hippocampus for execution. Rats were exposed to presentations of a white noise (S1) that terminated with a tone (S2). These pairings of ostensibly "neutral" stimuli supported no change in the behavior elicited by the noise. However, if the tone was subsequently paired with mild footshock (US), suppression of ongoing licking behavior (indicative of fear) was elicited by the noise, indicating that the animal had associated the noise with tone (S1-S2), and had represented the noise-tone-shock (S1-S2-US) relationship. Pre-training neurotoxic lesions of the hippocampus had no effect on conditioned suppression to tone after tone-shock (S2-US) pairings, but disrupted the expression of continued suppression to noise (S1) after tone-shock pairings. In a second experiment, sensory-evoked field potentials in the dorsal hippocampus were recorded with extracellular electrodes. No changes in the hippocampal response evoked by white noise were observed after pairings of noise and tone, i.e., no evidence for a memory trace could be detected. In contrast, after tone was paired with footshock, two short-latency negative potentials within the noise-evoked field response increased in amplitude, a response often presumed to reflect a neurophysiological correlate of memory storage. In total, these results suggest that although the hippocampus critically contributes to the processing of a behaviorally silent associative memory, there may be no role for changes in the amplitude of hippocampal sensory-evoked field potentials in storing representations of the relationships between sensory experiences.


FIG. 2. 
FIG. 4. 
The Role of the Hippocampus in Trace Conditioning: Temporal Discontinuity or Task Difficulty?

December 2001

·

384 Reads

·

242 Citations

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

It is well established that the hippocampal formation is critically involved in the acquisition of trace memories, a paradigm in which the conditioned (CS) and unconditioned stimuli (US) are separated by a temporal gap (Solomon et al., 1986). The structure is reportedly not critical for the acquisition of delay memories, where the CS and the US overlap in time (Berger & Orr, 1983; Schmaltz & Theios, 1972). Based on these results, it is often stated that the hippocampus is involved in "filling the gap" or otherwise associating the two stimuli in time. However, in addition to the presence of a temporal gap, there are other differences between trace and delay conditioning. The most apparent difference is that animals require many more trials to learn the trace task, and thus it is inherently more difficult than the delay task. Here, we tested whether the hippocampus was critically involved in delay conditioning, if it was rendered more difficult such that the rate of acquisition was shifted to be analogous to trace conditioning. Groups of rats received excitotoxic lesions to the hippocampus, sham lesions or were left intact. Using the same interstimulus intervals (ISI), control animals required more trials to acquire the trace than the delay task. As predicted, animals with hippocampal lesions were impaired during trace conditioning but not delay conditioning. However, when the delay task was rendered more difficult by extending the ISI (a long delay task), animals with hippocampal lesions were impaired. In addition, once the lesioned animal learned the association between the CS and the US during delay conditioning, it could learn and perform the trace CR. Thus, the role of the hippocampus in classical conditioning is not limited to learning about discontiguous events in time and space; rather the structure can become engaged simply as a function of task difficulty.


FIG. 1. Free fatty acid assessment using the fluorescent probe acrylodan-derivatized intestinal fatty acid-binding protein (ADIFAB). A: ratio of ADIFAB fluorescence at 505 to 432 nm as function of time. An increase in ratio of 432 to 502 nm was observed in the groups that were incubated in GABA or melittin but not in the group incubated in ASW. Brackets indicate SE of measurement. B: ratio of ADIFAB fluorescence at 505 to 432 nm as function of time in a partial replication of the experiment described in A. An increase in the ratios of 505 to 432 nm was again observed following exposure to GABA, but this effect was blocked if the exposure to GABA occurred in the presence of the PLA 2 inhibitor arachidonyltrifluoromethyl ketone (AACOCF3).  
FIG. 3. Representative voltage records from 4 B cells that contributed to the mean values summarized in Fig. 2. Responses to hyperpolarizing (left) and depolarizing (right) current injections were obtained during a baseline period (BL) and again 45 min after 1 of the 4 treatments described in Fig. 2. Concomitant exposure of the B cell to OA and light (OA L) resulted in an increase in the hyperpolarizing voltage response (indicative of an increase in membrane resistance) and an increase in the rate of spike discharge during depolarization.  
FIG. 4. Protein synthesis dependence of OA-mediated excitability increases. Top: voltage responses to 0.6 nA measured at 45 and 90 min. The groups exposed to light in the presence of OA (OA L) or OA and the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin (OA L ANI) exhibited significantly elevated input resistances that persisted for 90 min. Brackets indicate SE. Bottom: number of evoked spikes observed 45 and 90 min after training elicited by 0.6-nA current injections. The groups exposed to light presentations in the presence of OA (OA L and OA L ANI) exhibited significantly greater numbers of evoked spikes when they were trained in the normal bath or anisomycin (ANI).
Receptor-Stimulated Phospholipase A 2 Liberates Arachidonic Acid and Regulates Neuronal Excitability Through Protein Kinase C

April 2001

·

76 Reads

·

30 Citations

Journal of Neurophysiology

Type B photoreceptors in Hermissenda exhibit increased excitability (e.g., elevated membrane resistance and lowered spike thresholds) consequent to the temporal coincidence of a light-induced intracellular Ca(2+) increase and the release of GABA from presynaptic vestibular hair cells. Convergence of these pre- and postsynaptically stimulated biochemical cascades culminates in the activation of protein kinase C (PKC). Paradoxically, exposure of the B cell to light alone generates an inositol triphosphate-regulated rise in diacylglycerol and intracellular Ca(2+), co-factors sufficient to stimulate conventional PKC isoforms, raising questions as to the unique role of synaptic stimulation in the activation of PKC. GABA receptors on the B cell are coupled to G proteins that stimulate phospholipase A(2) (PLA(2)), which is thought to regulate the liberation of arachidonic acid (AA), an "atypical" activator of PKC. Here, we directly assess whether GABA binding or PLA(2) stimulation liberates AA in these cells and whether free AA potentiates the stimulation of PKC. Free fatty-acid was estimated in isolated photoreceptors with the fluorescent indicator acrylodan-derivatized intestinal fatty acid-binding protein (ADIFAB). In response to 5 microM GABA, a fast and persistent increase in ADIFAB emission was observed, and this increase was blocked by the PLA(2) inhibitor arachidonyltrifluoromethyl ketone (50 microM). Furthermore, direct stimulation of PLA(2) by melittin (10 microM) increased ADIFAB emission in a manner that was kinetically analogous to GABA. In response to simultaneous exposure to the stable AA analogue oleic acid (OA, 20 microM) and light (to elevate intracellular Ca(2+)), B photoreceptors exhibited a sustained (>45 min) increase in excitability (membrane resistance and evoked spike rate). The excitability increase was blocked by the PKC inhibitor chelerythrine (20 microM) and was not induced by exposure of the cells to light alone. The increase in excitability in the B cell that followed exposure to light and OA persisted for > or =90 min when the pairing was conducted in the presence of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin (1 microm), suggesting that the synergistic influence of these signaling agents on neuronal excitability did not require new protein synthesis. These results indicate that GABA binding to G-protein-coupled receptors on Hermissenda B cells stimulates a PLA(2) signaling cascade that liberates AA, and that this free AA interacts with postsynaptic Ca(2+) to synergistically stimulate PKC and enhance neuronal excitability. In this manner, the interaction of postsynaptic metabotropic receptors and intracellular Ca(2+) may serve as the catalyst for some forms of associative neuronal/synaptic plasticity.


The tractable contribution of synapses and their component molecules to individual differences in learning

July 2000

·

13 Reads

·

18 Citations

Behavioural Brain Research

Though once of central importance to psychologists and neurophysiologists alike, the elucidation of neural substrates for individual differences in learning no longer attracts a broad research effort and occupies a place of largely historical interest to the contemporary disciplines. The decline in interest in this subject ensued in part from the perception, arrived at decades ago, that individual differences in learning were not quantified as easily as had once been presumed. Furthermore, the dominant hypotheses in the field defied testing within the constraints imposed by the complex and largely inaccessible vertebrate nervous system. Using a 'model systems' approach where the individual cells and synaptic interactions that comprise a neural network can be identified, we have returned to this question and have established a framework by which we can begin to discern the basis for much of the variability between individuals in their capacity to learn. In the marine mollusc Hermissenda, we have found that a common influence on transmitter exocytosis is expressed homogeneously throughout the nervous system regardless of transmitter system or receptor class. Though uniformly expressed within an individual, this influence on synaptic efficacy is differentially expressed between animals. Importantly, the basal efficiency of exocytosis expressed in an individual nervous system is strongly correlated with the degree to which activity-dependent forms of neuronal/synaptic facilitation can be induced in that nervous system, and predicts the capacity for the intact animal to learn a Pavlovian association. Furthermore, we have established that a decline in basal synaptic efficacy in aged animals, arising from chronic presynaptic Ca(2+) 'leak', may contribute to age-related learning impairments. Because certain fundamental components of the exocytotic cascade are conserved widely across cell types, transmitter systems and species, the principles that we describe may have broad implications for understanding normal variability in learning, but also, in the development of specific strategies to compensate for mild learning deficits and age-related cognitive decline.


Impaired acquisition of a Morris water maze task following selective destruction of cerebellar Purkinje cells with OX7-saporin

May 2000

·

20 Reads

·

94 Citations

Behavioural Brain Research

Spatial learning in the Morris water maze task is believed to be dependent on an intact hippocampal system. However, evidence from human studies and animal experiments suggests a potential cerebellar involvement in spatial processing, place learning, and other types of 'higher-order' cognition. In order to investigate this possibility, intraventricular injections (ICV) of the anti-neuronal immunotoxin OX7-saporin were used to selectively destroy cerebellar Purkinje cells, without affecting other brain areas believed to be critically involved in spatial learning and memory. Bilateral ICV injections of 2 microg OX7-saporin (4 microg total) in adult male rats produced substantial loss of Purkinje cells (56%) throughout the cerebellum without affecting hippocampal morphology or biochemical indices of cholinergic, serotonergic, or catecholaminergic function in the hippocampus, frontal cortex, or striatum. ICV OX7-saporin significantly impaired acquisition and performance of the standard Morris water maze task (though the impairment was less severe than reported in earlier studies that used alternate lesion methods or mutant mice species), but did not alter performance on the cued version of the task, or locomotor activity. In addition, lesioned animals spent significantly less time in the target quadrant on probe trial days 4 and 7 and the average distance to target scores (ADT) were significantly greater than controls on those days. Swim speed was not affected. Based on the specificity of the behavioral and neurobiological alterations, these data support the hypothesis that the cerebellum is involved in spatial processing and place learning.


Synaptic efficacy is commonly regulated within a nervous system and predicts individual differences in learning

May 2000

·

11 Reads

·

8 Citations

Neuroreport

The hypothesis that an individual's capacity for learning might be predicted or influenced by basal levels of synaptic efficacy has eluded empirical tests, owing in part to the inability to compare between animals single identified synaptic responses in the mammalian brain. To overcome this limitation, we have focused our analysis on the invertebrate Hermissenda, whose nervous system is composed of identifiable cells and synaptic interactions. Hermissenda were exposed to paired presentations of light and rotation such that the light came to elicit a learned defensive motor response. An animal's rate of learning was strongly correlated with the amplitude of the synaptic potential evoked in that animal's visual (light sensitive) receptors in response to stimulation of presynaptic vestibular (rotation sensitive) hair cells. In naive animals, strong correlations between the amplitude of both inhibitory and excitatory synaptic potentials were observed between synapses distributed throughout an animal's nervous system, and this conservation of synaptic efficacy was largely attributable to a common influence on transmitter release. These observations suggest that basal synaptic efficacy may be uniformly regulated throughout a nervous system, and provide direct evidence that the basal efficacy of synaptic transmission predicts, and possibly contributes to, individual differences between animals in their capacity to learn.


Modulation of Presynaptic Action Potential Kinetics Underlies Synaptic Facilitation of Type B Photoreceptors after Associative Conditioning in Hermissenda

April 2000

·

56 Reads

·

36 Citations

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

Descriptions of conditioned response generation in Hermissenda stipulate that the synaptic interaction between type B and A photoreceptors should be enhanced after associative pairings of light and rotation. Although evidence from several laboratories has confirmed this assumption, the mechanism underlying this synaptic facilitation has not been elucidated. Here we report that in vitro conditioning (i.e., light paired with stimulation of vestibular hair cells) modifies the kinetics of presynaptic action potentials in the B photoreceptor in a manner sufficient to account for this synaptic facilitation. After paired training, we observed an increase in the duration of evoked action potentials and a decrease in the amplitude of the spike afterhyperpolarization in the B-cell. As previously reported, paired training also enhanced the excitability (i.e., input resistance and evoked spike rate) of the B photoreceptor. In a second experiment, simultaneous recordings were made in type B and A photoreceptors, and paired training was found to produce an increase in the amplitude of the IPSP in the A photoreceptor in response to an evoked spike in the B-cell. Importantly, there was no change in the initial slope of the postsynaptic IPSP in the A photoreceptor, suggesting that spike duration-independent mechanisms of neurotransmitter exocytosis or postsynaptic receptor sensitivity did not contribute to the observed synaptic facilitation. Perfusion of 4-aminopyridine (4-AP) mimicked a known effect of behavioral conditioning in that it specifically reduced the amplitude of the transient voltage-dependent K(+) current (I(A)) in the B-cell, but in addition, produced action potential broadening and synaptic facilitation that was analogous to that observed after in vitro conditioning. Finally, the effect of 4-AP on B-cell action potentials and on the postsynaptic IPSP in the A-cell was occluded by previous paired (but not unpaired) training, suggesting that the prolongation of the B-cell action potential by a reduction of I(A) was sufficient to account for the observed synaptic facilitation. The occlusion of the effects of 4-AP by paired training was not attributable to a saturation of the capacity of the B-cell for transmitter exocytosis, because it was observed that tetraethylammonium (TEA)-induced inhibition of the delayed voltage-dependent K(+) current induced both spike broadening and synaptic facilitation regardless of training history. Collectively, these results demonstrate that training-induced facilitation at B-cell synapses is attributable to the effects of a reduction of a presynaptic K(+) conductance on action potential kinetics and suggest another critical similarity between the cellular basis for learning in Hermissenda and other invertebrate systems.


Effects of Intraseptal Zolpidem and Chlordiazepoxide on Spatial Working Memory and High-Affinity Choline Uptake in the Hippocampus

April 2000

·

25 Reads

·

26 Citations

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

Injection of GABA(A)/benzodiazepine receptor ligands into the medial septum (MS) alters the activity of cholinergic neurons that innervate the hippocampus and can produce bidirectional modulation of spatial memory. Recent evidence suggests that two subtypes of the GABA(A) receptor are differentially localized to either GABAergic (alpha(1)/beta(2)/gamma(2)) or cholinergic (alpha(3)/beta(3)/gamma(2)) neurons within the MS. The present studies characterized the dose-related behavioral and neurochemical effects of intraseptal infusions of two benzodiazepine (BDZ) agonists that appear to exhibit different profiles of pharmacological specificity for these receptor subtypes. Male Sprague-Dawley rats were cannulated and then artificial CSF, chlordiazepoxide (CDP: 8 or 12 microg), or zolpidem (4, 8, or 12 microg) was injected into the MS. Spatial working memory was assessed in a delay radial-arm maze task and the activity of cholinergic neurons in the MS was evaluated by high-affinity choline uptake (HA-ChU) in the hippocampus. Intraseptal injection of either CDP or zolpidem produced dose-related impairments in spatial working memory and decreases in hippocampal HAChU. Both BDZ agonists were found to produce retrograde memory deficits and a decrease in HAChU following the highest dose tested (12 microg). However, intraseptal injection of 8 microg of zolpidem produced a behavioral deficit comparable to the high dose of CDP, but did not alter HAChU within the HPC. Although the cholinergic component of the septohippocampal pathway has been shown to be important in modulating hippocampal physiology and spatial memory processes, data from the present experiments suggest that the GABAergic component may also play an important role in the behavioral functions of the septohippocampal pathway.


Citations (15)


... The present behavioral deficit in learning is consistent with results using a different species (rats) and different learning tasks after intraseptal procaine infusion (e.g., Mizumori et al., 1989Mizumori et al., , 1990Rashidy-Pour, Motamedi, Motahed-Larijani, 1996;Walsh, Gandhi, & Stackman, 1998). In some of those studies, it was difficult to evaluate the specificity of septal inactivation effects on the learning process itself. ...

Reference:

Reversible Septal Inactivation Disrupts Hippocampal Slow-Wave and Unit Activity and Impairs Trace Conditioning in Rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus )
Reversible Inactivation of the Medial Septum or Nucleus Basalis Impairs Working Memory in Rats: A Dissociation of Memory and Performance

... Therefore, spike shape variations participate in short-term synaptic plasticity produced by repetitive firing (Zucker and Regehr, 2002). In addition, Hebbian or homeostatic forms of synaptic plasticity may result from modulation of presynaptic spike waveform via long-term regulation of ion channel density ( Gandhi and Matzel, 2000;Yang and Wang, 2006;Hoppa et al., 2014). ...

Modification of action potential kinetics following associative conditioning of Hermissenda contributes to synaptic facilitation of B photoreceptors
  • Citing Article
  • January 1997

... These training sessions required substantive investments (i.e., time and costs), with studies reporting pretraining sessions lasting from 18 to 20 days with a specific criterion needed to be reached by each rodent [85,25]. While it is known that animals learn at a different pace [120][121][122], setting a specific criterion to be reached prior to moving onto the next stage ensures that all animals understand the task and its associated rewards [123,124]. In this context, cooperation tasks tend to be considered of higher complexity in terms of required cognitive resources [80], hence rendering a pretraining period essential when using these types of paradigms. ...

Individual Differences in the Expression of a “General” Learning Ability in Mice

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

... Classical conditioning, in which a cue (the conditioned stimulus or CS) is paired with a reflex-evoking unconditioned stimulus (US) until the CS comes to produce an anticipatory response (the conditioned response or CR) has proven a useful testbed for examining the psychological principles and neurobiological substrates of learning. Under many circumstances, delay conditioning, in which the CS and US overlap and co-terminate, is spared or even mildly facilitated following hippocampal damage (e.g., Berger, Rinaldi, Weisz, & Thompson, 1983;Gabrieli et al., 1995;Ito, Everitt, & Robbins, 2005;Ito, Robbins, McNaughton, & Everitt, 2006;Schmaltz & Theios, 1972); conversely, under many conditions, hippocampal lesion disrupts trace conditioning, in which CS offset occurs before US onset, producing a temporal gap known as the trace interval (e.g., Beylin et al., 1999;McGlinchey-Berroth, Carrillo, Gabrieli, Brawn, & Disterhoft, 1997;Solomon, Vander Schaaf, Thompson, & Weisz, 1986;Weisz, Solomon, & Thompson, 1980). This has led some researchers to assume that the hippocampus plays a more important role in trace than in delay conditioning (Beylin, et al., 1999;McGlinchey-Berroth, et al., 1997;Solomon, et al., 1986)-as we discuss below, this assumption ignores empirical findings on the role of the hippocampus in delay conditioning. ...

Acquisition but not performance of trace eyeblink conditioning is dependent on the hippocampus
  • Citing Article
  • January 1999

... In keeping with its widespread innervation of the cerebral cortex, the basal forebrain cholinergic system has been implicated in a number of behaviors including sleep and arousal, mood and affect, and particularly attention and memory (Dingledine & Kelly, 1977;Drachman & Leavitt, 1974;Janowsky et al., 1972;Muir et al., 1992). (Aigner et al., 1987;Fine et al., 1997;Flicker et al., 1983;Leanza et al., 1996;Walsh et al., 1996). Basal forebrain lesions have also been shown to affect expectancy and particularly attention (Muir et al., 1992;Stoehr et al., 1997). ...

Injection of IgG 192-saporin into the medial septum produces cholinergic hypofunction and dose-dependent working memory deficits
  • Citing Article
  • August 1996

Brain Research

... Muscimol (GABA A agonist) and lidocaine (Na + channels blocker) were used to temporarily silence or reduce the activity in the targeted brain region through two opposite mechanisms: increasing inhibitory interneuron activity through disinhibition and reducing action potential firing probability, respectively. When applied in the MSDB, the most striking effect on the animal's behaviour was the impairment in tasks requiring memory and navigation (Chrobak et al., 1989;Nagahara and McGaugh, 1992;Walsh et al., 1998), confirming the crucial importance of septal projections to the HPC formation. MSDB inhibition via lidocaine seemed not to affect the running speed (Koenig et al., 2011), but to reduce anxiety and increase open arm exploration in a plus maze (Lamprea et al., 2010). ...

Reversible inactivation of the medial septum or nucleus basalis impairs working memory in rats: A dissociation of memory and performance

... Therefore, spike shape variations participate in short-term synaptic plasticity produced by repetitive firing (Zucker and Regehr, 2002). In addition, Hebbian or homeostatic forms of synaptic plasticity may result from modulation of presynaptic spike waveform via long-term regulation of ion channel density (Gandhi and Matzel, 2000;Yang and Wang, 2006;Hoppa et al., 2014). ...

Modulation of Presynaptic Action Potential Kinetics Underlies Synaptic Facilitation of Type B Photoreceptors after Associative Conditioning in Hermissenda

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

... The Purkinje cells are the most important neurons in the cerebellar cortex (Jelliffe et al., 2012). The cerebellar cortex is highly vulnerable to the effects of toxic metals, including Hg (Gandhi et al., 2000). Injury or damage to the cells of the cerebellum may lead to conditions like; ataxia, Joubert syndrome, and many others (Jelliffe et al., 2012). ...

Impaired acquisition of a Morris water maze task following selective destruction of cerebellar Purkinje cells with OX7-saporin
  • Citing Article
  • May 2000

Behavioural Brain Research

... In this chapter I discussed the one plausible mechanism based on the behavioral findings to consolidate the taste aversive conditioning according to the following consolidation hypothesis in terms of memory period: STM (short-term memory), ITM (intermediate-term memory) and LTM (long-term memory). STM lasting only minutes requires modification of pre-existing substrate proteins, while ITM lasting a few hours is dependent on de novo protein synthesis, and finally that LTM lasting more than a few hours to days, weeks and years requires both new protein synthesis and altered gene activity [45][46][47][48][49][50][51][52]. ...

Reference:

Sakakibara
Protein synthesis-dependent memory and neuronal enhancement in Hermissenda are contingent on parameters of training and retention

Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)

... It is very well stated in the literature that a decrease in hippocampus ACh levels is responsible for the decline of age-related cognitive functions. It is also stated that ACh levels in the hippocampus increase during the tasks related to spatial memory (Stancampiano et al., 1999), and damage to the medial septum results in the decline of ACh levels in the hippocampus thus impairing the spatial memory (Herzog et al., 2000). The enzyme AChE present in the synaptic cleft is very well known for degrading the ACh to choline and acetate and thus inhi-bits cholinergic transmission (Ahmad et al., 2014). ...

Effects of Intraseptal Zolpidem and Chlordiazepoxide on Spatial Working Memory and High-Affinity Choline Uptake in the Hippocampus
  • Citing Article
  • April 2000

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory