Chester A. Insko’s research while affiliated with University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (143)


Interindividual-intergroup discontinuity in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game: How common fate, proximity, and similarity affect intergroup competition
  • Article
  • Full-text available

December 2014

·

544 Reads

·

42 Citations

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

Chester A Insko

·

·

In two experiments with the PDG we manipulated the Campbell (1958), or Wertheimer (1923), indices of entitativity (common fate, proximity, and similarity) to examine when a set of individuals interacts with another set of individuals in the competitive manner that is characteristic of group-on-group interactions. Experiment 1 found that interactions between two 3-person sets were more competitive when participants within each set shared (vs. did not share) common fate. In Experiment 2, the Wertheimer-Campbell indices were manipulated for one 3-person set only (targets). Participants in the other 3-person set were sequestered in separate rooms (observers). Observers as well as targets were more competitive when targets shared (vs. did not share) common fate. Path analyses in both experiments supported the idea that common fate increases competition via increased own-set entitativity and subsequent greed, and via increased other-set entitativity and subsequent fear. We found no consistent evidence for the possible roles of proximity and similarity.

Download

Perceived relevance of honesty and agreeableness to situations with non-correspondent and correspondent outcomes: An interdependence perspective

December 2013

·

27 Reads

·

3 Citations

European Journal of Social Psychology

According to interdependence theory, interpersonal situations that vary in their surface characteristics can be united by similarities in their underlying structure. Likewise, factor analytic approaches to personality combine many traits into a small number of factors. In the current research, we use interdependence theory and existing factor analyses of personality traits to measure people's lay theories about the ways traits and situations interact. We predict that traits representative of honesty–humility/virtue will be rated as more relevant to situations with non-correspondent outcomes (a gain in one person's outcomes is associated with a loss in the other person's outcomes) than to situations with correspondent outcomes (a gain in one person's outcomes is associated with a gain in the other person's outcomes). Conversely, we predict that traits representative of agreeableness will be rated as more relevant to situations with correspondent outcomes than to situations with non-correspondent outcomes. An experiment found the expected trait X situation interaction revealing that subjects expect certain types of traits to be most relevant to specific types of situations. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


Table 1 Assessments of Various Reactions in the Eight Experimental Conditions 
The social psychological status of reward

October 2013

·

74 Reads

·

5 Citations

Psychonomic Science

Two experiments investigated the effect of reward on forced compliance attitude change in relation to two supposedly crucial interactive variables. The results of these experiments indicated that previous analyses of these effects are open to serious question.


Cognitive requirements of consistent and counterattitudinal roleplaying: A general set effect

June 2013

·

14 Reads

·

1 Citation

Psychonomic Science

This experiment demonstrates that Greenwald’s (1969) finding that the counterattitudinal roleplayer is open-minded can best be described as a general set effect. Potential advocates (counterattitudinal and consistent attitudinal) were found to judge attitudinal sta teme nts congruently with their expected roles.


Table 1 Study 1: Exploratory Factor Analyses of Each of the GASP Subscales Factor Eigenvalues 2 p RMSEA CFI TLI 
Figure 1. Study 2: Confirmatory factor analysis of the GASP in a sample of 862 adults (weighted least squares mean and variance estimation). Factor variances were fixed to 1; thresholds for each item were estimated but are not shown. GASP Guilt and Shame Proneness scale; NBE negative behavior-evaluation; NSE negative self-evaluation. p .05. 
Introducing the GASP Scale: A New Measure of Guilt and Shame Proneness

May 2011

·

25,821 Reads

·

616 Citations

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

Although scholars agree that moral emotions are critical for deterring unethical and antisocial behavior, there is disagreement about how 2 prototypical moral emotions--guilt and shame--should be defined, differentiated, and measured. We addressed these issues by developing a new assessment--the Guilt and Shame Proneness scale (GASP)--that measures individual differences in the propensity to experience guilt and shame across a range of personal transgressions. The GASP contains 2 guilt subscales that assess negative behavior-evaluations and repair action tendencies following private transgressions and 2 shame subscales that assess negative self-evaluations (NSEs) and withdrawal action tendencies following publically exposed transgressions. Both guilt subscales were highly correlated with one another and negatively correlated with unethical decision making. Although both shame subscales were associated with relatively poor psychological functioning (e.g., neuroticism, personal distress, low self-esteem), they were only weakly correlated with one another, and their relationships with unethical decision making diverged. Whereas shame-NSE constrained unethical decision making, shame-withdraw did not. Our findings suggest that differentiating the tendency to make NSEs following publically exposed transgressions from the tendency to hide or withdraw from public view is critically important for understanding and measuring dispositional shame proneness. The GASP's ability to distinguish these 2 classes of responses represents an important advantage of the scale over existing assessments. Although further validation research is required, the present studies are promising in that they suggest the GASP has the potential to be an important measurement tool for detecting individuals susceptible to corruption and unethical behavior.


Shame Proneness and Guilt Proneness: Toward the Further Understanding of Reactions to Public and Private Transgressions

October 2010

·

3,495 Reads

·

135 Citations

Self and Identity

In Study 1, participants completed five extant shame and guilt proneness inventories based on different theoretical conceptions of the difference between shame and guilt. Factor analyses revealed that despite very different theoretical distinctions, the shame proneness subscales loaded on one factor, and the guilt proneness subscales loaded on one factor. In Study 2, we altered scale items so that hypothetical transgressions were committed in either public or private, and likelihood response options were either typical of a ‘‘shame-prone response’’ (negative self-evaluation; avoidance behavior) or a ‘‘guilt-prone response’’ (negative behavior evaluation; approach behavior). Our findings indicate that shame and guilt proneness can be measured both by responses to transgressions (e.g., negative self-evaluation and avoidance responses vs. negative behavior evaluation and approach responses) and the situational context in which the transgression occurs (e.g., public vs. private). We provide recommendations regarding optimal measurement of shame and guilt proneness.


How communication increases interpersonal cooperation in mixed-motive situations

January 2010

·

208 Reads

·

64 Citations

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

a b s t r a c t Evidence from two experiments indicates that task-related communication promotes cooperation in mixed-motive situations by activating interpersonal norms related to fairness and trust. In Experiment 1, task-related communication increased cooperation between individuals in a three-choice prisoner's dilemma game (PDG-Alt) but task-unrelated communication did not. In Experiment 2, cooperation was increased both by sending a task-related message to one's counterpart and receiving a cooperative task-related message from one's counterpart. Mediation analyses revealed that task-related communica-tion increased cooperation by activating fairness and trust norms (Experiments 1 and 2). Specifically, whereas sending (relative to receiving) a task-related message increased cooperation by activating fair-ness norms, receiving (relative to sending) a task-related message increased cooperation by activating trust norms (Experiment 2).




Figure 1. Prisoner's Dilemma Game (PDG) used in the experiment. Values in the matrix represent the amount of money (in US cents) that each side received as a result of the various choice combinations. The outcomes in this matrix were used in the groups condition. Outcomes were divided by three in the individuals condition
Table 1 . Proportion of competition and distrust as a function of future training and groups versus individuals (N ¼ 139)
Figure 2. 
Reducing intergroup conflict through the consideration of future consequences

August 2009

·

1,721 Reads

·

33 Citations

European Journal of Social Psychology

·

·

Jeffrey L. Kirchner

·

[...]

·

Chester A. Insko

Basic social psychological research has suggested several interventions to reduce intergroup conflict. Most of these interventions, however, have been indirect and impractical to implement outside laboratory settings. Although past research has demonstrated that indirect manipulations of the consideration of future consequences reduce intergroup competition, no study of interindividual–intergroup discontinuity has tested this assumption with a direct manipulation. The present study found that when participants (individuals and members of groups) interacting in an iterated prisoner's dilemma game (PDG) were asked to predict how their opponent's choice on a second trial would be affected by their own choice on an initial trial, intergroup competition was reduced while interindividual competition remained low regardless of the manipulation. On a practical level, implications of this study provide a simple and easily implemented solution to reducing intergroup conflict in non-laboratory situations. Copyright © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


Citations (95)


... The results are noteworthy because it was doubted that mere social categorization might suffice to produce the interindividual-intergroup discontinuity effect in the absence of structural differences among interindividual and intergroup interactions (e.g. Drigotas, Insko, & Schopler, 1998). ...

Reference:

Social categorization and group-motivated interindividual-intergroup discontinuity
Mere Categorization and Competition: A Closer Look at Social Identity Theory and the Discontinuity Effect
  • Citing Chapter
  • January 1998

... As Masuda (2012) demonstrates, individuals often exhibit ingroup favoritism (i.e., cooperating only or preferably within their own group) rather than full cooperation (i.e., cooperating within their own and with other groups), even when the latter is-and the former is not-Pareto efficient. Ingroup favoritism results from the difficulty of building trust between groups (as opposed to building trust between individuals), a difficulty that often leads to intergroup competition or even conflict rather than cooperation (Insko et al. 2001). As a case in point, experiments show that individuals tend to be more generous in allocating resources to ingroup members compared to outgroup members (Chen and Li 2009;Hewstone 1990). ...

Interindividual–Intergroup Discontinuity Reduction Through the Anticipation of Future Interaction

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... Given the pervasive stereotype of women being more cooperative and men more competitive, exposure to competitive payoff structures could activate gender stereotypes, leading women to self-regulate behaviours according to the stereotype [3]. Evidence from economic games supports the hypothesis that gender differences might primarily emerged in situations involving greater conflict of interests [29], showing that women cooperated significantly more than men only in games that involved a stronger potential for exploitation (Prisoner's Dilemmas versus ultimatum games [30]) [31]. ...

When Groups Are More Competitive Than Individuals: The Domain of the Discontinuity Effect

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... Un individuo valora un sistema de creencias según el grado de acuerdo que muestra con el suyo y evalúa a los demás en función de que sus ideas concuerden con las propias. La mayoría de la investigación generada desde este modelo, durante las décadas de los 70 y 80, ha obtenido resultados que indican que las personas prejuiciosas asumen más diferencias de creencias que las menos prejuiciosas (Byrne y Wong, 1962;Moe, Nacoste e Insko, 1981). ...

Belief versus race as determinants of discrimination: A study of southern adolescents in 1966 and 1979.
  • Citing Article
  • January 1981

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... Moreover, we acknowledge that the outcome of our analyses of aggression intentions were somewhat different than the outcome of actual aggressive behavior. Our analysis of aggressive intentions showed that the classic discontinuity effect (Schopler & Insko, 1999;Wildschut et al., 2007) is observed more when people are ostracized than when included. This moderation of the discontinuity effect, however, was not observed on actual aggressive behavior. ...

Future consequences and the reduction of interindividual-intergroup discontinuity
  • Citing Article
  • January 1998

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... Jacobs & Campbell (1961) found that the overestimation bias induced by the confederates persisted for several generations after the final confederate had been removed. Similar methods, involving the removal and replacement of participants within groups, have since been adopted by Insko et al. (1980 Insko et al. ( , 1983) and also more recently by Baum et al. (2004). The benefit of this method lies not only in the power to manipulate certain key variables (e.g. the size of group and the number of confederates for Jacobs & Campbell 1961), but also in Review. ...

Social evolution and the emergence of leadership
  • Citing Article
  • September 1980

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... This asymmetry may have been due to the type of stimuli that we used (i.e., cookies). Specifically, people have considerable prior knowledge about cookies in general, which could have a large impact given that consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency do not provide all of the covariation information that may be considered relevant for attributions (e.g., Cheng & Novick, 1990;Försterling, 1989;Pruitt & Insko, 1980). For example, they do not specify how other people usually react to other stimuli of the same type. ...

Extension of the Kelley attribution model: The role of comparison-object consensus, target-object consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency
  • Citing Article
  • July 1980

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... Según el Diccionario de la Lengua Española (DLE), un estereotipo es cualquier conjunto de creencias sobregeneralizadas e inmutables relativas a los atributos de un grupo humano. Ese conjunto de creencias conduce a percibir a cada nuevo integrante de ese grupo según la expectativa que se ha construido previamente sobre ellas (Stroebe & Insko, 1989;Gorham, 1999). En pocas palabras, los estereotipos son categorías descriptivas simplificadoras que viabilizan calificaciones de sujetos y de grupos en función de clichés sociales. ...

Stereotype, Prejudice, and Discrimination: Changing Conceptions in Theory and Research
  • Citing Chapter
  • January 1989

... Although not explicitly tested in this study, it is possible that receivers are using the simplest attributional process available; that is, in the absence of contradictory information about the communicator's disposition, receivers apparently discount or ignore other information about the brand (i.e., consensus and consistency) and blame the communicator for the negativity of the message when it is not particularly distinctive. It is interesting to note that a direct linkage between low distinctiveness and communicator attributions is consistent with findings in other settings (e.g., Alicke & Insko, 1984;Bassili & Regan, 1977;Hansen, 1980). ...

Sampling of similar and dissimilar comparison objects as a function of the generality of attribution goal

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... Several studies indicated that choice may have no effect or even a negative effect on performance and adaptive motivation outcomes like effort and interest (Overskeid & Svartdal, 1996;Parker & Lepper, 1992;Reeve, Nix, & Hamm, 2003). Likewise, not all studies from dissonance and attribution theory perspectives have found that greater choice conditions produce greater dissonance and attitude change (Collins, Ashmore, Hornbeck, & Whitney, 1970;Melson, Calder, & Insko, 1969). ...

The social psychological status of reward

Psychonomic Science