Carol L. Gohm’s research while affiliated with University of Mississippi and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (25)


Phased Training for High-Reliability Occupations: Live-Fire Exercises for Civilian Firefighters
  • Article

October 2011

·

51 Reads

·

33 Citations

Human Factors The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society

·

Carol L Gohm

·

The aim of this study was to assess whether the stress reduction effects of phased training culminating in repeated exposure to a stressful scenario generalize to new scenarios. High-reliability occupations require personnel to operate in stressful situations involving complex environments, high degrees of uncertainty and time pressure, and severe consequences for mistakes. One method of training for such environments culminates in practice in high-fidelity, highly stressful simulations. For some domains, realism necessitates large-scale, difficult-to-modify physical simulations. This necessity often results in repeated exposure to one or very few scenarios. The literature gives reason to question whether the stress reduction effects of such exposure transfer to new scenarios. Anxiety and cognitive difficulties were measured among firefighter trainees during three live-fire drills. For each trainee, two drills involved the same scenario, and the other involved a new scenario that was structurally similar to the repeated scenario. As predicted, anxiety and cognitive difficulties decreased across repetitions of the same scenario. However, the reduction did not generalize to a new scenario, and a nontrivial portion of the sample showed signs of negative transfer. Repeated exposure to the same stressful scenario as the final phase of training has limited practical value for stress reduction. Methods for expanding the range of scenarios to which trainees are exposed or for increasing the value of the exposure are recommended. The findings could help improve design of training programs for high-reliability occupations.


Predicted and Experienced Affective Responses to the Outcome of the 2008 U.S. Presidential Election

December 2010

·

18 Reads

·

5 Citations

Psychological Reports

·

Grant C Corser

·

Carol L Gohm

·

[...]

·

Elizabeth L Foreman

People typically have intense feelings about politics. Therefore, it was no surprise that the campaign and eventual election of Barack Obama were highly anticipated and emotionally charged events, making it and the emotion experienced afterward a useful situation in which to replicate prior research showing that people typically overestimate the intensity and duration of their future affective states. Consequently, it was expected that Obama supporters and McCain supporters might overestimate the intensity of their affective responses to the outcome of the election. Data showed that while McCain supporters underestimated how happy they would be following the election, Obama supporters accurately predicted how happy they would be following the election. These data provide descriptive information on the accuracy of people's predicted reactions to the 2008 U.S. presidential election. The findings are discussed in the context of the broad literature and this specific and unique event.


Mean (SD) Validity Ratings and Emotional Response as a Function of Reappraisal Condition in Experiment 1
The effect of reappraising social exclusion on emotional distress
  • Article
  • Full-text available

January 2010

·

61 Reads

·

2 Citations

The purpose of this investigation was to examine whether reappraisal, which is a strategy where the personal meaning of an event is reevaluated, would influence participants' emotional reactions to social exclusion feedback. It was expected that reappraising this event would reduce the emotional distress that accompanies social exclusion, but engaging in this strategy would impair subsequent psychological processes associated with social success. The results showed that reappraising the feedback as an invalid threat reduced the emotional distress; however, there was no evidence that the reappraisal strategy impaired subsequent impression management. This work has theoretical implications for research in emotion and social exclusion.

Download

Mutual Self-Enhancement in Japan and the United States

March 2008

·

677 Reads

·

37 Citations

Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

The authors address the debate on pancultural self-enhancement by suggesting that some Japanese enhance their self-worth by praising and receiving praise from others included in the self. The authors call this process “mutual self-enhancement” and validated a scale for its measurement in Japan and the United States. Mutual self-enhancement was positively correlated with including others in the self, relational self-construal, family support, and sympathy. Mutual self-enhancement was not related to the interdependent self, collectivism, and approval from others, confirming that the process involves including others in the self via self-expansion rather than fitting into an interdependent relationship network. Future studies based on the self-expansion model will consider the inclusion of in-group members in the self as an important part of mutual self-enhancement that predicts well-being in East Asian countries such as Japan.


College Women and Sexual Assault: The Role of Sex-related Alcohol Expectancies

July 2007

·

372 Reads

·

99 Citations

Journal of Family Violence

This study examined the relationship between alcohol, sex-related alcohol expectancies, and sexual assaults among women college students. Participants completed measures of sexual behaviors, sexual victimization experiences, sex related alcohol expectancies, and drinking habits. Based on participants’ responses women were categorized as having experienced no assault, unwanted sexual contact, sexual coercion, attempted rape, and rape. It was observed across groups that relative to controls, women reporting attempted rape and rape consumed higher levels of alcohol. Within group comparisons revealed that relative to controls, victimized women endorsed higher levels of sex-related alcohol expectancies. In the prediction of severity of sexual victimization, regression analyses revealed an interaction between alcohol consumption and expectancy of vulnerability to sexual coercion. At higher levels of alcohol consumption women endorsing high vulnerability to sexual coercion experienced more severe victimatization. Implications of the findings are discussed.


Figure 1: Mean scores of the factors of the Attention to Positive and Negative Information Scale in each culture in Study 1. API, attention to positive information; ANI, attention to negative information; N/O, attention to negative information/others; N/S, attention to negative information/self; P/O, attention to positive information/others; P/S, attention to positive information/self. □, USA; ▪, Japan.
Alpha values of subscales of the Attention to Positive and Negative Information Scale in the first and the second study
Correlations between attention to positive information and attention to negative information
Cultural differences related to positive and negative valence

May 2007

·

402 Reads

·

17 Citations

Asian Journal of Social Psychology

Differences between North American and East Asian cultures were examined in terms of the valence of psychological constructs. Americans were more likely than Japanese to focus on positive things. In contrast, Japanese (vs Americans) were more likely to attend to negative information of the self, but not more or less likely to focus on negative things about others. Based on within-culture analyses, the Americans' data were better described by their tendency to focus on positive things over negative things than by their tendency for self-enhancement. In contrast, the Japanese data were better described by their self-critical tendency. This result was replicated in a second study. In addition, correlations between constructs with opposite valences were negative in the USA, but positive or absent in Japan.


TABLE 1 . Means and Standard Deviations for Distress, Though Suppression, & Health 
TABLE 3 . Means and Standard Deviations for Last Day of Writing Questionnaire (LDWQ) 
Revisiting Written Disclosure: The Effects of Warm Versus Cold Experimenters

May 2007

·

85 Reads

·

4 Citations

Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology

Early writing paradigm studies suggested that people who write about emotional or traumatic events accrue psychological and physiological benefits. However, recent studies suggest that a number of variables may play a role in determining when, and for whom, writing is beneficial. The current study examined the impact of experimenter demeanor in this paradigm. Though participants who disclosed a traumatic event after interacting with a warm experimenter were more engaged in the writing task and more likely to report gaining insight than those in other conditions, they did not demonstrate the expected psychological or physical benefits. In fact, they reported significantly more distress at follow-up than others. Conversely, those participants who disclosed a trauma after interacting with a cold, distant experimenter did not experience increases in distress or report a significant amount of insight gained as the result of written disclosure.


Fig. 2. (A) Models with attention not mediating the relationship between trait and aVect. (B) Models with attention mediating the relationship between trait and aVect.  
Factor loadings of the revised attention to Positive and Negative Information Scale
Cognitive tendencies of focusing on positive and negative information

December 2006

·

2,789 Reads

·

117 Citations

Journal of Research in Personality

We hypothesized individual differences in the tendency to attend to, think about, and focus on positive (or negative) information. A scale measuring these individual differences was constructed and its validity and reliability were examined in three studies. Attention to positive information was related to positive affectivity, extraversion, BAS, optimism. Attention to negative information was related to negative affectivity, neuroticism, BIS, and optimism (inversely). Validity was partially confirmed by a person perception task: individuals high in attention to positive information perceived the character in a story as happy. Attention to positive information was a partial mediator of the relation between extraversion and positive affect and attention to negative information was a partial mediator of the relation between neuroticism and negative affect.


Validity of the Asperger Syndrome Diagnostic Scale

June 2006

·

432 Reads

·

20 Citations

Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities

Until publication of the Asperger Syndrome Diagnostic Scale (ASDS) in 2001, no standardized and nationally-normed instrument existed specifically for the purpose of diagnosing Asperger syndrome (AS) [Myles etal., 2001. Asperger Syndrome Diagnostic Scale ASDS), PRO-ED, Austin]. This study sought to augment the existing psychometric data on the ASDS through examination of its divergent, convergent, and discriminative validity. Measures of AS, autism, and social skills competence were administered to 76 children with AS, autism, intellectually gifted children, and a control group of typically developing children without AS or autism. Results supported the divergent and convergent validity of the ASDS. Discriminative validity was partially supported. The ASDS was the best discriminator between children with AS and those without AS. Diagnostic accuracy for those with AS and autism was enhanced when scores on the ASDS were combined with scores on the Gilliam Autism Rating Scale (GARS) and the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS). Findings are discussed relative to the debate over the external validity of AS and the resulting limitations on research in the area.


An Examination of Sexual Violence Against College Women

April 2006

·

359 Reads

·

278 Citations

Violence Against Women

This investigation examined college women's experiences with unwanted sexual contact. Participants completed a questionnaire assessing the incidence of various types of forced sexual contact the women had experienced since enrolling in college. Demographic and situational variables associated with these incidents of sexual violence were also obtained. It was observed that since enrolling in college, 27% of the sample had experienced unwanted sexual contact ranging from kissing and petting to oral, anal, or vaginal intercourse. Type of sexual violence, perpetrator characteristics, and racial differences regarding types of unwanted sexual contact were examined. The implications of the data are discussed.


Citations (24)


... Often referred to in mindfulness as being the 'quiet observer' or 'peripheral observer' , this phenomenon consists of an ability to process and be aware of emotions (and indeed, all human experiences) from a place of detachment, allowing for deeper understanding and processing of emotions, which is likely aligned with emotional clarity. This suggestion is also consistent with previous research suggesting 'overwhelm' with the high attention pairing 56 . Without a balanced level of emotional clarity to calibrate the harmful effects of emotional attention alone, it seems sensical that one may find themself in a negative feedback loop for distressing or harmful thoughts, which over time, may exacerbate and result in common mental health symptoms. ...

Reference:

Emotional self-knowledge profiles and relationships with mental health indicators support value in ‘knowing thyself’
Mood Regulation and Emotional Intelligence: Individual Differences

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... Ek olarak, duygusal zekâ kavramının hem yetenek hem de özellik olarak kavramsallaştırılması, kavramın ölçülmesine ilişkin sorunları da beraberinde getirmektedir (Davies, Stankov veRoberts, 1998;Landy, 2005;Locke, 2005; Zeidner, Roberts ve Matthews, 2004). Tüm bunlar, duygusal zekânın alanyazındaki birçok konuda uygun olmayan şekilde kullanılmasına neden olmaktadır (Daus ve Ashkanasy, 2005;Gohm, 2004). ...

Moving forward with emotional intelligence
  • Citing Article
  • January 2004

Psychological Inquiry

... Kim et al., 2020;Riggs & Riggs, 2011;Vandeleur et al., 2009). Whether parents adopt warm accepting or harsh rejecting approaches to parenting can impact children's perceptions and feelings toward their family (Akhlaq et al., 2013;Edwards & Lopez, 2006;Gohm et al., 1998;Rask et al., 2003). Bearing this in mind and considering the value placed on familism and close filial bonds among Koreans in general, family functioning may have greater salience for childhood development in families with a collectivistic than individualistic orientation (Gomez et al., 2011;McWhiter et al., 2007). ...

Culture, Parental Conflict, Parental Marital Status, and the Subjective Well-Being of Young Adults
  • Citing Article
  • May 1998

Journal of Marriage and Family

... People who are more aware of their emotions tend to exhibit better emotion regulation (Grewal et al., 2006;McFarland and Buehler, 1997). Individuals good at identifying their emotions are more likely to respond actively and plan responses to emotional events, reconstructing and reappraising them positively (Gohm and Clore, 2002;Salovey et al., 1995;Wang et al., 2019). People with high emotional clarity focus on the event triggering the emotion and deal with it actively rather than being overwhelmed by emotions. ...

Four latent traits of emotional experience and their involvement in well-being, coping, and attributional style

... Researchers from several theoretical perspectives have proposed that underachievement stems from low valuation for school or negative attitudes toward academics. Historically, this has taken the form of underachieving students being described as "lacking" motivation for school or academic endeavors (Ford et al., 2005;Van Boxtel & Mönks, 1992), with the cause posited to be an insufficiently unchallenging or irrelevant academic curriculum (Ford, 1992(Ford, , 1993Gohm et al., 1998;Kanevsky & Keighley, 2003;Neihart, 2006;Rea, 2000;Reis & McCoach, 2000;Whitmore, 1986). Some research has reported lower general task valuation or value in engaging in school and academics is linked with underachievement (Baslanti & McCoach, 2006). ...

Underachievement Among Spatially Gifted Students
  • Citing Article
  • September 1998

American Educational Research Journal

... To evaluate attitudes toward using computers, we adapted the Computer Attitudes subscale of the well-validated Computer Aversion, Attitudes, and Familiarity Index (CAAFI) [38][39][40]. The subscale consists of 9 items asking participants to select the response, ranging from -3 (absolutely false) to 3 (absolutely true), that best describes how true or false a statement is to them. ...

The Computer Aversion, Attitudes, and Familiarity Index (CAAFI): A Measure for the Study of Computer-Related Constructs

Journal of Educational Computing Research

... First, concerning the first pathway focused on the emotion regulation of parents, it has been proven that parents are direct and indirect models in the comprehension and communication of emotions for their children [16]. In the field of emotion regulation of parents, emotional clarity emerges as one of the central aspects [17], and is understood as the ability to identify, recognize, discriminate, and understand one's emotions [18]. Emotional clarity is identified as a phase that comes prior to any process of emotional modeling from parents to children (for reviews see [19,20]). ...

Individual Differences in Emotional Experience: Mapping Available Scales to Processes

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

... This mindset enables empathetic and patient responses during conflicts and facilitates open and effective communication that strengthens emotional bonds and mutual support. Positive aspects of self-compassion also reduce stress and prevent resentment, promote a harmonious and loving relationship, and ultimately improve emotional intimacy and marital longevity [38][39][40]. The study hypothesized that positive aspects of self-compassion (self-kindness, common humanity, and mindfulness) would be positively cor- No. related with various well-being indices, while negative aspects would yield poorer positive or negative correlations. ...

Similarity of the Relations between Marital Status and Subjective Well-Being Across Cultures

Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

... Evidence regarding emotional clarity points to negative relationships with transdiagnostic symptomatology [18][19][20][21][22] , and its opposite is also supported such that high emotional clarity is directly linked to a broad range of positive adjustment such as adaptive attributional style and wellbeing 16,23,24 . Experts suggest that perceiving and understanding emotions requires less cognitive control and allows for more resources to be directed toward goal-oriented cognition and behavior, while those who must spend greater time and effort managing their emotional experiences may not have resources left 'in the tank' to dedicate toward self-actualization 16,23,25 . ...

Affect as Information: An Individual Differences Approach

... Previous research points out the association between collectivism and empathy (e.g., Dalsky et al. (2008)). This is because giving and receiving sympathy (the acknowledgment of another person's feelings), closely related to empathy, is theorized to be more closely tied to well-being within interdependent than independent cultural contexts (Kitayama et al. 1995). ...

Mutual Self-Enhancement in Japan and the United States

Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology