C. Peter Herman’s research while affiliated with University of Toronto and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (222)


Evidence of a self-serving bias in people’s attributions for their food intake
  • Article

June 2024

·

9 Reads

Appetite

Lenny R. Vartanian

·

Natalie M. Reily

·

·

[...]

·

Janet Polivy

Influence of social-normative information on the modeling of food-related decisions

May 2022

·

50 Reads

·

1 Citation

Appetite

Social modeling is a powerful influence on people's food intake: When there is a clear and consistent norm, people eat more when eating with someone who eats a lot and eat less when eating with someone who eats only a little. In three studies, the present research examined how clear versus ambiguous social-normative information influences the modeling of food-related decisions. Using a novel online decision-making paradigm, female participants (total N = 1042) were provided with information about how many cookies previous participants had supposedly selected (no information was provided in the control condition), and then decided how many cookies they would choose for a snack. When there was a clear and consistent norm, the typical social modeling effect was observed. When there was a small number of “dissenters” whose responses conflicted with the norm set by the majority, participants' cookie selection still conformed to the behaviour of the majority (Studies 1 and 2). It was only when the behaviour of the previous participants was highly ambiguous that participants behaved as if they had been given no normative information (Study 3). By demonstrating that, except in extreme cases, people use the available information to discern a social norm that influences their behaviour, these findings highlight the power of social norms related to food-related decisions.



What is restrained eating and how do we identify it?

August 2020

·

296 Reads

·

83 Citations

Appetite

Defining and measuring such qualities as restrained eating or dieting may require more than simply administering questionnaires and assuming that we are identifying the population that we wish to study. Different questionnaires may identify different types of restrained eaters, and even deciding what restrained eating consists of is a complicated endeavor. We discuss how to define and measure restrained eating, specifying key attributes, and acknowledging the problems inherent in relying on self-report instruments. We conclude that given the difficulties in defining such constructs as restrained eating, we need to specify more clearly exactly what our research questions are, in order to be sure that we are identifying the populations with the attributes necessary to answer those questions.


Modeling of food intake among restrained and unrestrained eaters

July 2020

·

55 Reads

·

8 Citations

Appetite

The Theory of Normal Eating suggests that how much others eat sets an upper limit for how much it is appropriate to eat. This study tested the hypothesis that restrained eaters, who typically eat less than they want to, would be more responsive to a high-intake norm than would unrestrained eaters. Data were combined from 8 experimental studies (total N = 735 female participants; 305 restrained eaters, 430 unrestrained eaters). Each study: (a) included a low-intake norm, a high-intake norm, and a no-norm control condition; (b) measured participants’ food intake; and (c) included the Restraint Scale as a measure of dietary restraint. There were no differences between restrained unrestrained eaters in the no-norm control condition or in the magnitude of the inhibition effect (i.e., the difference between the low-intake norm condition and the control condition). There was, however, a restraint difference in the magnitude of the augmentation effect (i.e., the difference between the high-intake norm condition and the control condition). Restrained eaters showed a larger augmentation effect (d = 0.58; 95% CI = 0.29, 0.87) than did unrestrained eaters (d = 0.20; 95% CI = −0.05, 0.45). Social norms provide an upper limit for acceptable food intake, with high-intake norms permitting (but not requiring) individuals to indulge themselves. The fact that restrained eaters were more responsive to the high-intake norm than were unrestrained eaters suggests that the high-intake norm gives restrained eaters permission to indulge when they typically eat less than they want to.



What does it mean to overeat?

May 2020

·

59 Reads

·

5 Citations

Eating Behaviors

“Overeating” is a significant public health concern, but little is known about how lay people conceptualize overeating. This study explored participants' conceptions of overeating. Participants were 175 university students and 296 community members (56% women) who were asked to rate the extent to which several statements reflected the concept of “overeating” (1 = Does not capture the meaning at all; 7 = Captures the meaning perfectly). Results showed that eating outside of hunger was viewed as being most strongly linked to the conceptualization of overeating; mindless eating, eating more than some normative amount, and a lack of restriction were all moderately linked to the conceptualization of overeating; and the quality of the food/eating style was rated as least relevant to the conceptualization of overeating. Participants with a higher BMI, participants who perceived themselves as fat, restrained eaters, and participants with an indication of eating pathology rated all of the constructs as more relevant to the concept of overeating than did their respective counterparts, but their overall pattern of responding was the same. The present research provides some initial insights into people's thoughts on what it means to “overeat,” which is important given that simply believing that one has overeaten (regardless of the actual amount consumed) can have adverse psychological and behavioral consequences. These insights provide an avenue for future research to explore whether lay conceptions could be more adaptively reshaped to reduce the negative effects of perceived overeating.


Overeating in Restrained and Unrestrained Eaters
  • Article
  • Full-text available

March 2020

·

779 Reads

·

39 Citations

Download

Modeling of Food Intake

September 2019

·

32 Reads

·

2 Citations

Modeling of food intake refers to the tendency for people to adjust the amount of food they eat to approximate the amount eaten by their eating companion. People eat more when eating with someone who eats a lot, and they eat less when eating with someone who eat very little. In this chapter, we summarize the research documenting the modeling effect. We also examine the potential impact of factors such as how hungry people are, their gender, and their body weight/size. Finally, we consider potential explanations for why people model the food intake of others, including the desire to affiliate with others and the social norms that are communicated by the other person’s behavior.


A Theory of Normal Eating—Reprise and Non-social Examples

September 2019

·

38 Reads

·

1 Citation

Research on modeling, consumption stereotypes, and impression management is consistent with our theory emphasizing norms of appropriateness. In this chapter we turn to some bodies of research that are less obviously related to norms of appropriateness; however, we believe they are related. The basic idea is that people can figure out what and how much it is appropriate to eat in a particular situation even when there are other people are not available to tell them or show them what is appropriate. Research on portion size shows that as the size of a portion increases so does the amount that people eat. We believe that portion size is an indirect social cue; presumably, “someone” has decided that a portion of a particular size is appropriate, so we use that cue to determine how much we should eat. People’s idea of appropriateness can also come from cultural knowledge about how eating is generally structured throughout a day, in terms of meals versus snacks. Meals and snacks tend to alternate, and meals tend to be large in relation to snacks. Thus, after eating a meal, it is appropriate to eat relatively lightly on the next eating occasion; after eating a snack, it is appropriateto eat relatively heavily on the next eating occasion. Research has shown that when people are induced to interpret a set of foods they are eating as a meal, they eat less on the next eating occasion than do those who are induced to interpret exactly the same set of foods as a snack. Similarly, beliefs about the caloric content of a food or foods can serve as an appropriateness cue. If we are eating something we believe to be highly caloric, our cultural knowledge tells us that it is appropriate to eat less of it than if we believe it to be relatively low in calories—and this can be independent of its actual caloric value. And finally, simply remembering or being reminded of what we have eaten in the recent past can affect subsequent eating—again, presumably because our cultural knowledge tells us what is appropriate on that later occasion.


Citations (78)


... La alimentación restrictiva (ARE) se caracteriza por limitar o excluir ciertos alimentos o grupos de alimentos de la dieta, con el objetivo de controlar la mantención o disminución del peso corporal (Van Strien, 1986). Un foco relevante de la ARE radica en el efecto de desinhibición, es decir, comer en exceso, posterior al abandono de la limitación de la ingesta (Polivy et al., 2020). En este contexto, algunas investigaciones han vinculado este EAD con mayores niveles del índice de masa corporal (IMC) (Andrés, 2017;Barthels, 2019, Cebolla, 2014. ...

Reference:

Estilos de alimentación desadaptativos y conductas alimentarias de riesgo en Chile
What is restrained eating and how do we identify it?
  • Citing Article
  • August 2020

Appetite

... Consumers follow the lead of their eating companions both in food intake (Vartanian et al., 2015;Herman et al., 2019) and in food waste (Wansink and Van Ittersum, 2013). This is especially true of "unrestrained eaters," who have no moderating inhibitions (such as dieting or cost constraints) that may otherwise limit their consumption (Vartanian et al., 2020). In the case of buffet restaurants, where customers eat their fill for a fixed price, over-consumption and food waste at the end of the meal are driven by both financial considerations and communal behaviours. ...

Modeling of food intake among restrained and unrestrained eaters
  • Citing Article
  • July 2020

Appetite

... Mindlessness is characterized by carelessness and a lack of thought, attention, or focus. The relationship between mindless eating and LOC eating has been controversial, with studies suggesting a clear boundary between the two disordered eating behaviors(Carey et al., 2017;Long et al., 2020) and others indicating close correlations.Examples of the correlations include, first, mindless and LOC eating both reflect deficits in self-regulation(Framson et al., 2009). Second, quantitative studies have reported a negative association between mindful eating and uncontrolled eating(Peitz & Warschburger, 2022), and mindfulness-based interventions have been shown to significantly reduce LOC eating(Alberts et al., 2010). ...

What does it mean to overeat?
  • Citing Article
  • May 2020

Eating Behaviors

... Numerous studies have explored strategies for regulating food intake. Previous studies have explored the social consequences of eating disorders and overeating (Florack et al., 2013;Polivy and Herman, 2020;Versluis and Papies, 2016). Hollands et al. (2015) demonstrated that reducing portion size effectively decreased the amount of food consumed. ...

Overeating in Restrained and Unrestrained Eaters

... We examined these relationships in a sample of 4,382 undergraduate students, 98% of whom were emerging adults (aged [18][19][20][21][22][23][24][25][26][27][28][29], with enough men (1,754) to provide a basis to compare men and women with adequate statistical power. Sampling a large number of men is important because men are under-represented in research on restrained eating. ...

Effects of Social Eating
  • Citing Chapter
  • September 2019

... The meal purpose may have great influence on food waste, and previous studies pointed out that plate waste was found to be higher for social diners (like social and business gatherings) than food waste for work or private dining experience (Wang et al., 2017;Cao et al., 2014;Yi-Chi Chang et al., 2022). It was because that individuals may be more susceptible to social cues and consume (and waste) more food in social activities, according to the theory of "Normal Eating" (Herman et al., 2019). ...

A Theory of Normal Eating
  • Citing Chapter
  • September 2019

... Finally, for character demographics one hypothesis was prespecified concerning character gender. Owing to gender stereotypes about healthy eating (Cavazza et al., 2015;Vartanian et al., 2007) and national patterns that American women eat healthier than men (Rehm et al., 2016), the seventh hypothesis predicted that male characters would consume less healthy foods than female characters. In addition to these seven preregistered hypotheses, a number of exploratory measures related to the context (e.g., urban vs. rural setting, local setting such as the home, grocery store, or restaurant), character behaviors (additional linguistic analyses of character speech), and character demographics (e.g., race/ethnicity, age) were quantified and presented as exploratory. ...

Consumption Stereotypes and Impression Management: Food Choice
  • Citing Chapter
  • September 2019

... Recent insights suggest that standards about what is normal and socially acceptable to consume can be inferred from physical aspects of food environments, even in the absence of a person being told or shown what and how much to eat (Burger et al., 2010;Herman, Polivy, Pliner, & Vartanian, 2019;Prinsen, de Ridder, & de Vet, 2013;Raghoebar, Haynes, Robinson, Van Kleef, & De Vet, 2019;Raghoebar, van Rongen, et al., 2019). To illustrate, it has been shown that consumers interpret the amount of food served in a given situation as a social cue that determines their food intake 24 h later, as they are likely to believe that the served portion size was not chosen at random by the food provider. ...

A Theory of Normal Eating—Reprise and Non-social Examples
  • Citing Chapter
  • September 2019

... They depend, inter alia, on biological and social factors. Their modeling may result from the preferences for eating in accordance with social norms, create a sense of security and belonging (Herman, 2019). The changing needs of consumers influence the adaptation of gastronomy to them through the continuous development of new trends (Bilska et al., 2014). ...

Modeling of Food Choice
  • Citing Chapter
  • September 2019

... Parental food behaviour and friends' social pressure were considered to have both positive and negative influences on individual eating habits [42]. Much of this food choice literature identified the importance of social factors and social norms [43][44]. ...

Social Influences on Eating
  • Citing Book
  • January 2019