Bruce Abernethy’s research while affiliated with The University of Queensland and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (368)


Healthy People 2010
  • Chapter

February 2024

·

9 Reads

·

1 Citation

Cindy H. P. Sit

·

·

Bruce Abernethy

·




Moderate-to-vigorous physical activity and sedentary behavior in children with and without developmental coordination disorder: Associations with fundamental movement skills

November 2021

·

71 Reads

·

27 Citations

Research in Developmental Disabilities

Background There is a pandemic of physical inactivity in children. Compared to children with typical development (TD), those with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) are less physically active. Mastery of movement skills such as fundamental movement skills (FMS) are potential correlates of physical activity. Aims To examine the associations of FMS with moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA) and sedentary behavior (SB) in children with DCD and TD. Methods and procedures This cross-sectional study included 172 children with DCD (n = 73; boys = 49) or TD (n = 99; boys = 48) aged 6–10 years. Five components of FMS (running, jumping, throwing, catching, kicking) were assessed using process- and product- oriented approaches using the Test of Gross Motor Development-second edition. The time spent in MVPA and SB was measured using accelerometers. Outcomes and results Children with DCD had significantly poorer performance in FMS proficiency in terms of specific movement patterns (jumping and catching) and outcomes (running, jumping, catching, and kicking) when compared to children with TD. MVPA and SB were significantly associated with certain process-oriented FMS assessments in children, which was moderated by motor coordination status and sex. Movement patterns of catching (odds ratio = 1.686, p < .05) was a significant predictor of children’s attainment of the MVPA guideline. Conclusions and implications It is important to develop FMS patterns and ball skills in early childhood, particularly for children with DCD, to combat physical inactivity and its related health problems.


Figure 1. Screenshot of each spatial frequency video clip: (a) normal SF information, (b) high SF information only, and (c) low SF information only.
Figure 2. Mean response accuracy (%) one frame before shuttle-racquet contact and at contact in deceptive trials. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Dotted line represents chance level response accuracy. SF ¼ spatial frequency. Note: Please refer to the online version of the article to view the figures in colour.
Falling for a Fake: The Role of Kinematic and Non-kinematic Information in Deception Detection
  • Article
  • Full-text available

March 2019

·

534 Reads

·

14 Citations

Perception

Kinematic and non-kinematic visual information have been examined in the context of movement anticipation by athletes, although less so in deception detection. This study examined the role of kinematic and non-kinematic visual information in the anticipation of deceptive and non-deceptive badminton shots. Skilled (n = 12) and less skilled (n = 12) badminton players anticipated the direction of deceptive and non-deceptive shots presented via video footage displayed in normal (kinematic and non-kinematic information), low (kinematic information emphasized), and high (non-kinematic information emphasized) spatial frequency conditions. Each shot was occluded one frame before shuttle-racquet contact or at contact. In deceptive trials, skilled players showed decreased anticipation accuracy in the high spatial frequency condition (p = .050) compared to normal and low spatial frequency conditions, which did not differ. The study suggests that an emphasis on kinematic information results in accurate anticipation in response to deceptive movements and that an emphasis on non-kinematic information results in less accurate anticipation by experts.

Download

Figure 1. Age structure of the career of Super-Elite (above) and Elite athletes (below). CS = championship. Mean values (standard deviations omitted for clarity).
Developmental Biographies of Olympic Super- Elite and Elite Athletes: A Multidisciplinary Pattern Recognition Analysis

March 2019

·

2,723 Reads

·

38 Citations

International Journal of Environment and Health

This multidisciplinary study used pattern recognition analyses to examine the developmental biographies of 16 Great British Olympic and World Champions ("Super-Elite") and 16 matched international athletes who had not won major medals ("Elite"). Athlete, coach and parent interviews (260 total interview hours) combined in-depth qualitative and quantitative methods. A combination of demographics, psychosocial characteristics, coach and family relationships, practice, competition, and performance development discriminated Super-Elite from Elite athletes with > 90% accuracy. Compared to Elite athletes, Super-Elite athletes were characterized by: (1) An early critical negative life experience in close proximity to significant positive sport-related events; (2) higher relative importance of sport over other aspects of life, stronger obsessiveness/perfectionism, and sport-related ruthlessness/selfishness; (3) conjoint outcome and mastery focus, and use of counterphobic and/or "total preparation" strategies to maintain/enhance performance under pressure; (4) coaches who better met their physical and psychosocial needs; (5) coming back after severe performance setbacks during adulthood, and career "turning points" leading to enhanced determination to excel; (6) more pronounced diversified youth sport engagement, and prolonged extensive sport-specific practice and competitions; and (7) continued performance improvement over more years during adulthood, eventually attaining their (first) gold medal after 21 ± 6 practice years. The findings are discussed relative to potential causal interactions and theoretical implications.


Stepovers and Signal Detection: Response Sensitivity and Bias in the Differentiation of Genuine and Deceptive Football Actions

October 2018

·

186 Reads

·

29 Citations

The ability to differentiate genuine and deceptive actions was examined using a combination of spatial and temporal occlusion to examine sensitivity to lower body, upper body, and full body sources of visual information. High-skilled and low-skilled association football players judged whether a player genuinely intended to take the ball to the participant’s left or right or intended to step over the ball then take it in the other direction. Signal detection analysis was used to calculate measures of sensitivity (d′) in differentiating genuine and deceptive actions and bias (c) toward judging an action to be genuine or deceptive. Analysis revealed that high-skilled players had higher sensitivity than low-skilled players and this was consistent across all spatial occlusion conditions. Low-skilled players were more biased toward judging actions to be genuine. Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) curves revealed that accuracy on deceptive trials in the lower body and full body conditions most accurately classified participants as high-skilled or low-skilled. The results highlight the value of using signal detection analysis in studies of deceptive actions. They suggest that information from the lower body or upper body was sufficient for differentiating genuine and deceptive actions and that global information concurrently derived from these sources was not necessary to support the expert advantage.


Demonstration of each of the three spatial frequency stimuli used in the training intervention; (a) Normal-SF information, (b) low-SF information only, and (c) high-SF information only.
Mean response accuracy and response time for deceptive (A,B) and non-deceptive (C,D) trials. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean.
Mean fixation duration (A) and mean saccadic amplitude (B). Percentage of total viewing time toward each of four key AoIs for each group (C). To reduce complexity, only the four most frequently fixated AoIs are shown: head, racquet, torso, and location of contact. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean.
Mean response accuracy (A) and response time (B) during training intervention. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean.
The Perception of Deceptive Information Can Be Enhanced by Training That Removes Superficial Visual Information

August 2018

·

404 Reads

·

29 Citations

The ability to detect deceptive intent within actions is a crucial element of skill across many tasks. Evidence suggests that deceptive actions may rely on the use of superficial visual information to hide the basic kinematic information which specifies the actor’s intent. The purpose of this study was to determine whether the ability of observers to anticipate deceptive actions could be enhanced by training which removes superficial visual information. Novice badminton players (n = 36) were allocated to one of three groups who performed perceptual training over 3 days, with the efficacy of training assessed using tests of anticipatory skill conducted at pre-test, post-test, and a 1-week retention test. During training, participants watched a series of non-deceptive badminton shots performed by actors, with the footage manipulated to display either (i) low spatial-frequency information only (low-SF training group; blurring to remove superficial information); (ii) high spatial-frequency information only (high-SF training group; an ‘edge detector’ to highlight superficial information); or (iii) normal vision (normal-SF group). Participants were asked to anticipate the direction of the shuttle when footage was occluded at the moment of racquet-shuttle contact. In the post-test, response accuracy (RA) when viewing deceptive trials was higher for the low-SF training group when compared to the normal-SF (control) training group (p = 0.005), with the difference retained in the retention test (p = 0.020). High-SF training resulted in greater performance at post-test (p = 0.038) but not retention (p = 0.956). The analysis of gaze provided some explanation for the findings, with the low-SF training group spending more time after training fixating on the location of racquet-shuttle contact than did the normal training group (p = 0.028). The findings demonstrate that training which conveys only the basic kinematic movements visible in low-SF information may be effective in learning to ‘see-through’ deceptive intent.


Fig. 1. Schematic design of the theoretically-grounded MPTF that hypothesises the predicted transfer effect of MPT tools on competitive performance. The MPTF's volumetric space, created by three continua, captures the possible states of interaction between the three key MPT design factors that may influence their effect on improving performance; targeted perceptual function (y-axis, low-order to high-order), stimulus correspondence (x-axis, generic to sport-specific) and response correspondence (z-axis, generic to sportspecific). Increasing stimulus correspondence, conveyed by small left-hand arrowheads and large right-hand arrowheads on the x-axis, resets at the origin allowing generic stimuli with high behavioural correspondence to be distinguished from sport-based stimuli with low behavioural correspondence. The MPTF's key hypothesis is indicated by the expanding double-broken arrow-as each factor approaches maximal correspondence with its function and existence in competitive sport environments, MPT should provide stronger transfer effects. 
Fig. 2. Approximated schematic plot of exemplar MPT tools overlayed on the MPTF. For clarity, the MPTF has been divided based on stimulus correspondence; part (a) portrays all MPT tools comprised of generic (low visual correspondence) stimuli, part (b) portrays all MPT tools comprised of sport-based (high visual correspondence) stimuli. The z-axis gridlines have been labelled according to the different classifications of MPT response modes. MPT tools/approaches portrayed as a region indicate that within the one approach, depending on the region's directional characteristic, either multiple perceptual skills may be targeted and/or multiple stimulus types and/or response modes may be used. 
Modified perceptual training in sport: A new classification framework

February 2018

·

3,961 Reads

·

169 Citations

Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport

Objectives: To overview a framework that provides a theoretically-grounded approach to predicting the types of modified perceptual training tasks that will stimulate transfer of improved perceptual skills to sport performance environments. Modified perceptual training (MPT) collectively describes on- or off-field sports training tasks that are specifically designed to develop visual and perceptual-cognitive skill. Traditional training approaches in sport include sports vision training and perceptual-cognitive training, while recently, new technologies have enabled a broad range of additional MPT tools to become available to coaches and athletes. Design: Short literature review and opinion article. Methods: Literature in the fields of sports vision training and perceptual-cognitive training are summarised and contrasted. A selection of emerging MPT technologies are then overviewed. This leads to the identification of three interacting factors of MPT task design that may influence the task's capacity to transfer improved training performance to actual competition: (i) the targeted perceptual function, (ii) stimulus correspondence, and (iii) response correspondence, which are assimilated with key tenets of representative learning design. Results: These three theoretically-grounded differences are adopted to support and justify the structure of the Modified Perceptual Training Framework which sets out predictions for future research to test in order to clarify the transfer effect of MPT tools. Conclusions: The application of the Modified Perceptual Training Framework may assist in future testing, design and selection of beneficial training tools in sport and as such, is predicted to have significant impact in empirical and practical settings.


Reduced attentional focus and the influence on expert anticipatory perception

October 2017

·

106 Reads

·

6 Citations

Attention Perception & Psychophysics

The anticipatory memory encodings of expert and novice basketball players were examined under conditions of both full (attended condition) and reduced (unattended condition) attention (see also Gorman, Abernethy, & Farrow in Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 835–844, 2013a). Participants completed a typical pattern recall task using dynamic playing sequences from basketball, and their responses were compared to both the original target pattern as well as to the series of patterns that occurred immediately after and immediately before the target image. The latter had not previously been employed in a pattern recall task when examining the anticipatory encoding of pattern information. Results revealed that the overall extent of the forward displacement for both the attended and unattended patterns was generally significantly greater for the experts, with the expert advantage tending to be most prominent for the attacking patterns. The novel addition of both forward and backward scenes may provide a more precise measure of the anticipatory effect, suggesting that future research in this domain should use a similar methodological design.


Citations (59)


... This study draws attention to adult elite athletes' experiences of the meaningfulness of using physiological tests for their athlete development during the transition from youth talent in the investment years (age 16+, see Côté and Abernethy, 2012) to elite athletes in adulthood. Developing talented youths in sport is about providing a learning environment that supports the process of developing sporting abilities (Baker et al., 2017;Vaeyens et al., 2008) and the transition from junior to top-level sports (Henriksen, 2010). ...

Reference:

The role of physiological testing for athlete development in sport: The elite athlete perspective
A Developmental Approach to Sport Expertise
  • Citing Chapter
  • November 2012

... This discrepancy underscores the detrimental impact of excessive perfectionistic concern over mistakes and the potential benefits of moderate levels of this trait. Such results may help elucidate the complex role of maladaptive perfectionism within an academic setting (e.g., Hill et al., 2018;Stoeber, 2012). These insights also highlight the importance of considering both the type (e.g., adaptive versus maladaptive) and degree (i.e., specific threshold values) of perfectionism to fully understand its effects. ...

The Oxford Handbook of Sport and Performance Psychology

... Threats included emphasis on academic • Device-measured data: 73 of 88 children with DCD aged 6-10 years had valid data. Two of 73 (2.74%) children met the benchmark (Sit, Masters, et al., 2017;Yu et al., 2021). ...

Physical activity and fundamental movement skills in children with developmental coordination disorder: abridged secondary publication
  • Citing Article
  • June 2022

Hong Kong Medical Journal

... Our study found that physical activity was significantly related to FMS in elementary school children. These results are supported by previous studies [6,[28][29][30]. For example, Odraska et al. [28] found that 91 children's leisure physical activity time was significantly associated with FMS performance. ...

Moderate-to-vigorous physical activity and sedentary behavior in children with and without developmental coordination disorder: Associations with fundamental movement skills
  • Citing Article
  • November 2021

Research in Developmental Disabilities

... This questionnaire is based on eight major items: demographics, sociology, important ages in the career, competition history, psychosocial characteristics, volume of practice, athlete services, and traumatic events. This questionnaire is based on studies [1,140], as well as on shared experiences with coaches and performance directors from federations responsible for these topics. Today, among all this diverse information, a reductionist tendency may be why some identification programs fail [20]. ...

Developmental Biographies of Olympic Super- Elite and Elite Athletes: A Multidisciplinary Pattern Recognition Analysis

International Journal of Environment and Health

... Some studies suggest that experienced athletes perform similarly or even better in anticipation performance under blurry video conditions compared to normal conditions 46,47 , attributing this to the enhanced motion perception brought about by visual blur. Moreover, video training focused on low spatial frequency (such as blurred action outlines) has been found to better enhance the retention of deceptive movement recognition in badminton players compared to normal video training 11,48 . This may be because participants spend less time focusing on high spatial frequency information (such as facial details of the opponent) and more time on other key areas (like the contact area between the racket and shuttlecock). ...

Falling for a Fake: The Role of Kinematic and Non-kinematic Information in Deception Detection

Perception

... In such situations, a vast amount of information must be gathered, selected, and processed in a split second to make decisions resulting in successful performances. Such fast decision-making (DM) can be observed, for example, when a striker in soccer suddenly decides to make an unusual move to surprise the defender (Jackson et al., 2018;Wright et al., 2013) or when a goalkeeper in handball attempts to predict the direction of a penalty to initiate an appropriate reaction (e.g., Helm et al., 2020;Mann et al., 2014). Due to the high temporal demands in such DM situations, it oftentimes seems as if skilled athletes automatically and almost effortlessly choose the right move, pass or action, resulting in successful performance outcomes (Raab, 2012). ...

Stepovers and Signal Detection: Response Sensitivity and Bias in the Differentiation of Genuine and Deceptive Football Actions

... Two articles examined perceptual training on deceptive actions in handball (Alsharji & Wade, 2016) or badminton (Ryu et al., 2018). While Alsharji and Wade (2016) used signaling to highlight diagnostic visual cues, Ryu et al. (2018) used video editing techniques that highlighted cues relevant to movement. ...

The Perception of Deceptive Information Can Be Enhanced by Training That Removes Superficial Visual Information

... The analysis of perceptual-motor performance in athletes should account for various factors, including the intrinsic characteristics of specific sports (Erickson, 2021b). Tools that integrate eye-hand and eye-foot coordination have demonstrated potential for sport-specific assessments, enabling the incorporation of sport-specific actions into the evaluation or training of athletes (Appelbaum & Erickson, 2016;Hadlow et al., 2018). However, assessing perceptual-motor skills in a sports context is challenging due to uncontrollable variables, such as environmental unpredictability, the strength and power of actions, opponent characteristics, and technological limitations. ...

Modified perceptual training in sport: A new classification framework

Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport

... Its effectiveness has been confirmed by research for several decades (for reviews, see Hüttermann, Noël, & Memmert,, 2018;Kredel, Vater, Klostermann, & Hossner, 2017). In basketball, for example, eye-tracking studies have been conducted to investigate several areas, such as the effects of shot deception on players' anticipation behavior (Meyer et al., 2022a), the coordination of basketball referees' gaze behavior Klatt et al., 2021), the use of defensive gaze strategies by defensive players (Meyer et al., 2022b), and the players' visual search characteristics during recall, decision-making, and anticipation tasks (Gorman, Abernethy, & Farrow, 2018Meyer et al., 2022a;Ryu, Abernethy, Mann, Poolton, & Gorman, 2013). ...

Reduced attentional focus and the influence on expert anticipatory perception
  • Citing Article
  • October 2017

Attention Perception & Psychophysics