Brendan Nyhan’s research while affiliated with Dartmouth College and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (79)


Survey flow of wave 1. The survey flow is described in the figure. Participants who did not consent or failed the attention check or did not meet the age requirement were screened out from the survey. Eligible participants continued to report socio-demographic characteristics. Then they were entered into the conjoint experiment, following which they were entered into two different between-subjects’ experiment. The first between-subjects assessment evaluated people’s support for a vaccine policy given varying levels of vaccine efficacy whereas the second experiment assessed people’s intentions for taking up booster vaccination and its support based on four randomised policy scenarios. Participants reported optimism about the future before ending the survey.
(left) Confidence in government and likelihood to avoid large indoor gatherings; (right) Confidence in government and likelihood to wear a face mask indoors. The left panel of figure shows a positive correlation between individuals’ confidence in government and likelihood to avoid large indoor gatherings whereas the right panel of figure shows a positive correlation between individuals’ confidence in government and likelihood to wear a face mask indoors.
Social media is primary news source (%) by country. Figure plots the proportion that social media is the primary source of news on COVID-19 across the G-7 countries. The proportion is the highest in the USA (slightly more than 25%) and lowest in Italy (slightly below 15%).
Social media as the primary news source predicts higher levels of concern about COVID-19 vaccine side effects. Figure shows a positive correlation between social media as the primary news source and higher levels of concern about COVID-19 vaccine side effects in the pooled sample of G-7 countries.
What nudges you to take a vaccine? Understanding behavioural drivers of COVID-19 vaccinations using large-scale experiments in the G-7 countries
  • Article
  • Full-text available

April 2025

·

12 Reads

·

·

·

[...]

·

Brendan Nyhan

Introduction We present a unique multi-country, two-wave dataset of 42,417 survey responses drawn from nationally representative samples of citizens from the G-7 countries: Canada, France, Germany, Italy, Japan, UK, and USA. This data note outlines the motivation and methodology of the survey instrument and describes the measures contained in the dataset. We highlight areas for future research. Methods We fielded an online survey over two waves (January 27 to February 26 [n = 24,303] and wave 2 from March 6 to May 12 [n = 18,114]) measuring a range of demographic, social, political, and psychological variables. Samples were nationally representative by age, education, gender, and subnational region. Each wave included of three experiments (one conjoint and two between-subjects) to facilitate randomised evaluation of behavioural health policies promoting the uptake of COVID-19 booster vaccinations. Results The dataset has produced two peer-reviewed publications at the time of writing ([Banerjee, S., John, P., Nyhan, B., Hunter, A., Koenig, R., Lee-Whiting, B., Loewen, P. J., McAndrews, J., & Savani, M. M. (2024). Thinking about default enrollment lowers vaccination intentions and public support in G7 countries. PNAS Nexus, 3(4), pgae093]; [Koenig, R., Savani, M. M., Lee-Whiting, B., McAndrews, J., Banerjee, S., Hunter, A., John, P., Loewen, P. J., & Nyhan, B. (2024). Public support for more stringent vaccine policies increases with vaccine effectiveness. Scientific Reports, 14(1), 1748]). A summary report is posted online (https://www.thebritishacademy.ac.uk/publications/overcoming-barriers-to-vaccination-by-empowering-citizens-to-make-deliberate-choices/). Additional research outputs are currently under preparation. Discussion Our dataset combines observational and experimental data on behavioural health policies, offering numerous insights. First, the dataset's extensive size and geographical diversity enables comparative analysis of public health issues involving social, political, and behavioural factors. Second, the dataset is suited to advanced statistical methods that can explore heterogeneity in the uptake of behavioural health policies, such as vaccine nudges. Third, the timing of the data collection, coinciding with the rise of the Omicron variant, provides valuable insights into why some previously vaccinated individuals might hesitate to receive additional doses, potentially improving our understanding of the COVID-19 pandemic and possible responses to pandemics and other public health emergencies in the future.

Download


How the relationship between education and antisemitism varies between countries

June 2024

·

23 Reads

·

1 Citation

Research & Politics

We investigate the relationship between education and antisemitism using unique individual-level survey data on antisemitism from more than 100 countries. Our findings show that education is associated with greater favorability toward Jews, but the relationship between education and endorsement of antisemitic stereotypes and conspiracy theories varies between countries. In countries that actively supported recent statements condemning Holocaust denial and antisemitism at the United Nations—which we use as a proxy for country-level opposition to antisemitism in education and politics—greater education is associated with reduced endorsement of antisemitic stereotypes. By contrast, more educated people are more likely to endorse antisemitic stereotypes than less educated people in countries that declined to endorse those statements. These descriptive findings provide new evidence about the association between education and intolerance.


Fig. 1. The CI bar plots of A) intentions to get the booster dose and B) approval of actions of the government.
Fig. 2. Coefficient plot of ITT effects for A) intentions to get the booster dose and B) approval of actions of the government.
ITT effects on vaccination intentions and policy approval.
Thinking about default enrollment lowers vaccination intentions and public support in G7 countries

February 2024

·

12 Reads

·

7 Citations

PNAS Nexus

Policymakers often face a conundrum between being transparent about policies and ensuring that those policies are effective. This challenge is particularly relevant for behavioral nudges, which are not usually disclosed. Rather than avoiding transparency, we suggest that policy-makers encourage citizens to reflect on nudges to help them understand their own views and align those views with their behaviors. Using data from an online survey experiment with 24,303 respondents in the G7, we examine the impact of reflection on a hypothetical default nudge policy for COVID-19 booster appointments. Contrary to expectations, participants say they would be less likely to get the booster when automatically enrolled compared to a control condition. Similarly, encouraging citizens to think about the status quo (baseline) policy also reduces intentions for boosters. These interventions had no effect on approval of the policy. Further, encouraging people to think about automatic enrollment decreased approval of the policy and further decreased their intentions to get vaccinated. These findings suggest that reflection on a nudge can increase backlash from a nudge and also elicit policy disapproval, thereby aligning policy support with behavioral intentions.


Mean booster policy stringency index by treatment condition with 95% CIs (Study 1). Support for more stringent policies is greater when vaccine effectiveness is greater, but support plateaus beyond 70% vaccine effectiveness.
Mean booster policy stringency index by treatment condition with 95% CIs (Study 2). Support for more stringent policies is greater when the booster is presented as “equally” or “more effective”.
Support for each booster policy by treatment condition (Study 1). Mean policy support by treatment with standard errors of means in parentheses. Means represent the proportion of people who said 'yes' to Policies 2, 3, and 4 (the stringent policies); and who said 'no' in response to Policy 1. Overall mean support for each individual policy is reported in the final row. Policy support is summed by experimental group to generate the booster policy support index.
Public support for more stringent vaccine policies increases with vaccine effectiveness

January 2024

·

43 Reads

·

2 Citations

Under what conditions do citizens support coercive public policies? Although recent research suggests that people prefer policies that preserve freedom of choice, such as behavioural nudges, many citizens accepted stringent policy interventions like fines and mandates to promote vaccination during the COVID-19 pandemic—a pattern that may be linked to the unusually high effectiveness of COVID-19 vaccines. We conducted a large online survey experiment (N = 42,417) in the Group of Seven (G-7) countries investigating the relationship between a policy’s effectiveness and public support for stringent policies. Our results indicate that public support for stringent vaccination policies increases as vaccine effectiveness increases, but at a modest scale. This relationship flattens at higher levels of vaccine effectiveness. These results suggest that intervention effectiveness can be a significant predictor of support for coercive policies but only up to some threshold of effectiveness.



The science of fake news

July 2023

·

1,582 Reads

Fake news emerged as an apparent global problem during the 2016 U.S. Presidential election. Addressing it requires a multidisciplinary effort to define the nature and extent of the problem, detect fake news in real time, and mitigate its potentially harmful effects. This will require a better understanding of how the Internet spreads content, how people process news, and how the two interact. We review the state of knowledge in these areas and discuss two broad potential mitigation strategies: better enabling individuals to identify fake news, and intervention within the platforms to reduce the attention given to fake news. The cooperation of Internet platforms (especially Facebook, Google, and Twitter) with researchers will be critical to understanding the scale of the issue and the effectiveness of possible interventions.


Figure 1. Survey flow. Blue rounded boxes indicate survey question batteries, and yellow rectangles indicate mock social media feeds.
The Corrections Dilemma: Media Retractions Increase Belief Accuracy But Decrease Trust

March 2023

·

34 Reads

·

2 Citations

Journal of Experimental Political Science

Why are prominent news media retractions so rare? Using data from a survey experiment in which respondents view simulated Twitter newsfeeds, we demonstrate the dilemma facing news organizations that have published false information. Encouragingly, media retractions are effective at informing the public – they increase the accuracy of news consumers’ beliefs about the retracted reporting more than information from third parties questioning the original reporting or even the combination of the two. However, trust in the news outlet declines after a retraction, though this effect is small both substantively and in standardized terms relative to the increase in belief accuracy. This reputational damage persists even if the outlet issues a retraction before a third party questions the story. In a social media environment that frequently subjects reporting to intense scrutiny, the journalistic mission of news organizations to inform the public will increasingly conflict with organizational incentives to avoid admitting error.


Figure 1: Mean estimates of outcome measures across experimental conditions
Citizen reflection reduces support for default vaccination appointments. Evidence from G7 countries

August 2022

·

58 Reads

·

2 Citations

Democratic governments vary in the extent to which they encourage citizens to reflect on the policies that they enact. This trade-off is particularly acute for behavioral nudges like setting default choices, which are often not made explicit to citizens. Using data from an online survey experiment administered to 24,303 respondents in the G7, this paper tests the effects of encouraging citizens to reflect on a hypothetical default nudge policy (automatic enrolment for COVID-19 booster appointments) on both vaccination intentions and policy support. Contrary to expectations, individuals report that they would be less likely to get the booster in the default nudge condition compared to a control condition in which citizens would initiate their own booster appointments. At the same time, individuals were more supportive of the default nudge policy than the control condition policy. However, encouraging people to reflect on the nudge eliminates the increase in policy approval it otherwise generates while strengthening the negative effect it has on vaccination intentions. These results suggest that reflection may increase backlash but can better align expressed policy approval with the behavioral effects the nudge generates.


A platform penalty for news? How social media context can alter information credibility online

August 2022

·

102 Reads

·

6 Citations

Journal of Information Technology & Politics

Growing concern about dubious information online threatens the credibility of legitimate news. We examine two possible mechanisms for this effect on social media. First, people might view all news on social media as less credible. Second, questionable information elsewhere in a news feed might discredit legitimate news coverage. Findings from a preregistered experiment confirm that people see information on Facebook as less credible than identical information on news websites, though the effect is small, suggesting that observational data overstates this platform penalty. Prior exposure to low (versus high) credibility information on Facebook also reduces engagement with a target article, but not its perceived credibility. However, exploratory analyses show that the effects of prior exposure to low credibility information vary depending on the plausibility of the target article, decreasing the credibility of a less plausible article (a spillover effect) but increasing the credibility of a more plausible one (a contrast effect).


Citations (54)


... At the institutional level, the study underscores the importance of building more resilient democracies by strengthening the institutions that citizens are less likely to prioritize under conditions of insecurity. Indeed, my findings echo the cautions expressed by Nyhan and Titiunik (2024), who argue that interventions targeting public opinion must be paired with institutional reforms that incentivize political actors to respect democratic norms and principles. This is particularly true, as this pre-registered experiment demonstrates, for institutions like checks and balances and political accountability mechanisms. ...

Reference:

Are people equally willing to trade different dimensions of democracy for material and physical security?
Public opinion alone won't save democracy
  • Citing Article
  • October 2024

Science

... Regarding contributions, our study adds to a nascent literature on education and conflict by showing that expanding access to schooling can ease intergroup tensions at the community level. Past research has shown that individual educational attainment can reduce hostility toward immigrants (Cavaille and Marshall, 2019), diminish anti-Semitic attitudes (Nyhan, Yamaya and Zeitzoff, 2024), and usher in more cosmopolitan worldviews (Solodoch, 2024). We provide evidence that broad-based, systemic education reforms can also improve conflict outcomes at a societal scale. ...

How the relationship between education and antisemitism varies between countries
  • Citing Article
  • June 2024

Research & Politics

... Communicators who expect a certain outcome but admit that things might end up worse should face a (slightly) more forgiving public if and when that happens. By contrast, communicators having to admit that they were wrong face a reputational hit (Freitag et al. 2023). ...

The Corrections Dilemma: Media Retractions Increase Belief Accuracy But Decrease Trust

Journal of Experimental Political Science

... Finally, the third experiment presented respondents with another future COVID-19 policy scenario and randomised people into one of four conditions in a between-subjects design (see Banerjee et al., 2024b). This experiment was designed to test the role of reflection in nudges in improving effectiveness and legitimacy of behavioural nudges, especially when combined, an approach that has been labelled as nudge+ as outlined earlier . ...

Thinking about default enrollment lowers vaccination intentions and public support in G7 countries

PNAS Nexus

... The second experiment evaluated the relationship between perceived vaccine effectiveness and support for more stringent vaccine promotion policies (see Koenig et al., 2024). In this between-subjects experiment, we randomly varied the effectiveness of a hypothetical COVID-19 booster vaccine against a new variant. ...

Public support for more stringent vaccine policies increases with vaccine effectiveness

... Our interpretation of these results is that the more we move away from highly urbanized and literate samples, the less appropriate online interventions themselves are for countering misinformation (see also Blair et al., 2023;Roozenbeek et al., 2024). In other words, beyond digital boundaries, there are other cultural and social boundaries that compromise the effectiveness of interventions when translating them into another context (Dahdouh-Guebas et al., 2003). ...

Interventions to Counter Misinformation: Lessons from the Global North and Applications to the Global South
  • Citing Article
  • November 2023

Current Opinion in Psychology

... Second, although the outcome variable remained a booster policy support index, participants were now presented with seven instead of four policies and the response options of "I would support this" and "I would oppose this" instead of "Yes", "No" and "Unsure". The aim was to capture greater variation in the stringency and costs imposed by policies, which were designed to mirror real world interventions 16 . Table 4 displays the survey instrument. ...

Overcoming Barriers to Vaccination by Empowering Citizens to Make Deliberate Choices Overcoming Barriers to Vaccination by Empowering Citizens to Make Deliberate Choices

... This suggests that the skepticism towards true news documented in this meta-analysis may be partially due to the social media format of the news headlines. Past work has shown that people report trusting news on social media less 3,19 , and experimental manipulations have shown that the Facebook news format reduces belief in news 50,51 , although the causal effects documented in these experiments are much smaller than the ones observed in surveys 52 . Low trust in news on social media may be a good thing, given that on average, news on social media may be less accurate than news on news websites, but it is also worrying since most of news consumption worldwide is shifting online and on social media in particular 43 . ...

A platform penalty for news? How social media context can alter information credibility online
  • Citing Article
  • August 2022

Journal of Information Technology & Politics

... We noted several novel studies. For example, an intervention suggested that fact-checks can successfully change beliefs, but only for a short period (Carey et al., 2021). The study with the largest sample size, 4069, assessed the role of infographics developed by several The mean effect size for mitigation of COVID-19 misinformation was positive but not statistically significant (d = 2.018, 95% CI (−0.14, 4.18), p = .065, ...

The ephemeral effects of fact-checks on COVID-19 misperceptions: Evidence from the United States, Great Britain, and Canada
  • Citing Preprint
  • August 2021

... Research from the social sciences indicates that interfaces that flag information critically impose backfire effects [3] and that interaction and durability effects should be spent more regard on. Nyhan [18] sees potential in corrective information targeting previously built perceptions and that connections between group-identities and false claims play strong roles. ...

Why the backfire effect does not explain the durability of political misperceptions
  • Citing Article
  • April 2021

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences