Brendan J. Barrett’s research while affiliated with University of Konstanz and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (39)


Proposed four step workflow for converting historical records of animal locations into usable data for movement analyses (figure by A. Ashbury).
Frame of interactive map used to annotate historical sleepsite data. Map generated using reproducible code and spatial objects in RMarkdown with the mapview package. Yellow grid includes a uniquely numbered one hectare quadrat. Points of interest (sleepsites, fruiting trees, buildings, landmarks) are colour coded, as are spatial features of interest (i.e. trails, roads, rivers, streams, fences). Spatial features can be toggled on and off for ease of viewing.
Location data and home range estimates from 12 comparisons between GPS‐tracking data versus georeferenced sleep‐site data based on semantic descriptions from 2009 to 2010. Panel titles indicate group identifier and year of data collection. Solid lines indicate mean utilisation distribution estimates. Dashed lines show 95% confidence intervals. Points represent location data. Orange indicates sleep‐site data and range estimates. Yellow indicates tracking data and range estimates. Scale is 1 km.
Validation plots comparing areas from sleep‐site derived 98% utilisation distributions (UDs) with GPS‐tracking‐derived 95% UDs. (a, b) Predictions from Gamma measurement error regressions. Dashed black 1‐to‐1 reference line signifies perfect agreement. Data points, with dashed confidence intervals, represent raw data. Shaded regions indicate the predicted 90%, 75%, and 50% credible intervals. (c, d) Bland‐Altmann plots compare the difference between measurements with the mean between measurements. Raw data are denoted by open circles, with horizontal dashed lines indicating the mean difference across all comparisons. Horizontal dotted lines represent two standard deviations from the mean difference. Colours distinguish validations of UDs from GPS‐based sleep‐site data (orange) and georeferenced description‐based sleep‐site data (blue).
Model predictions for changes in home range area (HRA) as a function of group size. Colours correspond to all 11 groups. Top left panel shows marginal posterior predictions. All other panels display per‐group predictions. Lighter lines are 100 randomly sampled posterior predictions, with the posterior median as a dark line. Vertical lines indicate the 89% HPDI of our estimated home range posterior distribution, while points lie at posterior median home range area.

+1

A new approach to geostatistical synthesis of historical records reveals capuchin spatial responses to climate and demographic change
  • Article
  • Full-text available

May 2024

·

86 Reads

Ecology Letters

·

Brendan J. Barrett

·

·

[...]

·

Margaret C. Crofoot

Recent proliferation of GPS technology has transformed animal movement research. Yet, time‐series data from this recent technology rarely span beyond a decade, constraining longitudinal research. Long‐term field sites hold valuable historic animal location records, including hand‐drawn maps and semantic descriptions. Here, we introduce a generalised workflow for converting such records into reliable location data to estimate home ranges, using 30 years of sleep‐site data from 11 white‐faced capuchin (Cebus imitator) groups in Costa Rica. Our findings illustrate that historic sleep locations can reliably recover home range size and geometry. We showcase the opportunity our approach presents to resolve open questions that can only be addressed with very long‐term data, examining how home ranges are affected by climate cycles and demographic change. We urge researchers to translate historical records into usable movement data before this knowledge is lost; it is essential to understanding how animals are responding to our changing world.

Download

Male-biased stone tool use by wild white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus imitator)

January 2024

·

32 Reads

·

1 Citation

American Journal of Primatology

Tool‐using primates often show sex differences in both the frequency and efficiency of tool use. In species with sex‐biased dispersal, such within‐group variation likely shapes patterns of cultural transmission of tool‐use traditions between groups. On the Panamanian islands of Jicarón and Coiba, a population of white‐faced capuchins ( Cebus capucinus imitator )—some of which engage in habitual stone tool use—provide an opportunity to test hypotheses about why such sex‐biases arise. On Jicarón, we have only observed males engaging in stone tool use, whereas on Coiba, both sexes are known to use tools. Using 5 years of camera trap data, we provide evidence that this variation likely reflects a sex difference in tool use rather than a sampling artifact, and then test hypotheses about the factors driving this pattern. Differences in physical ability or risk‐aversion, and competition over access to anvils do not account for the sex‐differences in tool‐use we observe. Our data show that adult females are physically capable of stone tool use: adult females on Coiba and juveniles on Jicarón smaller than adult females regularly engage in tool use. Females also have ample opportunity to use tools: the sexes are equally terrestrial, and competition over anvils is low. Finally, females rarely scrounge on left‐over food items either during or after tool‐using events, suggesting they are not being provisioned by males. Although it remains unclear why adult white‐faced capuchin females on Jicarón do not use stone‐tools, our results illustrate that such sex biases in socially learned behaviors can arise even in the absence of obvious physical, environmental, and social constraints. This suggests that a much more nuanced understanding of the differences in social structure, diet, and dispersal patterns are needed to explain why sex‐biases in tool use arise in some populations but not in others.


The Importance of Representative Sampling for Home Range Estimation in Field Primatology

October 2023

·

125 Reads

·

4 Citations

International Journal of Primatology

Understanding the amount of space required by animals to fulfill their biological needs is essential for comprehending their behavior, their ecological role within their community, and for effective conservation planning and resource management. The space-use patterns of habituated primates often are studied by using handheld GPS devices, which provide detailed movement information that can link patterns of ranging and space-use to the behavioral decisions that generate these patterns. However, these data may not accurately represent an animal’s total movements, posing challenges when the desired inference is at the home range scale. To address this problem, we used a 13-year dataset from 11 groups of white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus imitator) to examine the impact of sampling elements, such as sample size, regularity, and temporal coverage, on home range estimation accuracy. We found that accurate home range estimation is feasible with relatively small absolute sample sizes and irregular sampling, as long as the data are collected over extended time periods. Also, concentrated sampling can lead to bias and overconfidence due to uncaptured variations in space use and underlying movement behaviors. Sampling protocols relying on handheld GPS for home range estimation are improved by maximizing independent location data distributed across time periods much longer than the target species’ home range crossing timescale.


Coupling of coastal activity with tidal cycles is stronger in tool-using capuchins (Cebus capucinus imitator)

September 2023

·

77 Reads

·

5 Citations

Terrestrial mammals exploiting coastal resources must cope with the challenge that resource availability and accessibility fluctuate with tidal cycles. Tool use can improve foraging efficiency and provide access to structurally protected resources that are otherwise unavailable (e.g. molluscs and fruits). To understand how variable accessibility of valuable resources shapes behavioural patterns, and whether tool use aids in the efficient exploitation of intertidal resources, we compared the relationship between tidal cycles and activity patterns of tool-using versus non-tool-using groups of white-faced capuchin monkeys on Jicarón Island in Coiba National Park, Panama. Although tool use on Jicarón is localized to a small stretch of coast (approx. 1 km), all coastal groups forage on intertidal resources. Using more than 5 years of camera trap data at varying distances from the coast, we found that capuchins on Jicarón showed increased coastal activity during specific parts of the tidal cycle, and that this relationship differed between tool-using and non-tool-using groups, as well as between seasons. Activity patterns of tool-using capuchins were more strongly and consistently tied to tidal cycles compared with non-tool-users, indicating that tool use might allow for more efficient exploitation of tidal resources. Our findings highlight the potential of tool use to aid niche expansion.


Figure 2. Marginal effects of the type of camera location (random, anvil, or streambed) and
Figure 4. Marginal effects of the type of camera location (random, anvil, or
Male-biased stone tool use by wild white-faced capuchins ( Cebus capucinus imitator )

September 2023

·

50 Reads

Tool-using primates often show sex differences in both the frequency and efficiency of tool use. In species with sex-biased dispersal, such within-group variation likely shapes patterns of cultural transmission of tool-use traditions between groups. On the Panamanian islands of Jicarón and Coiba, a population of white-faced capuchins ( Cebus capucinus imitator ) — some of which engage in habitual stone tool use — provide an opportunity to test hypotheses about why such sex-biases arise. On Jicarón, we have only observed males engaging in stone tool use, whereas on Coiba, both sexes are known to use tools. Using 5 years of camera trap data, we show that this variation reflects a true sex difference in tool use rather than a sampling artefact, and then test hypotheses about the factors driving this pattern. Differences in physical ability or risk-aversion, and competition over access to anvils do not account for the sex-differences in tool-use we observe. Our data show that females are physically capable of stone tool use: females on Coiba and juveniles on Jicarón as small and smaller than adult females regularly engage in tool use. Females also have ample opportunity to use tools: the sexes are equally terrestrial, and competition over anvils is low. Finally, females rarely scrounge on left-over food items either during or after tool-using events, suggesting they are not being provisioned by males. Although it remains unclear why white-faced capuchin females on Jicarón do not use stone-tools, our results illustrate that such sex biases in socially learned behaviors can arise even in the absence of obvious physical, environmental and social constraints. This suggests that a much more nuanced understanding of the differences in social structure, diet and dispersal patterns are needed to explain why sex-biases in tool use arise in some populations but not in others.



Coupling of Coastal Activity with Tidal Cycles is Stronger in Tool-using Capuchins (Cebus capucinus imitator)

December 2022

·

66 Reads

·

1 Citation

Terrestrial mammals exploiting coastal resources must cope with the challenge that resource availability and accessibility fluctuate with tidal cycles. Tool use can improve foraging efficiency and provide access to structurally protected resources that are otherwise unavailable (e.g., mollusks and fruits). To understand how variable accessibility of valuable resources shapes behavioral patterns, and whether tool use aids in the efficient exploitation of intertidal resources, we compared the relationship between tidal cycles and activity patterns of tool-using vs. non-tool-using groups of white-faced capuchin monkeys on Jicarón Island in Coiba National Park, Panama. Although only a single group of capuchins on Jicarón uses tools, all coastal groups forage on intertidal resources. Using data from >3 years of camera trapping at varying distances from the coast, we found that capuchins on Jicarón showed increased coastal activity during specific parts of the tidal cycle, and that this relationship differed between tool-using and non-tool-using groups, as well as between seasons. Activity patterns of tool-using capuchins were more strongly and consistently tied to tidal cycles compared to non-tool-users, indicating that tool use might allow for more efficient exploitation of tidal resources. Our findings highlight the potential of tool use to aid niche expansion.


Acquisition curves for positive frequency‐dependent social learning when the number of behavioural options are k = 2, 3, 4, 5. Y‐axis shows the frequency of p1 after social learning, x‐axis shows the frequency of p1 before social learning. Dashed diagonal line is unbiased choice. Acquisition curves are for the special entropy‐maximizing case when the frequencies of all behaviours, except for the focal behaviour p1, are equal. Here, logf=log3≈1.09 (see Equation 1).
Acquisition curves for six simplexes of positive frequency dependence where k = 3. Y‐axis shows the frequency of p1 after social learning, x‐axis shows the frequency of p1 before social learning. Each line is a unique acquisition curve for the simplex K = {p1, p2, p3} where p2 = x * p3. Here, logf=log3≈1.09 (see Equation 1).
of posterior predictions of log(f) for 100 simulations of positive frequency‐dependent social learning where f = 3. Sample size increases down rows n = 5, 10, 25, 50, 100, 250, number of options increases across columns k = 2, 3, 4, 5. Coloured points lie at posterior median of each simulation and 30π % highest posterior density intervals (HPDIs) are visualized by horizontal black bars. Vertical dashed line at zero indicates unbiased social learning, with negative frequency‐dependent learning to the left and positive frequency‐dependent learning to the right. Solid coloured vertical line is at known simulated value of log(f) = log(3).
Inferential power in identifying frequency‐dependent social learning strengthened by increasing behavioural options

October 2022

·

24 Reads

·

4 Citations

Two‐option choice experimental designs are the most commonly employed framework for identifying evidence of social learning or social learning strategies in captive and wild populations. In nature, however, animals often choose from more than two behaviours, and multiple innovations may arise simultaneously. Studies of animal social learning are often constrained by small sample sizes, which limit researchers' ability to convincingly identify the proposed social learning strategy responsible for behavioural choice. In this study, I examine whether expanding behavioural options from k = 2 to k > 2 and increasing sample size affects inferential power in identifying social learning strategies. I focus on three frequency‐dependent learning strategies: conformist transmission, unbiased transmission and anti‐conformist transmission. I simulate 100 datasets for 72 parameter combinations, yielding 7200 simulations. I evaluate number of options (k = 2, 3, 4, 5), population size (n = 5, 10, 25, 50, 100, 250) and the logarithmic strength of frequency dependence (log(f) = log(1∕3), log(1), log(3)). I then fit a Bayesian social learning model to simulated data to evaluate the percent of the posterior consistent with type of frequency dependence, posterior standard deviations, highest posterior density intervals and posterior medians relative to the true simulated value of log(f). I show that increasing the number of options an animal can choose from increases the accuracy and certainty of identifying the type and magnitude of frequency‐dependent social learning. These effects are particularly pronounced at small to intermediate sample sizes, which are common in empirical studies of animal social learning. These findings suggest that knowing what an animal did not choose is equally important as knowing what an animal did choose when identifying social learning strategies. By strategically increasing the number of behaviours from which an animal can choose, researchers can increase inferential power in identifying social learning strategies without increasing sample size, that is, adding additional animals or collecting more data.


Figure 3. Strong social information bias slowed diffusion. Diffusion curves for the novel behaviour with distributions of time-to-diffusion at the top, pooled within social information bias (colour, mean with 95% bootstrapped CI and traces from individual simulations). Data shown for reference-level (300 simulations), and x-axis is square-root transformed for ease of reading. Strong social information bias within the sub-model of production greatly slowed diffusion. (Online version in colour.)
Cultural diffusion dynamics depend on behavioural production rules

August 2022

·

124 Reads

·

16 Citations

Culture is an outcome of both the acquisition of knowledge about behaviour through social transmission, and its subsequent production by individuals. Acquisition and production are often discussed or modelled interchangeably, yet to date no study has explored the consequences of their interaction for cultural diffusions. We present a generative model that integrates the two, and ask how variation in production rules might influence diffusion dynamics. Agents make behavioural choices that change as they learn from their productions. Their repertoires may also change, and the acquisition of behaviour is conditioned on its frequency. We analyse the diffusion of a novel behaviour through social networks, yielding generalizable predictions of how individual-level behavioural production rules influence population-level diffusion dynamics. We then investigate how linking acquisition and production might affect the performance of two commonly used inferential models for social learning; network-based diffusion analysis, and experience-weighted attraction models. We find that the influence that production rules have on diffusion dynamics has consequences for how inferential methods are applied to empirical data. Our model illuminates the differences between social learning and social influence, demonstrates the overlooked role of reinforcement learning in cultural diffusions, and allows for clearer discussions about social learning strategies.


Cultural diffusion dynamics depend on behavioural production rules

December 2021

·

138 Reads

·

1 Citation

Culture is an outcome of the acquisition of knowledge about behaviour through social transmission, and its subsequent production. Transmission and production are often discussed interchangeably or modeled separately, yet to date, no study has accounted for both processes and explored their interaction. We present a generative model that integrates the two in order to explore how variation in either might shape cultural diffusion dynamics. Agents make behavioural choices that change as they learn from their behavioural productions. Their repertoires also change over time, and the social transmission of behaviours depends on their frequency. We diffuse a novel behaviour through social networks across a large parameter space to demonstrate how accounting for both transmission and production reveals dependencies between individual-level behavioural production rules and population-level diffusion dynamics. We then investigate how such dependencies might affect the performance of two commonly used inferential models for social learning; Network-based Diffusion Analysis (NBDA), and Experienced Weighted Attraction models (EWA). By clarifying the distinction between acquisition and usage, we illuminate often-overlooked theoretical differences between social learning and social influence. These distinctions yield consequences and new considerations for how inferential methods are applied to empirical studies of culture.


Citations (20)


... A correct home range estimate is essential to define habitat use, resource selection, and movement pattern analyses [15,16]. Additionally, estimating home range enables stakeholders and policymakers to formally preserve the critical habitats frequently utilised by species of conservation interest [17,18]. Factors like prey distribution [19], habitat quality [20], competition [21], and physiological needs [22] usually influence the size, shape, and spatial pattern of a home range. ...

Reference:

Spatial Ecology of a Resident Avian Predator During the Non-Breeding Period in Managed Habitats of Southeastern Europe
The Importance of Representative Sampling for Home Range Estimation in Field Primatology

International Journal of Primatology

... Previous implementations of the method of finite differences on GAMMs were done in a frequentist context [40,41]. We built on this approach by developing a fully Bayesian extension of this method (as in [42] code [43]). To accomplish this, we first used the 'posterior_smooths' function in 'brms' to obtain posterior predictions. ...

Coupling of coastal activity with tidal cycles is stronger in tool-using capuchins (Cebus capucinus imitator)

... Tanzania's growing human population has undoubtedly led to a rise in socio-economic activities like farming and livestock keeping in areas near protected areas (Hariohay et al. 2017, Kegamba et al. 2024. As a result, local communities have been exposed to various wildlife-associated conflicts (Denninger Snyder et al. 2023). Pastoralists and their livestock have been found sharing ecosystems with wildlife, and particularly with dangerous predators (e.g. ...

Differentiated drivers in wildlife-induced damage necessitate species-specific mitigation strategies in the western Serengeti, Tanzania
  • Citing Article
  • September 2023

Biological Conservation

... Heeding previous calls (Dukas, 1998;McNamara and Houston, 2009;Fawcett et al., 2013), our study provides an analytical solution to facilitate productive research on proximate and ultimate explanations of seemingly flexible (or not) behaviour: because we publicly provide step-bystep code to examine individual decision-making, two core underlying learning mechanisms, and their theoretical selection and benefit (see GitHub, copy archived at Breen, 2024), which can be tailored to specific research questions. The reinforcement learning model, for example, generalises to, in theory, a variety of choice-option paradigms (Barrett, 2023), and these learning models can be extended to estimate asocial and social influence on individual decision-making (e.g., McElreath et al., 2005;Aplin et al., 2017;Barrett et al., 2017;Deffner et al., 2020;Chimento et al., 2022), facilitating insight into the multi-faceted feedback process between individual cognition and social systems (Tump et al., 2024). Our open-access analytical resource thus allows researchers to dispense with the umbrella term behavioural flexibility, and to biologically inform and interpret their scienceonly then can we begin to meaningfully examine the functional basis of behavioural variation across taxa and/or contexts. ...

Inferential power in identifying frequency‐dependent social learning strengthened by increasing behavioural options

... Using the spatial diffusion model to visualize and quantify the dissemination and evolution of Chinese ethnic traditional sports culture helps to further inherit and develop the excellent traditional Chinese culture [3][4]. Cultural diffusion refers to the process in which a particular cultural thing or phenomenon is transmitted from one region to another region through certain forms in a certain period, which is mainly realized through the communication and migration of people as cultural carriers [5][6][7][8]. ...

Cultural diffusion dynamics depend on behavioural production rules

... Further investigations of observation biases for apparent learning purposes (in naïve individuals) are required to elucidate the factors involved in diverse species and contexts to build a better picture of the variety of ways social learning strategies may be combined ( 7 ). Such is not confined to model-based biases as indicated by wild vervet monkeys exhibiting a content/direct pay-off bias alongside a bias to copy higher rank individuals ( 63 ). Future studies will benefit from ensuring diverse trait variants (e.g., task options) are used by individuals within a group, even while perhaps manipulating their relative payoff, to enable investigation of the extent to which observation biases translate into social learning. ...

Processing of novel food reveals payoff and rank-biased social learning in a wild primate

... In many species, animals are more likely to rely on social information when they are 'uncertain' and do not have a solution for a novel problem they face (e.g., Bombus terrestris [40]; Lasius niger [41]; Rattus norvegicus [42]; Pan troglodytes [43]) and when they are young and/or of low rank (e.g., Cyanistes caeruleus [44]). Moreover, animals appear more likely to copy older and/or dominant individuals (e.g., Corvus monedula [45]; Chlorocebus pygerythrus [46]; Sapajus spp. [47]; Pan troglodytes [43]), their kin (e.g., Chlorocebus pygerythrus [48]), familiar individuals (e.g., Sus spp. ...

Processing of novel food reveal payoff and rank-biased social learning in a wild primate

... Next to agricultural fields, hedgerows made of a mixture of native species, inlcuding toyon, supported significantly more bird species, evenness and abundance than the weedy margin alternative (Heath et al. 2017). Duskyfooted woodrats (Neotoma fuscipes ) also feed on plants (Barrett et al 2016). ...

Life on the edge: diet preferences reflect adaptation to drought in Neotoma fuscipes

... As our observations include some mothers and sires who had multiple offspring within the study period, and both individuals within each pair (female and male) are represented across multiple observations, we included varying effects for each offspring (to account for mothers/sires with multiple offspring) and each individual within a pair (to account for non-independence of the pairwise relatedness outcome variable). Individual varying effects were included using a joint indexing notation as described by [58], which allowed us to accurately pool individual-level information across dyads. To improve model efficiency, we used a non-centred parametrization by applying a Cholesky decomposition to the varying effects priors [59]. ...

Female–male relationships influence the form of female–female relationships in olive baboons, Papio anubis
  • Citing Article
  • September 2017

Animal Behaviour

... Capuchin monkeys are the only platyrrhine known to use tools for nut-cracking, mainly using stone tools. Although a few island-inhabitant white-faced capuchin (Cebus imitator) groups use stone tools to process nuts, crabs and snails [12,13], most observations of this behaviour are from wild populations of robust capuchin species (Sapajus spp.), mainly bearded capuchins, S. libidinosus [14][15][16][17][18][19][20]. ...

White-Faced Capuchin, Cebus capucinus imitator, Hammerstone and Anvil Tool Use in Riparian Habitats on Coiba Island, Panama

International Journal of Primatology