Bradley Karstadt’s research while affiliated with Simon Fraser University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (2)


Visualizations of experimental stimuli
(A) Examples of three stimuli. From top to bottom: Lion with direct gaze, lion with averted gaze, and impala with averted gaze. (B) Regions of interest used in the analyses (eyes, head, body, background). (C) Salience maps [62]. Heatmaps (aggregated across all subjects) for gaze patterns of (D) humans and (E) chimpanzees. An image of an impala with averted gaze is not pictured due to photograph copyright issues. Lion with direct gaze printed under a CC BY license with permission from copyright holder Kanwar Deep Juneja. Lion with averted gaze printed under a CC BY license with permission from copyright holder Cass Womack. Impala with averted gaze printed under a CC BY license with permission from copyright holder Tris Enticknap.
Normalized fixation proportions plotted as a function of stimulus species, gaze direction and ROI for Experiment 1
Horizontal lines indicate statistically significant planned contrasts.
Proportion of initial fixations falling on eyes, heads, bodies, or background, as a function of stimulus species and gaze direction
Horizontal lines indicate statistically significant planned contrasts.
Normalized fixation proportions plotted as a function of stimulus species, gaze direction and ROI for Experiment 2
Horizontal lines indicate statistically significant planned contrasts.
Proportion of initial fixations falling on eyes, heads, bodies, or background, as a function of stimulus species and gaze direction for Experiment 2
Horizontal lines indicate statistically significant planned contrasts.

+4

Predator gaze captures both human and chimpanzee attention
  • Article
  • Full-text available

November 2024

·

100 Reads

·

1 Citation

·

Bradley Karstadt

·

·

[...]

·

Primates can rapidly detect potential predators and modify their behavior based on the level of risk. The gaze direction of predators is one feature that primates can use to assess risk levels: recognition of a predator’s direct stare indicates to prey that it has been detected and the level of risk is relatively high. Predation has likely shaped visual attention in primates to quickly assess the level of risk but we know little about the constellation of low-level (e.g., contrast, color) and higher-order (e.g., category membership, perceived threat) visual features that primates use to do so. We therefore presented human and chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) participants with photographs of potential predators (lions) and prey (impala) while we recorded their overt attention with an eye-tracker. The gaze of the predators and prey was either directed or averted. We found that both humans and chimpanzees visually fixated the eyes of predators more than those of prey. In addition, they directed the most attention toward the eyes of directed (rather than averted) predators. Humans, but not chimpanzees, gazed at the eyes of the predators and prey more than other features. Importantly, low-level visual features of the predators and prey did not provide a good explanation of the observed gaze patterns.

Download

Participants’ view of targets in the video clips
Note. Example of the view of targets presented to participants (only the schematic is presented here to protect the privacy of the targets). After viewing the clips, and before completing the Dominance and Prestige Peer Rating Scales [88] for each target, participants were given a screenshot from the videos that identified the targets as A, B, or C. Adapted from “Gaze Allocation in a Dynamic Situation: Effects of Social Status and Speaking,” by Foulsham, et al., 2010. Cognition, 117(3), p. 319–331.
Proportions of fixations to body, head, and eyes
Note. Proportions of fixations to the areas of interest (AOIs) for Status (green triangles), Prestige (orange circles), and Dominance (purple squares) by AQ-10 group.
LAQ and HAQ participants’ status-relevant ratings of targets
Fixations to the eyes of low, medium, and high ranked targets
Reading the room: Autistic traits, gaze behaviour, and the ability to infer social relationships

March 2023

·

191 Reads

·

2 Citations

Individuals high in autistic traits can have difficulty understanding verbal and non-verbal cues, and may display atypical gaze behaviour during social interactions. The aim of this study was to examine differences among neurotypical individuals with high and low levels of autistic traits with regard to their gaze behaviour and their ability to assess peers’ social status accurately. Fifty-four university students who completed the 10-item Autism Quotient (AQ-10) were eye-tracked as they watched six 20-second video clips of people (“targets”) involved in a group decision-making task. Simulating natural, everyday social interactions, the video clips included moments of debate, humour, interruptions, and cross talk. Results showed that high-scorers on the AQ-10 (i.e., those with more autistic traits) did not differ from the low-scorers in either gaze behaviour or assessing the targets’ relative social status. The results based on this neurotypical group of participants suggest that the ability of individuals high in autistic traits to read social cues may be preserved in certain tasks crucial to navigating day-to-day social relationships. These findings are discussed in terms of their implications for theory of mind, weak central coherence, and social motivation theories of autism.

Citations (1)


... An additional concern with using the AQ-10 is that it showed low internal consistency as estimated with Cronbach's alpha, both in our sample and in previous adult samples from the general population (Jia et al., 2019;Sizoo et al., 2015;Taylor et al., 2020). To be sure, the AQ-10 has been commonly used in research since it was developed, specifically as a more efficient and less time-consuming measure of autistic traits in the general population (e.g., Bertrams & Schlegel, 2020;Forby et al., 2023;Gollwitzer et al., 2019;Mason et al., 2021;Pazhoohi et al., 2021;Rudolph et al., 2018). We thus used the AQ-10 in this study because it had been used by other researchers and allowed us to substantially reduce the time it would take for participants to complete the study, an important consideration due to the limited amount of funding we had to compensate them. ...

Reference:

Autistic traits, alexithymia, and emotion recognition of human and anime faces
Reading the room: Autistic traits, gaze behaviour, and the ability to infer social relationships