Bianca Grohmann’s research while affiliated with Concordia University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (57)


The effect of retail shelf position on private-label brand evaluations
  • Article

May 2024

·

43 Reads

·

3 Citations

International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management

Maryam Tofighi

·

Bianca Grohmann

Purpose This research examines the effects of physical proximity (close vs distant) of retailers’ private label brands (PLBs) relative to national brands (NBs) and brand display orientation (horizontal [brands occupy the same shelf] vs vertical [brands occupy different shelves]) on consumers’ PLB quality perceptions and PLB evaluations. Design/methodology/approach Two experiments involving real brands in different product categories tested the hypotheses. Findings A PLB positioned close (vs distant) to a NB is evaluated more favorably and this effect is mediated by increased PLB quality perceptions, but only in a horizontal brand display. In a vertical brand display, a PLB positioned close (vs distant) to a NB is evaluated less favorably and this effect is mediated by decreased PLB quality perceptions. Research limitations/implications The findings suggest that to enhance consumers’ PLB quality perceptions and evaluations, PLBs be positioned next to (rather than on separate shelves) and close to (rather than distant from) NBs in the same product category. Originality/value Although the literature suggests that the best shelf position for PLBs is close to NBs, there is a lack of empirical research on the effects of relative shelf positioning on consumers’ quality perceptions and subsequent PLB evaluations. This research finds that both physical proximity and brand display orientation play an important role.



Figure 1 LVMH's Hero Plus helper journey.
Confronting climate crisis through corporate narratives: the fairy tale in LVMH’s 2020 and 2021 social and environmental responsibility reports
  • Article
  • Full-text available

December 2023

·

103 Reads

·

1 Citation

Luxury

Situated in the context of the climate crisis this research examines the LVMH Corporation’s 2020 and 2021 Social and Environmental Responsibility Reports through the lens of the narrative structure found in fairy tales. Central characters and the trajectory of the narrative are consistent with this literary form, and counter narratives that provide context to the corporate narrative also emerge. This analysis suggests the significance of narratives to corporate communications, their application extending beyond the brand communication or user-generated content generally investigated in the marketing and consumer research literature. Moreover, the article elaborates on how luxury corporations, like LVMH, communicate their efforts to counter climate crisis in ways that meet the expectations of stakeholders, especially investors and consumers. The findings have implications for future research on how narratives impact stakeholder perceptions of luxury corporations. Available open access at: https://doi.org/10.1080/20511817.2023.2280321

Download


Figure 2. Mean accuracy performance by group across stimulus types and working memory loads. A) Mean accuracy in percent (%) for individuals with dyslexia (blue) and without dyslexia (yellow) for all three stimulus types (numbers, logos, and symbols), collapsed across working memory load conditions (3-and 6-load). B) Mean accuracy (%) for individuals with (blue) and without dyslexia (yellow) for all three stimulus types (numbers, logos, and symbols), separated by working memory load condition (3-and 6-load). C) Individual participant mean accuracy (%) separated by working memory condition for individuals with (blue) and without dyslexia (yellow) for the numbers stimulus type. D) Individual participant mean accuracy (%) as in panel C for the logos stimulus type. E) Individual participant mean accuracy (%) as in panel C for the symbols stimulus type.
Figure 4. Correlations of speed-accuracy trade-offs and performance with dyslexia symptomatology. All correlation values result from robust bend correlations (Pernet et al., 2013), while plots depict least-square fits. A) Correlations between median reaction time in milliseconds (ms) and mean accuracy (%) collapsed across both working memory load conditions (3-and 6-load) for individuals with (r = -0.45, p = 0.0009; blue) and without dyslexia (r = -0.40, p < 0.001; yellow). B) Correlations between median reaction time (ms) and mean accuracy (%) across the 3-load working memory condition for individuals with (r = -0.49, p = 0.0003; blue) and without dyslexia (r = -0.50, p < 0.001; yellow). C) Correlations between median reaction time (ms) and mean accuracy (%) across the 6-load working memory condition for individuals with (r = -0.40, p = 0.0033; blue) and without dyslexia (r = -0.45, p < 0.001; yellow). D) Correlations between all participants' Adult Dyslexia Checklist scores and mean accuracy (%) on the left (r = -0.18, p = 0.019) as well as reaction time (ms) on the right (r = .10, p = 0.202), across 3-and 6-load working memory conditions for logos and symbols only. E) Correlations between Adult Dyslexia Checklist scores and mean accuracy (%) for individuals with (r = -0.20, p = 0.158; blue) and without dyslexia (r = -0.03, p = 0.739; yellow), across 3-and 6-load working memory conditions for logos and symbols only. F)
Non-Linguistic Working Memory Challenges in Dyslexia in The Context of Applied Consumer Psychology

September 2023

·

119 Reads

Nathan Gagné

·

Bianca Grohmann

·

·

[...]

·

In our daily lives, we frequently encounter non-linguistic visual content that we must remember in a spatial context. Individuals with dyslexia are known to experience difficulty with linguistic content and show impairments in cognitive functioning. Recently researchers have highlighted that vision and executive functioning may play a role in explaining dyslexia symptoms, but the specific role of visuo-spatial working memory skills in dyslexia is poorly understood. In the present study, we used non-linguistic everyday brand logos from consumer psychology to investigate visuo-spatial working memory performance in relation to dyslexia. We used an adapted online version of the Sternberg task, where 51 participants with dyslexia and 129 participants without a dyslexia diagnosis recalled content from the same stimulus category presented in either three (low-working memory load condition) or six (high-working memory load condition) specific locations – analogous to recalling a particular branded item on a grocery shelf. Every trial included one of three types of non-linguistic content novel to participants, such as fictious brand logos without their brand name, symbols, or numbers. Bayesian modelling provided evidence for lower and slower working memory performance across all stimulus types for the dyslexia group compared to the non-dyslexia group. However, individuals in both groups showed similar decreases in performance when the working memory load was increased. These findings speak to the role of dyslexia as a multifaceted learning disorder that stretches beyond its linguistic definition and may exert wider reaching effects that also touch upon aspects of daily living that do not involve language.


Presents the conceptual framework tested in this research
Consumers' moral licensing of firms' CSR transgressions

December 2022

·

69 Reads

·

3 Citations

Despite corporate social responsibility (CSR) engagement, firms may be implicated in CSR transgressions. Research is equivocal on whether CSR buffers negative consumer responses to subsequent firm transgressions. This research extends the observer moral licensing framework to consumer‐firm contexts and examines under what conditions consumers license transgressions following firms' CSR engagement. Study 1 demonstrates that consumer responses to firm transgressions depend on whether the transgression occurs in the same (vs. different) domain relative to CSR engagement and on transgression ambiguity. Study 2 shows that consumer responses to transgressions are less negative when a firm has (vs. has not) previously engaged in CSR. It replicates a shielding effect of CSR that is contingent on transgression domain relative to prior CSR and transgression ambiguity, and finds that blatant same domain transgressions generate hypocrisy perceptions and mitigate licensing effects. Study 3 further extends the moral licensing framework and shows that firm communication that situates CSR efforts on a continuum (continuous CSR positioning) before a transgression moderates, and insincere firm motive attributions mediate, the detrimental effect of blatant same domain transgressions.



Validation survey results, experimental paradigm and example of stimuli
a) Median ratings for each of the three other validation scales (experience with the brand, knowledge of the brand’s products, product use) correlated with the band familiarity ratings. Correlation coefficients and p values obtained from robust bend correlations; 5% downweighted [37]. Similar high correlation values show that image ratings were similar across scales. b) Example of a pixel-scrambled mask of a mostly white and brownish familiar branded product image (e.g., bag of brand coffee beans) included in the pupillometry experiment. c) Example of a pixel-scrambled mask of a colorful branded product image (e.g., container of chocolate powder). d) Sequence of events of one trial of the pupillometry experiment. Each trial began with a black fixation cross on a grey background. The intact image (i.e., familiar or unfamiliar branded image) was then framed by a pixel-scrambled version of the intact image. This product image is represented only by its white background but an intact product was presented on this background. Each trial lasted about five seconds.
Pupil size change relative to baseline and individual dynamic range
a) Comparison of pupil responses to both familiar (blue) and unfamiliar (orange) intact branded images over time. Colorful lines show the 5% trimmed mean and shaded areas the bootstrapped standard errors of the mean. As a result of the temporal cluster-based bootstrapping procedure, the solid black line depicts the bootstrapped difference between the two familiarity conditions and black dots denote clusters of significant windows. The statistical test was conducted for every window with 30 ms onset intervals (i.e., 30 Hz). A data-driven minimum cluster size of 30 consecutive significant windows were required for a significant cluster. Dashed grey line depicts the median baseline pupil size of the last 150 ms leading up to the viewing of the intact image. The light blue and orange patches represent the bootstrapped standard errors for their respective familiarity condition. b) Fraction of participants showing an effect in the direction of the entire group; computed for each window. Dashed grey line depicts 50% of participant showing the same effect as the group. c) Individual participant-based comparison of pupil responses to both familiar (blue) and unfamiliar (orange) intact branded images over time. Vertical dashed lines indicate peak pupil responses within the first two seconds of viewing intact branded images by familiarity condition. If only one line is present both peaks were of identical latency. Color scheme as in panel a. All 15 participants shown. d) Individual difference between familiarity conditions (familiar–unfamiliar) relative to baseline and individual dynamic range in percent. Dashed grey lines depict the individual mean baseline across all trials of a participant. Note, black lines illustrate the effective difference and not the bootstrapped difference that is presented in panel a.
Product category-based pupil size change relative to baseline and individual dynamic range
a) Comparison of pupil responses to both familiar (blue) and unfamiliar (orange) intact branded images over time. Colorful lines show the group mean and shaded areas the bootstrapped standard errors of the mean for each condition. As a result of the temporal cluster-based bootstrapping procedure, the solid black line depicts the bootstrapped difference between the two familiarity conditions and black dots denote clusters of significant windows (for details, see Methods). Dashed grey line depicts the median baseline pupil size of the last 150 ms leading up to the viewing of the intact image. The light blue and orange patches represent the bootstrapped standard errors for their respective familiarity condition. b) Mean group difference between familiar and unfamiliar (familiar–unfamiliar) intact branded images in percent of the individual dynamic range for each of the four product categories: beverages (green), personal care (purple), cleaning (red), food (yellow). Shaded areas represent bootstrapped standard errors of the mean. Note, difference lines illustrate the effective difference and not the bootstrapped difference.
Correlations between pupil data and attention questions
a) Raincloud plot comparing bootstrapped means of subgroups split by self-reported usual product choice preference. Means generated from 1000 bootstrap iterations. b) Robust shift function comparing deciles of the two distributions shown in panel a [35]. The y-axis depicts by how much the “by brand” distribution (blue) has to be shifted along the x-axis to match the “by cost” distribution (yellow). c—e) Bend correlations between individual max pupil dilation difference and self-reported travelling frequency within different global contexts. Colored dots represent downweighted values in the x (red) and y direction (green) respectively. Some single-participant dots may be masked by dots of similar values.
Number of familiar and unfamiliar images selected for the pupillometry study
Individual pupil size changes as a robust indicator of cognitive familiarity differences

January 2022

·

301 Reads

·

19 Citations

Cognitive psychology has a long history of using physiological measures, such as pupillometry. However, their susceptibility to confounds introduced by stimulus properties, such as color and luminance, has limited their application. Pupil size measurements, in particular, require sophisticated experimental designs to dissociate relatively small changes in pupil diameter due to cognitive responses from larger ones elicited by changes in stimulus properties or the experimental environment. Here, building on previous research, we present a pupillometry paradigm that adapts the pupil to stimulus properties during the baseline period without revealing stimulus meaning or context by using a pixel-scrambled image mask around an intact image. We demonstrate its robustness in the context of pupillary responses to branded product familiarity. Results show larger average and peak pupil dilation for passively viewed familiar product images and an extended later temporal component representing differences in familiarity across participants (starting around 1400 ms post-stimulus onset). These amplitude differences are present for almost all participants at the single-participant level, and vary somewhat by product category. However, amplitude differences were absent during the baseline period. These findings demonstrate that involuntary pupil size measurements combined with the presented paradigm are successful in dissociating cognitive effects of familiarity from physical stimulus confounds.


Figure 1. Conceptual framework linking consumer experience, brand image, and brand loyalty
Examples of 4E's and negative/positive social interactions
Indicators of brand loyalty in consumers' five-star ratings
Examples of positive attitudinal statements in consumers' five-star ratings
How Winery Tourism Experience Builds Brand Image and Brand Loyalty

December 2021

·

411 Reads

·

9 Citations

Wine Business Journal

This research examines the role of the winery tourism experience in the formation of brand image and brand loyalty. A qualitative analysis of 2540 TripAdvisor reviews—a user-generated form of electronic word of mouth—of four wineries of the Okanagan Valley posted over six years (2014-2020) reveals not only Pine and Gilmore’s (1999) four categories of consumer experiences (i.e., esthetics, education, entertainment, and escape), but also an additional factor (i.e., social interactions with employees and other visitors). The TripAdvisor reviews also show that—based on their winery tourism experiences—consumers express differentiated brand image impressions associated with wineries and brand loyalty. The contribution of this research lies in the identification of social interactions as a complementary dimension of winery tourism experiences, and in linking winery tourism experiences with brand image and brand loyalty. From a theoretical perspective, the findings encourage a greater integration of the consumer experience and the brand image and loyalty literature, as well as quantitative research examining their relation. The findings also have managerial implications for brand experience management in the wine tourism sector.



Citations (39)


... Nevertheless, the literature suggests that this management practice can actually reduce sales by alienating curious consumers or flexitarians, who are the primary target market for PBMA (McClements, 2023). Moreover, placing products in close proximity supports efficient information processing and reinforces the perception that these products belong to the same category, as supported by the perceptual grouping theory (Tofighi and Grohmann, 2024). Fourth, confirming Reis' research (2023), animal-based protein-oriented brands are significantly more prevalent in supermarkets in terms of quantity compared to plant-based protein-oriented brands, and the burger is the most common format, followed by cutlets and meatballs, for both meat and PBMA. ...

Reference:

Retail practices for plant-based meat alternatives in Italy
The effect of retail shelf position on private-label brand evaluations
  • Citing Article
  • May 2024

International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management

... This strategic alignment between digital advancements and community development emphasizes transforming technological capabilities into tangible societal benefits (Joy et al., 2023). Studies such as D' Adamo et al., (2023aAdamo et al., ( , 2023b show how photovoltaic systems optimize energy consumption within community frameworks, enhancing local sustainability efforts. ...

Confronting climate crisis through corporate narratives: the fairy tale in LVMH’s 2020 and 2021 social and environmental responsibility reports

Luxury

... CSR marketing tactics affect brand perception and the industry environment. Openness and sincerity in com-munication impact consumer perception (Bodur, Tezer, & Grohmann, 2023). Practical CSR activities that focus on results and show social responsibility are more likely to boost brand perception. ...

Execution novelty: Improving brand evaluations in cause sponsorship
  • Citing Article
  • October 2023

Journal of Business Research

... However, research on CSR's effects has yielded inconsistent findings. Some studies assert that CSR can mitigate the impact of bad publicity (Bhattacharya et al., 2020;Cho and Kim, 2012;Choi and La, 2013;Eisingerich et al., 2011;Flammer, 2013;Godfrey et al., 2009;Jia et al., 2020;Kim, 2019, 2020;Kang et al., 2016;Kim and Choi, 2018;Kim and Park, 2020;Klein and Dawar, 2004;Lin et al., 2011;Minor and Morgan, 2011;Peasley et al., 2021;Price and Sun, 2017;Tsarenko and Tojib, 2015;Vanhamme and Grobben, 2009;Wu and Overton, 2022), while others find that CSR can worsen brand perceptions if seen as hypocritical (Demasi and Voegtlin, 2023;Effron et al., 2018;Kliamenakis et al., 2023;Liu et al., 2020;Love et al., 2022;Oh et al., 2017;Oikonomou et al., 2012;Shim and Yang, 2016;Wagner et al., 2009). Nevertheless, these studies typically explore either the buffering or backfiring effect of CSR without considering that CSR can have both effects (Ryoo, 2022;Vanhamme et al., 2015;Yoon et al., 2006). ...

Consumers' moral licensing of firms' CSR transgressions

... We investigated eye features associated with human memory. In particular, pupil size has been shown to effectively isolate cognitive effects of familiarity from physical stimulus characteristics [16]. Furthermore, research has shown distinct pupillary responses based on familiarity with branded products, highlighting the link between pupil dynamics and recognition processes [16]. ...

Individual pupil size changes as a robust indicator of cognitive familiarity differences

... Pine and Gilmore (1999) emphasize the importance of emotional engagement in creating lasting memories and generating economic value, highlighting that experiences are inherently personal and exist only in the individual's mind (Pine & Gilmore, 1999;Chan & Saikim, 2022). The emotional dimension is significant in forming loyalty and image in tourism experiences (Joy et al., 2021). The Emotional Dimension category articulates the emotional and sensory facets of backpacking visitors' experiences traversing the Lycian Way. ...

How Winery Tourism Experience Builds Brand Image and Brand Loyalty

Wine Business Journal

... Origin stories have been widely employed in storytelling as a powerful persuasive strategy (Júnior et al. 2023). This narrative follows a brand or product's history from its inception to the present (Tezer, Bodur, and Grohmann 2022), helping consumers acquire more brand or product knowledge, understand the intrinsic meaning (Shavitt and Barnes 2020), and enhance their comprehension of the essence and value of the product (Pfannes et al. 2021). Origin stories include the founder's story (Hamby, Brinberg, and Daniloski 2019), the hero's journey (Cooper et al. 2023), the brand biography (Delgado-Ballester 2021;Grohmann 2020, 2022), and the product origin story (Kamleitner, Thürridl, and Martin 2019). ...

When goliaths win and davids lose: The moderating role of perceived risk in brand biography effects

... The term terroir encompasses not just land but the combination of natural factors-such as soil, climate, and topography-that influence wine (Ballantyne et al., 2019;Wilson, 2001). Over centuries, French winemakers began to emphasize terroir to explain how specific environmental conditions and human practices plays a crucial role in shaping a wine's flavor, aroma, acidity, and body imparting a "sense of place" unique to each wine (Joy et al., 2021;Ballantyne et al., 2019). In France, terroir has become the organizing principle of the wine industry, tied to economic protection and classification systems (Fourcade, 2012). ...

The artification of wine: lessons from the fine wines of Bordeaux and Burgundy
  • Citing Article
  • January 2021

Arts and the Market

... On the other hand, wine novices develop their attitudes and make decisions largely based on price, vintage, and label design (Chocarro and Cortiñas, 2013;Robertson et al., 2018). Consumers with expertise in wine often take a holistic view of the product, encompassing the "big picture," whereas those new to wine focus on a limited set of attributes (Joy et al., 2020). ...

A multi-sensory and embodied understanding of wine consumption

Journal of Wine Research

... The findings indicate that dual-earner parents especially value the specific features of these systems. This aligns with Tofighi et al. (2020), who found that consumers favor utilitarian brands when there is congruity between the product attributes and the brand concept. Characteristics-related messages address the practical needs of dual-earner parents, who are more likely to invest in these technologies to increase daily efficiency. ...

Ethical attribute and brand concept congruity enhances brand evaluations
  • Citing Article
  • December 2019

European Journal of Marketing