Beverly I. Fagot’s research while affiliated with Oregon Social Learning Center and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (78)


Gender Labeling, Gender Stereotyping, and Parenting Behaviors
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

March 1992

·

1,967 Reads

·

114 Citations

Developmental Psychology

Beverly I. Fagot

·

Mary D. Leinbach

·

Cherie O'Boyle

Are gender labeling and gender stereotyping in 24-, 30-, and 36-mo-old children related to each other and to mothers' sex-role attitudes and responses to sex-typed behavior in a free-play situation with their children? The gender stereotyping measure indicated that gender schemata include information that is metaphorically rather than literally associated with each sex. Children who understood labels for boys and girls displayed more knowledge of gender stereotypes than children who did not. Mothers whose children had mastered labels for boys and girls endorsed more traditional attitudes toward women and toward sex roles within the family. The same mothers also initiated and reinforced more sex-typed toy play with their children. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

View access options

Observations of Parent Reactions to Sex-Stereotyped Behaviors: Age and Sex Effects

July 1991

·

113 Reads

·

212 Citations

Child Development

To examine differential socialization of boys and girls by mothers and fathers, home observations were completed for families of 92 12-month-old children, 82 18-month-old children, and 172 5-year-old children. Mothers gave more instructions and directions than did fathers, while fathers spent more time in positive play interaction. Differences in parents' reactions to 12- and 18-month boys and girls were as expected, with the exception that boys received more negative comment for communication attempts than did girls. The suggestion in the literature that fathers would be more involved in sex typing than mothers was not confirmed in this study. The only 2 significant sex-of-parent x sex-of-child effects occurred at 18 months; fathers gave fewer positive reactions to boys engaging in female-typical toy play, and mothers gave more instruction to girls when they attempted to communicate. We argue that the second year of life is the time when children are learning many new skills and when parents are still experimenting with parenting styles and may well use stereotypical responses when unsure of themselves.


Is there coherence among the cognitive components of gender acquisition?

February 1991

·

19 Reads

·

25 Citations

This study examined the relationships among five measures that assess various cognitive components of the child's acquisition of gender. At around 2 years of age, children were given a task assessing their ability to accurately label as a boy or a girl some head-and-shoulders pictures of boys and girls. At 4 years of age, these children were given tasks measuring (1) the degree to which they found gender a salient parameter of categorization, (2) the amount of gender-related knowledge they could display (SERLI-SRD), (3) the degree to which their preferences were gender-typed (SERLI-SRP) and (4) the accuracy of their memory for gender-typed information. There was no consistent pattern of relationship among the children's scores on these five tools for measuring gender acquisition. Our findings suggest that gender is a multidimensional construct in children's development, and thus these results challenge the undimensional manner in which gender is repeatedly addressed in developmental theory and research.


Attractiveness in young children: Sex-differentiated reactions of adults

January 1991

·

528 Reads

·

15 Citations

Attractiveness ratings of 50 children aged 12–38 months were investigated in relation to adult socializing agents' behavior toward the children in their homes and play groups and their attitudes toward the children. Attractiveness scores were derived from 43 judges' ratings of facial photographs of the children. The same children were observed in play groups with female care givers; 40 of them were also observed in their homes with their parents. Play-group care givers rated the children for likability, tractability, and peer relations. Parents rated their attitudes toward giving routine care to their child. Attractiveness scores were not correlated with any adult attitude measures. Girls' attractiveness scores were positively correlated with their attempts to communicate in home and play group, and with mothers', fathers', and care givers' instructional responses, but negatively correlated with passive behavior in the home and with mothers' positive reactions. Boys' attractiveness scores were negatively correlated with passive behavior in the play group; no other significant correlations were found in the data for boys.


Are people's notions of maleness more stereotypically framed than their notions of femaleness?

August 1990

·

56 Reads

·

98 Citations

In the present research, 400 undergraduates were asked to describe their various construals of the male and female stereotypes using a modified form of the Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ; Spence, Helmreich, & Stapp, 1974). Half of these subjects described their construals in terms of personality trait-adjectives and half described their construals in terms of appearance trait-adjectives. One-fourth of each subject pool were females describing males, one-fourth were females describing females, one-fourth were males describing females, and one-fourth were males describing males. All construals were scored such that each was depicted by a single score of stereotypicality ranging from nonstereotypic to extremely stereotypic. The scores from the various construals of the male and female stereotypes were averaged within subject and target groups, and the group means were compared for significant patterns of difference. Regardless of whether the subjects described the stereotypes in terms of personality trait-adjectives or in terms of appearence trait-adjectives, construals of maleness were framed more stereotypically than were construals of femaleness. In addition, construals of maleness were framed in particularly more stereotypic terms by females when the descriptions were phrased in terms of appearance trait-adjectives. Finally, the social construal of the male stereotype (How does society view males?) was more stereotypically framed by females using both personality and appearance adjectives, while the ideal construal male stereotype (Describe your ideal male) was more stereotypically framed by males using both personality and appearance adjectives.


Sex differences in responses to the stranger in the strange situation

August 1990

·

35 Reads

·

3 Citations

Children are exposed to differences in adult interactive styles from an early age. The Ainsworth Strange Situation. designed as a standard measure to activate attachment behaviors in the young child, allows us to examine the child's reaction to individual differences in strangers' styles. In the present study, the effect of 11 different strangers was examined to determine if different stranger styles influenced the scoring of behavioral ratings of the child's behavior in the Strange Situation. Boys and girls reacted differently to the different strangers. Eighteen-month-old children showed more variations in their reactions to different interactional styles than did 12-month-olds. Boys showed more resistance and avoidance to strangers who used more direction and initiation. Girls did not react this way. The results are discussed in terms of the child's expectations of sex-determined styles of interaction learned from past social interactions.


The Prediction of Antisocial Behavior from Avoidant Attachment Classifications

July 1990

·

53 Reads

·

156 Citations

Child Development

109 Children were classified using the Ainsworth Strange Situation at 18 months. 81 children who were unequivocally classified as insecure/avoidant (A1, A2) or securely attached (B1, B2, B3) were used in this study. The children's parents reported on occurrences of problem behaviors at 24 months, 27 months, 30 months, and 48 months using several methods. The children were observed at 18 and 30 months in their homes with their families and in toddler playgroups during the same period. The only significant effect for attachment classification was that teachers and observers of the playgroups rated girls classified as insecure/avoidant as more difficult to deal with and as having more difficulty with peers than girls rated as securely attached.


The Young Child's Gender Schema: Environmental Input, Internal Organization

July 1989

·

183 Reads

·

181 Citations

Child Development

The relation among children's ability to apply gender labels, their tendency to emit sex-typed behavior, and their parents' attitudes and reactions toward sex-typed behaviors was studied. The children were observed at home with their parents when the children were 18 months old, before any of them had passed the gender-labeling task, and at 27 months, when half had passed (early labelers) and half had not (late labelers). At 18 months, there were no differences in the children's sex-typed behavior, but parents of future early labelers gave more positive and negative responses to sex-typed toy play. By 27 months, early labelers showed more traditional sex-typed behavior than late labelers; parents of early and late labelers no longer differed in their responses. At age 4, when given an inventory of sex stereotyping, early labelers scored higher on Sex Role Discrimination; there were no differeces on Sex Role Preference scores.


A Comparison of the Play Behaviors of Sexually Abused, Physically Abused, and Nonabused Preschool Children

July 1989

·

27 Reads

·

28 Citations

Topics in Early Childhood Special Education

Behavioral observations were performed on three groups of preschool children: nonabused, physically abused and neglected, and sexually abused. Physically abused and neglected children were more antisocial, disruptive, and aggressive yet passive. Sexually abused children were relatively quiet and engaged in more female-type activities. Peer interaction in the two groups of clinically referred children was significantly less positive than in the control group.


Chronic Stress and Coping Styles: A Comparison of Male and Female Undergraduates

December 1988

·

176 Reads

·

159 Citations

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

Male and female coping behaviors were compared in order to test the theory that men use instrumental coping strategies more frequently than women, who are thought to use emotion-focused coping solutions. We interviewed 51 female and 39 male first-year undergraduates by telephone three times a week for 8 weeks, using an inventory developed for 28 chronic stressors. Analyses of variance were used to test gender differences in frequency of daily stressors, concomitant perceptions of stress, and utilization of problem-solving behaviors. The majority of analyses showed no gender differences. The implications of these findings are discussed in light of cultural expectations.


Citations (72)


... The limitations stemming from insufficient access to education and job prospects often culminate in a multitude of hardships, particularly prevalent among recipients of social grants and aspiring entrepreneurs in South Africa. Research by Egeland, Carlson & Sroufe (1993); Steinberg, Darling, Fletcher, Brown & Dornbusch (1995) and Fagot & Gauvain (1997) illuminate the extensive ramifications, including diminished quality of life, strained familial dynamics, and weakened parent-child relationships. Within this context, as elucidated by Duckworth & Sabates (2005), the pivotal role of parental guidance, familial environment, and socio-economic factors becomes even more pronounced, shaping the trajectory and aspirations of individuals navigating the complex landscape of social grants and entrepreneurship in South Africa. ...

Reference:

Social Issues Affecting Social Cohesion in Low-resource Communities in South Africa
Mother–Child Problem Solving: Continuity Through the Early Childhood Years

Developmental Psychology

... Subsequently, these styles may adversely affect peer relationships, or children may have inhibitions about engaging in social behavior based on a history of harsh treatment by parents (Patterson, 1982;Anthony et al., 2005;Hartas, 2011). Concurrent and longitudinal studies have found that hostile parenting was associated with lower levels of social competence and fewer prosocial skills in preschool-aged children (Eddy et al., 2001;Laible et al., 2004;Anthony et al., 2005;Hartas, 2011;Walker and Mac Phee, 2011). Overall, this literature finds that responsive parenting and hostile/ overreactive parenting shape children's level of social competence. ...

Coercive family processes: A replication and extension of Patterson?s coercion model
  • Citing Article
  • January 2001

Aggressive Behavior

... Some evidence does suggest that traditional types of scaffolding (e.g. development of problem solving skills) contribute not only to cognitive development, but also to individual differences in social competence with peers (Leve & Fagot, 1997;Vandell & Wilson, 1987). Findings from one study of caregiver interventions in peer conflicts of older children provide some initial evidence regarding the effects of caregiver scaffolding. ...

Prediction of Positive Peer Relations from Observed Parent-Child Interactions
  • Citing Article
  • July 1997

Review of Social Development

... La repetición de este patrón interactivo, originado en el contexto familiar, se generalizará al contexto escolar y a las interacciones entre iguales, cuyas consecuencias serán el rechazo de sus compañeros, el bajo rendimiento académico, el descenso de la autoestima y la implicación con otros iguales problemáticos entre los que la conducta coercitiva será valorada y reforzada (Patterson, 1986). Más recientemente, Eddy, Leve y Fagot (2001) replican el modelo Coercitivo de Patterson. Sus resultados muestran que los datos se ajustan al modelo de forma similar tanto en chicos como en chicas y, consecuentemente, los procesos de coerción se aplican similarmente a ambos sexos. ...

Coercive family processes: A replication and extension of Patterson’s coercion model
  • Citing Article
  • January 2001

Aggressive Behavior

... Ketika seorang anak laki-laki suka bermain dengan boneka, ia masih terkungkung oleh kepercayaan "boneka hanya untuk anak perempuan". Ini terkait dengan apa yang dilaporkan Fagot (1995) bahwa ibu dan ayah memperlakukan anak laki-laki dan perempuan secara berbeda. Orang tua pada umumnya memilih membeli mainan terkait jenis kelamin yang berbeda untuk putra dan putri mereka. ...

Psychosocial and Cognitive Determinants of Early Gender-Role Development
  • Citing Article
  • January 1995

Annual Review of Sex Research

... Pour Eisenberg, Murray, and Hite (1982), ces différences d'intérêt genrées pourraient être liées à la fonction, à l'utilisation de ces jouets. Ainsi ces jouets stéréotypés seraient associés à des activités qui influencent le développement de compétences cognitives sexuées, les poupées stimulant par exemple les comportements de soin et de maternage (Connor & Serbin, 1977;Fagot & Littman, 1976 ;Robert & Heroux, 2004 ;Serbin & Connor, 1979 ;Voyer, Nolan, & Voyer, 2000), le développement de traits de personnalité sexués (Eisenberg, Murray, & Hite, 1982) ainsi que le développement de patterns d'interactions sociales entre adultes (Maccoby, 1998). Serbin et al. ...

Relation of Preschool Sex-Typing to Intellectual Performance in Elementary School
  • Citing Article
  • December 1976

Psychological Reports

... However, study results may not generalize to samples who have committed more serious criminal behavior or to community samples. Notably, there may be a greater range of betweenindividual differences in parental monitoring that is not captured in the current sample due to the overall greater level of family dysfunction in high-risk samples (Fisher et al., 1998). Without properly accounting for between-individual variation in parental monitoring, it is difficult to conclude how parental monitoring may function over time in a wide variety of samples. ...

Assessment of Family Stress across Low-, Medium-, and High-Risk Samples Using the Family Events Checklist
  • Citing Article
  • July 1998

Family Relations

... A secure attachment is also associated with better syntax development (90) and overall communication (89,94). On the contrary, maltreating caregivers interact less, ignore their children more, react infrequently when their children talk, and use less diverse vocabulary and syntactic structures during communication (95)(96)(97). These children, in turn, demonstrate delays in syntactic development, reduced vocabulary, poor auditory comprehension skills and pragmatic impairments (98)(99)(100). ...

Interactions Between Children and Abusive Versus Control Parents
  • Citing Article
  • June 1988

Journal of Clinical Child Psychology

... Taylor et al. (2013) found that, when comparing the color preferences of British adults with those of Himba adults (a nonindustrialized population in rural Namibia), gender-typed pink-blue preferences are not universal and are based on culture. Thus, young children may establish gender-based knowledge on abstract attributes, such as color, as they gradually interact with the world around them (Leinbach et al., 1997;Yeung & Wong, 2018). It is interesting that even if children's responses are gender-neutral, some drawings show stereotyped aesthetics. ...

Bears are for boys: Metaphorical associations in young children's gender stereotypes
  • Citing Article
  • March 1997

Cognitive Development

... These results are also consistent with former findings that girls are (already) more open-minded about a greater variety of different sex roles and therefore more willing to take over non-traditional roles (see Cramer & Skidd, 1992;Kasten, 1986). In general, the male sex role is more rigidly defined than the traditional female role (Burge, 1981;Fagot & Littman, 1975;Freeman, 2007) and also enjoys a higher status (Helwig, 1998). Thus, boys might receive more negative reinforcement by caregivers and even peers for showing non-traditional behavior (Morrow, 2006). ...

Stability of Sex Role and Play Interests from Preschool to Elementary School
  • Citing Article
  • March 1975