Austin Roorda’s research while affiliated with University of California, Berkeley and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (310)


Figure 3: Saccade rate and performance. A). Saccade rate for a representative subject over time. The average number of saccades per second is plotted as a function of time. Stimulus onset is at time 0. The gray bar represents the time the stimulus was presented. The green and red lines represent saccade rate during trials in which the observer responded correctly and incorrectly. B). Last saccades and performance. Percent-correct performance in the acuity task is plotted as a function of how much time the last microsaccade preceded stimulus presentation. The data have been averaged across the 5 subjects. Each data point is the percent correct for 80msec bins centered on the time values on the abscissa. Percent correct was calculated excluding trials with Vernier offsets of +/-18secarc because performance was at ceiling for those trials. Error bars are standard errors of the mean.
Figure 4: Variation of fixation during the course of a trial. A) ISOA over time. Area of the ISO contour containing 68% of retinal positions is plotted over the 2-sec duration of a trial for a representative subject. The time at which the stimulus was presented is represented by the gray bar. The data have been averaged across all 2100 trials. A running median was applied at each time point over 20 preceding and 20 succeeding time samples. Green and red lines represent contour areas on correct and incorrect trials, respectively. Shaded regions around each line indicate standard deviations. B) Fixation relative to PRL and CDC during the course of a trial. The lower solid curves represent median distance of fixation from the PRL over the duration of a trial for the same subject. The distance goes to zero at the time of the stimulus, by definition. The gray bar represents stimulus presentation time. The data have been averaged across 2100 trials. The green and red lines represent distance from PRL on correct and incorrect trials, respectively. The upper dashed curves represent the median distance of fixation from the CDC for the same subject. Again green and red represent correct and incorrect responses.
The Optimal Retinal Locus for High-Resolution Vision in Space and Time
  • Preprint
  • File available

May 2025

·

8 Reads

Josselin Gautier

·

Norick R Bowers

·

·

Austin Roorda

Humans exhibit machine-like eye-movement behavior in space and time while performing challenging visual resolution tasks. Fewer microsaccades occur as stimulus presentation is imminent. Drifts and microsaccades combine to confine the landing location of an anticipated visual stimulus to a tiny retinal region: the preferred retinal locus (PRL). We find that this location confers the best visual acuity despite it being offset from the anatomical fovea (the location of maximum cone density). We also find that acuity is best when the last microsaccade occurs 400msec or longer before stimulus presentation. The machine-like eye movements are involuntary and not perceived. Our findings thus reveal a highly evolved oculomotor system such that gaze direction during fixation is rarely far enough from the PRL to cause a decline in visual resolution. Teaser Humans exhibit machine-like eye-movement behavior during fine visual tasks that rarely deviates from an optimal retinal location.

Download









A paradoxical misperception of relative motion

November 2024

·

4 Reads

·

1 Citation

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

Detecting the motion of an object relative to a world-fixed frame of reference is an exquisite human capability [G. E. Legge, F. Campbell, Vis. Res. 21 , 205–213 (1981)]. However, there is a special condition where humans are unable to accurately detect relative motion: Images moving in a direction consistent with retinal slip where the motion is unnaturally amplified can, under some conditions, appear stable [D. W. Arathorn, S. B. Stevenson, Q. Yang, P. Tiruveedhula, A. Roorda, J. Vis. 13 , 22 (2013)]. We asked: Is world-fixed retinal image background content necessary for the visual system to compute the direction of eye motion, and consequently generate stable percepts of images moving with amplified slip? Or, are nonvisual cues sufficient? Subjects adjusted the parameters of a stimulus moving in a random trajectory to match the perceived motion of images moving contingent to the retina. Experiments were done with and without retinal image background content. The perceived motion of stimuli moving with amplified retinal slip was suppressed in the presence of a visible background; however, higher magnitudes of motion were perceived under conditions when there was none. Our results demonstrate that the presence of retinal image background content is essential for the visual system to compute its direction of motion. The visual content that might be thought to provide a strong frame of reference to detect amplified retinal slips, instead paradoxically drives the misperception of relative motion.


Citations (50)


... Pursuing optimal visual outcomes following cataract surgery remains a cornerstone of modern ophthalmology [1]. Central to this objective is the precise calculation of intraocular lens (IOL) power, which is instrumental in determining the refractive outcome post-surgery. ...

Reference:

Comparison of Traditional and AI-Based Methods: Barrett Universal II vs. Ladas Super Formula in IOL Power Calculation
Roadmap on Advances in Visual and Physiological Optics
  • Citing Preprint
  • November 2024

... 21 It is not clear how, for example, an artificial lens or deformations of the retina due to retinal detachment impact the projection. More research is required, but with better knowledge of the individual eye, one can envision a re-projection through the exact anatomy using sophisticated mathematical eye models, 22,23 which could reduce the associated uncertainties. ...

Wide-field optical eye models for emmetropic and myopic eyes

Journal of Vision

... He concludes that "any slowly-accumulated weak responses that may be due to the drifting eye are negligible relative to the strong persistent volley generated by the landing saccade". But Examples of real (no drawings) eye drift traces acquired with three instruments: (A) a digital Dual Purkinje Image eye-tracker (Wu et al., 2023); (B) an adaptive optics scanning laser ophthalmoscope (Roorda et al., 2002;Moon et al., 2024); (C) the oscillating field monitor, a specially designed coil-based system that enables recording of fine eye movements without restraining the head (Eibenberger et al., 2016). (D) Instantaneous speed distribution of eye drift during normal head-free fixation (from Aytekin et al. (2014)). ...

Alignment, calibration, and validation of an adaptive optics scanning laser ophthalmoscope for high-resolution human foveal imaging

... It has been shown that as the beam diameter increases, the threshold for two-photon vision decreases, which is associated with a smaller spot size on the retina [17]. However, due to aberrations naturally occurring in the eye, without adaptive optics correction, the full NA = 0.23 for the dilated pupil cannot be fully exploited [18]. ...

Boosting 2-photon vision with adaptive optics
  • Citing Article
  • October 2023

Journal of Vision

... Another outstanding issue is that loss of Nr2e3 produces distinct phenotypes in mice and humans (19). While rd7 mice show relatively modest changes in rod gene expression and exhibit no evidence of enhanced sensitivity to short-wavelength light, humans with mutations in NR2E3 have enhanced S-cone syndrome characterized by a supranormal ERG response to shortwavelength light (44). These observations suggest that the function of NR2E3 is not entirely conserved between the mouse and human and that NR2E3-mutant human rods may be transfated into supernumerary blue cones. ...

Enhanced S-Cone Syndrome: Elevated Cone Counts Confer Supernormal Visual Acuity in the S-Cone Pathway

Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science

... In most retinal degenerative diseases, the function of photoreceptors, including rods and all types of cones, is attenuated, whereas in ESCS, the ability of the blue cone is enhanced based on an increase in the number of these cells [28]. Several research groups have studied the reasons for the increased blue cone in ESCS. ...

Enhanced S-cone Syndrome, a Mini-review
  • Citing Article
  • July 2023

Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology

... As discussed briefly in the Introduction, veridical features of the environment are not immediately available (and arguably never become available) to a viewer. The viewer must construct a mental representation of the veridical features through a combination of sensory information (Kanai & Verstraten, 2006;Perdreau & Cavanagh, 2013;Vanston et al., 2023). For elevation, this could be a combination of retinal position, vestibular feedback, and contextual cues like the position of the ground or uprightness of objects. ...

It's not easy seeing green: The veridical perception of small spots
  • Citing Article
  • May 2023

Journal of Vision

... Adaptive optics optical coherence tomography (AO-OCT) is a powerful imaging modality that allows for cellular imaging in the living human retina [1][2][3]. Using this technology enables the visualization of various cell types, such as cone photoreceptors [4,5], rod photoreceptors [6,7], retinal pigment epithelium cells [8][9][10], ganglion cells [11,12] and microglia [13,14]. Despite the success of this imaging modality in healthy subjects, the translation to clinically relevant cases is rather challenging. ...

Evolution of adaptive optics retinal imaging [Invited]

... While controlling diffraction and scatter is challenging, monochromatic aberrations (both lower and higher orders) can be effectively measured and corrected with modern optical and surgical techniques [1,2]. However, despite these advancements, ocular chromatic aberrations, especially longitudinal chromatic aberration (LCA), are known to significantly degrade visual quality [3][4][5][6]. Actively controlling chromatic aberrations remains difficult. ...

The visual benefits of correcting longitudinal and transverse chromatic aberration

Journal of Vision