February 1985
·
31 Reads
·
11 Citations
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
February 1985
·
31 Reads
·
11 Citations
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis
January 1985
·
4 Reads
·
24 Citations
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment
Children (75 female, 52 male) in grades 2 through 8 completed the Fear Survey Schedule for Children (FSS-FC). Their mothers completed the same questionnaire twice: once for themselves and a second time to estimate their children''s fears. Spearman''s rhos indicated that there was a high degree of correspondence between the mothers'' rank-ordered estimates of their children''s fears and the children''s rank-ordered self-reports (=.86 for males, =.96 for females.However, mothers'' overall estimates of their children''s general fearfulness were significantly correlated only with their daughters'' fearfulness, not with their sons''. Further analyses showed that mothers could generally identify their children''s highest-rated fears. All test-retest measures were significant. In summary, these results suggest that mothers may provide useful clinical information about their children''s specific fears and that the FSS-FC is a reliable instrument with which to assess children''s fears.
November 1984
·
8 Reads
·
4 Citations
A comparison of scores from the Slosson and McCarthy Scales with exceptional preschool children (both retarded and gifted) showed Slosson scores were significantly higher. Caution is recommended when using the Slosson as a screening instrument.
February 1983
·
6,986 Reads
·
104 Citations
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis
The negative side effects of aversive control have been extensively discussed in clinical literature and textbooks. The symmetry between aversive and appetitive control in basic experimental research implies that parallel negative side effects of reward exist. These negative side effects are described and their implications for clinical practice and research are discussed.
December 1982
·
19 Reads
·
5 Citations
Child & Youth Care Forum
Preschool teachers and staff completed questionnaires for 58 preschool children. Ratings for overall ability, verbal and quantitative skills, perceptual performance skills as well as likability and sociability were obtained. The pooled ratings were correlated with McCarthy Scale scores for the children. Very strong (r>.63) correlations were found between teachers' estimates and the children's McCarthy scores. No significant correlations were found between likability or sociability estimates and the child's McCarthy scores. A mild correlation (r=.29) was found between teachers' estimates of sociability and their overall estimate of ability, while no significant correlation was found between likability estimates and overall ability estimates. In general, it appears that preschool teachers can provide reasonable estimates of children's cognitive skills without undue bias from non-academic sources.
April 1982
·
6 Reads
·
3 Citations
A comparison was made between the Slosson Intelligence Test and the McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities for 44 minority preschool children. The mean Slosson score was 13.8 points higher than the mean McCarthy score and the correlation between scores on the two tests was .81. One source of this discrepancy in means may be the heavy reliance the Slosson has upon the 1960 Stanford-Binet. The potential effects of inflated Slosson scores relative to McCarthy scores for screening purposes are discussed.
February 1982
·
5 Reads
·
4 Citations
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
Twelve preschool children participated in a study of the effects of explicit training on the imitation of modeled behavior. The responses trained involved a marble-dropping pattern that differed from the modeled pattern. Training consisted of physical prompts and verbal praise during a single session. No prompts or praise were used during test periods. After operant levels of the experimental responses were measured, training either preceded or was interposed within a series of exposures to modeled behavior that differed from the trained behavior. Children who were initially exposed to a modeling session immediately imitated, whereas those children who were initially trained immediately performed the appropriate response. Children initially trained on one pattern generally continued to exhibit that pattern even after many modeling sessions. Children who first viewed the modeled response and then were exposed to explicit training of a different response reversed their response pattern from the trained response to the modeled response within a few sessions. The results suggest that under certain conditions explicit training will exert greater control over responding than immediate modeling stimuli.
January 1982
·
26 Reads
·
59 Citations
Child & Family Behavior Therapy
Investigated the effectiveness of graduated exposure, verbal coping skills, and their combination in treating fear of the dark in 32 4- and 5-yr-old normal children, using both behavioral and subjective measures of fearfulness. Only those groups that received direct exposure to the dark during treatment demonstrated significant changes in dark tolerance. Nonsignificant differences in dark tolerance for high- vs low-instructional demand conditions in 2 posttests were obtained. Analysis of subjective fear ratings suggested questionable validity in Ss' abilities to label subjective fearfulness. (20 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
November 1978
·
4 Reads
·
2 Citations
Behavior Therapy
October 1978
·
3 Reads
·
4 Citations
Teaching of Psychology
Students achieved skill in observation and assessment techniques, but did not fully develop the objectivity hoped for.
... IMPORTANCE OF COUNTERCONTROL Approximately 25 years after Skinner formally introduced the concept, Balsam and Bondy (1978) argued that countercontrol had yet to facilitate analysis of complex behavior and implied that we would be better off without it. Given that countercontrol is but a subclass of negative reinforcement, might it be preferable to discard the term? ...
Reference:
Countercontrol in behavior analysis
November 1978
Behavior Therapy
... Approaches designed to help students be active learners have shown promise in large lecture settings (Bleske-Rechek, 2001;Butler, Phillmann, & Smart, 2001) as well as in specific areas such as statistics (Dolinsky, 2001;Perkins & Saris, 2001) and psychology and law (Perry, Huss, McAuliff & Galas, 1996). More specifically, actively practicing examination material in an examination format (Balch, 1998) and actively reviewing questions (Bondy, 1978) can improve subsequent examination performance. ...
October 1978
Teaching of Psychology
... A total of 64 studies was considered methodologically robust in that they met M.1 to M.5 criteria. Twelve studies did not meet M.5 criterion because the sample size per condition/comparator was too small to detect reliable effects (i.e., Attwood et al., 2012;Barrett et al., 2001;Dewis et al., 2001;Gallagher et al., 2004;Ginsburg & Drake, 2002;Hains, 1992;Muris et al., 2002Muris et al., , 1998Sheslow et al., 1983;Stallard et al., 2011;Tillfors et al., 2011;Whiteside et al., 2015). ...
January 1982
Child & Family Behavior Therapy
... A number of studies indicate that parents can accurately report on their children's fears. 7,8 Researchers have found a high correlation between mothers' estimates and children's self-reports of their most common fears. 8 Studies also suggest that parents can sometimes underestimate the intensity or frequency of specific fright experiences their child has had. ...
January 1985
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment
... Participants were recruited in the fall of 1995 and 1996 during their Head Start year and were tested during their kindergarten year. Children were administered the PPVT-R (Dunn & Dunn, 1981) and the McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities (Bondy, Norcross, & Constantino, 1982) during the spring of their kindergarten year. Teachers completed the CPSCS (Levine, Elzey, & Lewis, 1969) during the spring of the children's kindergarten year. ...
December 1982
Child & Youth Care Forum
... In Cameron, Banko, and Pierce's study, negative effects were shown in high-interest tasks when the rewards were tangible, expected (offered beforehand), and loosely tied to the level of performance (Cameron et al., 2001). Balsam and Bondy (1983) also argued the symmetry between aversive and appetitive control in basic experimental research implied that parallel negative side effects of reward did exist. Therefore, the effectiveness of reward and punishment still remains unclear. ...
February 1983
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis
... Interestingly, researchers have found covariation in some instances of thumb sucking and hair pulling (Friman & Hove, 1987;Watson & Allen, 1993). Covariation has been defined as changes in the response rate of one behavior that are correlated with changes in the response rate of another behavior (Baslam & Bondy, 1985). For example, Sanchez (1979) reduced both finger sucking and hair pulling by directly targeting finger sucking for intervention. ...
February 1985
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis
... Little is known about how imitation procedures interact with other training procedures (Bondy, 1982); nevertheless, negative modeling has been of concern to those who question the efficacy of aversive control (Table 1, column 10). Individuals whose behavior is punished may observe and learn to control others in a similar manner. ...
Reference:
The negative effects of reward
February 1982
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior