Alexandra Horowitz’s research while affiliated with DeVry College and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (9)


Introduction to Dog Behavior
  • Chapter

May 2022

·

93 Reads

·

8 Citations

Julie Hecht

·

Alexandra Horowitz

Our relationship with dogs runs thousands of years deep. Today, we might know dogs intimately as members of our human family, but we can also know and consider dogs on their own terms, as members of Canis familiaris , with a unique evolutionary history and species‐specific characteristics and needs. This chapter is a resource for all types of dog knowers and caretakers. It relies heavily on empirical research to anchor readers in the foundations of canine behavior—such as dog behavioral development, normal dog behavior, factors influencing behavior, and relationships with people—and considers how these topics affect dogs of all ages and backgrounds who find themselves in the shelter environment.


Introduction to dog behavior

May 2017

·

398 Reads

·

13 Citations

This chapter begins with the evolutionary history of dogs. A debate rages about how long ago, and where, a distinct species of dog appeared, given conflicting evidence from archeological sites and genetic analyses. When interacting with dogs, people need to be aware of dog visual, acoustic, and olfactory communication. Olfaction plays an important role in intra- and inter-specific social encounters. The chapter discusses the various patterns of dog communication that are particularly relevant for shelter and foster-care settings. It also presents some of the factors that can affect dog in-shelter behavior. Dogs tend to be on leash (or in kennels) when seeing other dogs, and interaction might be thwarted due to shelter regulations. Because of the importance of inter- and intra-specific interactions and exposure to stimuli and social experiences, shelters with puppies under their care should prioritize early-life socialization or find appropriate housing outside the shelter.


Relation between variables and positive-affect bouts. a High levels of physical contact (“Touch”) were highly correlated with positive-affect bouts (p < 0.001). b High levels of physical activity (“Movement”) were highly correlated with positive-affect bouts (p < 0.001)
Comparison of rates of positive- and neutral-affect bouts among the top three play types: fetch, tease, and tug (*p < 0.001)
Screenshot of positive-affect, active, high-contact play
Screenshot of a neutral-affect, low-movement, low-contact play
Examining dog–human play: the characteristics, affect, and vocalizations of a unique interspecific interaction
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

July 2016

·

397 Reads

·

42 Citations

Animal Cognition

Despite the growing interest in research on the interaction between humans and dogs, only a very few research projects focus on the routines between dogs and their owners. In this study, we investigated one such routine: dog-human play. Dyadic interspecific play is known to be a common interaction between owner and charge, but the details of what counts as play have not been thoroughly researched. Similarly, though people represent that "play" is pleasurable, no study has yet undertaken to determine whether different forms of play are associated with different affective states. Thus, we aimed to generate an inventory of the forms of dyadic play, the vocalizations within play, and to investigate the relationship of affect to elements of play. Via a global citizen science project, we solicited videotapes of dog-human play sessions from dog owners. We coded 187 play bouts via frame-by-frame video playback. We then assessed the relationship between various intra-bout variables and owner affect (positive or neutral) during play (dog affect was overwhelmingly positive). Amount of physical contact ("touch"), level of activity of owner ("movement"), and physical closeness of dog-owner dyad ("proximity") were highly correlated with positive affect. Owner vocalizations were found to contain different elements in positive- and neutral-affect play. One novel category of play, "tease", was found. We conclude that not all play is created equal: the experience of play to the owner participant is strongly related to a few identifiable characteristics of the interaction.

View access options

Seeing Dogs: Human Preferences for Dog Physical Attributes

March 2015

·

979 Reads

·

52 Citations

This study systematically investigated peoples' preferences for dog physical attributes in images of real-life dogs. Participants (n = 124) completed an aesthetic-preference test wherein they viewed 80 image pairs of mixed-breed adult dogs and were asked which image they “liked best.” In each image pair, the two dog images appeared to be nearly identical, but unbeknownst to participants, the images differed slightly in that one physical characteristic had been manipulated. By altering a single physical characteristic in each image pair, participant preference for each of 14 different physical attributes could be investigated over the course of the study. The feature manipulations examined theories relating to Kindchenschema (an infantile appearance, per Konrad lorenz), ostensive human similarity, the process of domestication, as well as differences in attribute size and symmetry. The results showed that participants preferred some features associated with the infant schema—large eyes and wider-set eyes. They also preferred certain human-like attributes: colored irises and a distinct upturn of the commissure, seeming to capture a smile. By contrast, participants did not reveal a preference for physical characteristics relating to feature size, symmetry or the process of domestication. The findings suggest that identifiable physical attributes of domestic dogs have aesthetic value and, additionally, that human preferences for aesthetic differences can be systematically investigated. Given that people attend to and care about the way dogs look, the effects of particular physical attributes on peoples' preferences merit investigation.



Looking at Dogs: Moving from Anthropocentrism to Canid Umwelt

February 2014

·

272 Reads

·

17 Citations

As a companion to humans, the domestic dog is naturally interpreted from a human-centered (anthropocentric) perspective. Indeed, dog behavior and actions are often explained by using anthropomorphisms: attributions to the dog that would hold if the actor were human. While sometimes useful, anthropomorphisms also have the potential to be misleading or incorrect. In this chapter we describe work to replace an anthropocentric perspective with a more dog-centered research program. First we detail research systematically testing anthropomorphisms of emotional complexity-the appearance of guilt and jealousy-that are made of dogs, by testing the context of appearance of the guilty look and by testing advantageous and disadvantageous inequity aversion. Relatedly, we describe research looking at the contribution of specific dog physical attributes to human preference and anthropomorphizing. Finally, we identify anthropocentric and canid-centric elements of our own and others' research, and suggest ways that research can be more sensitive to the dog's umwelt. © 2014 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. All rights are reserved.


Smelling more or less: Investigating the olfactory experience of the domestic dog

November 2013

·

378 Reads

·

65 Citations

Learning and Motivation

The performance of tracking dogs and drug-, disease-, and explosives-detection dogs is a testament to trained dogs’ olfactory acuity. The olfactory experience of an untrained dog, by contrast, has not been well documented. In the current research we begin to remedy that by testing untrained pet dogs’ olfactory perception of quantity. While previous research found that dogs could discriminate visible quantities of more or less food (Prato-Previde, Marshall-Pescini, & Valsecchi, 2008), our results find that, by contrast, companion dogs do not reliably discriminate quantities when the food can be smelled but not seen. Sixty-one percent of dogs (39 of 64), given a choice between closed plates with one and five morsels of food, approached plates with the larger quantity: not significantly more than approached plates with the lesser quantity (binomial, p = .169). We did find that during dogs’ initial investigation of both food amounts, subjects gave more attention to the plate containing the larger quantity (binomial, p < 0.001). In a second condition, we replicated, with closed plates, Prato-Previde et al.’s (2008) finding that owner interest in a plate holding a lesser quantity of food reliably leads dogs to approach that plate (binomial, p < 0.001). Though research has demonstrated dogs’ preference for a larger amount of food (Ward & Smuts, 2007), in a third condition testing the effect of adding a strong odor to a visibly larger food quantity, we found that the addition of odor often reversed that preference (44/69 dogs; p < .03). Finally, we consider the methodological implications of this work on future dog cognition studies.



Citations (8)


... Stereotypy is a problem often reported in animals in captivity and is nothing more than repetitive behavioral sequences without any defined functionality [17]. Another study also included behavioral disorders in this category: repeated jumping, self-mutilation, spinning in a circle or insistent licking of a body region [18]. ...

Reference:

Welfare assessment of a dog shelter using the Shelter Quality Protocol
Introduction to Dog Behavior
  • Citing Chapter
  • May 2022

... (14) Additionally, their amount of fur can impede seeing the individual's behavioral repertoire because it covers their face or piloerection. (20) In general, a dog's body size can predict variations in its behavioral traits, such as aggression, anxiety, and trainability. (21,22) For example, small breeds tend to exhibit behavioral issues more frequently, (23) whereas larger breeds display a greater inclination towards training. ...

Introduction to dog behavior
  • Citing Chapter
  • May 2017

... Interestingly, this is in direct contrast to human-dog interactions, in which dogs who initiated play more frequently were found to be significantly less amenable and more likely to display aggressive behaviors [50]. However, in general, activities involving physical contact and proximity during play have also been highly correlated with positive emotional scores in both dogs and their owners [51]. Therefore, the bias we found in active and passive interactions based on species preference could indicate suboptimal wellbeing for the less preferred species. ...

Examining dog–human play: the characteristics, affect, and vocalizations of a unique interspecific interaction

Animal Cognition

... Humans' emphasis on visual stimuli can contribute to a lack of consideration for the perception of other nonvisual sensory information. For example, research into dog (Canis familiaris) cognition often sets up conditions on the basis of human sensory abilities (e.g., examining dog behavior when their owner experiences a fake heart attack) without considering the sensory information that dogs might use in those situations (Horowitz & Hecht, 2014). Do dogs detect heart attacks visually? ...

Looking at Dogs: Moving from Anthropocentrism to Canid Umwelt
  • Citing Article
  • February 2014

... A citizen scientist is defined as, "a volunteer who collects and/or processes data as part of a scientific enquiry" (Silvertown, 2009). More recently citizen science methods have been used in the field of canine cognition research with dog owners and their dogs (Hecht and Rice, 2015;Horowitz and Hecht, 2014;Stewart et al., 2015). Dog owners are often enthusiastic about learning about and working with their dogs, and thus are often keen to engage with tasks relating to their pet (e.g. ...

Categories and consequences of dog-human play: A citizen science approach
  • Citing Article
  • November 2014

Journal of Veterinary Behavior

... For instance, several articles indicated that the appearance of dogs is the main factor that highly influences people's decisions to buy or adopt them [115][116][117]. Humans may choose some features that are thought to be associated with an infantile aesthetic, such as bigger eyes and a larger space between the eyes [118]. Also, dogs that can enhance paedomorphism (change the eye size and height by raising the inner brow) through greater facial flexibility are found to be more desirable to humans [119]. ...

Seeing Dogs: Human Preferences for Dog Physical Attributes
  • Citing Article
  • March 2015

... Previous studies have introduced visual landmarks to look at landmark learning in dogs [7,8,10]; cognition studies often fail to include an olfactory component in their experimental design, though dogs use olfaction as a primary sense [11]. In a human-designed world that is not organized around smell, dogs' use of olfaction can be reduced, but the presence of novel odors has the potential to change their behavior [12]. As such, in this study, we aim to investigate the usefulness of both visual and olfactory landmarks to examine their effect on dogs' spatial navigation. ...

Smelling more or less: Investigating the olfactory experience of the domestic dog
  • Citing Article
  • November 2013

Learning and Motivation

... This underpins the emerging interest in the dog-human dyad and the attributes that characterise successful bonds between members of the two species . Exploring which characteristics of canine morphology may lead to handlers ascribing anthropomorphic traits, such as guilt, is an interesting development in this area (Hecht and Horowitz, 2013). The anthrozoology of interactions between humans and horses has been effectively categorised to provide a template of how to interact with horses safely and has been shown to deliver excellent results under the banner of equitation science (Goodwin et al., 2009;Hawson et al., 2010;McLean and McGreevy, 2010). ...

Physical prompts to anthropomorphism of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)
  • Citing Article
  • July 2013

Journal of Veterinary Behavior