Alexander G. Stahlmann’s research while affiliated with University of Zurich and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (23)


Expressing Virtue in Natural Language: The Character Strengths Adjective Lists
  • Preprint

January 2023

·

3 Reads

·

2 Citations

Alexander Georg Stahlmann

·

Gian Carlo Arbenz

Personality aspects and proenvironmental attitudes

November 2022

·

147 Reads

·

14 Citations

Journal of Personality

Objective: Climate change is a serious threat. Personality psychologists can help address this threat by understanding what kind of people tend to endorse proenvironmental attitudes and engage in sustainable behavior. Previous research supports reliable associations between proenvironmental attitudes and personality traits. However, this research has generally aggregated different kinds of attitudes into a single composite and has focused on the domain level of personality traits. Method: This study explored how 10 lower-order aspects of the Big Five personality traits were related to eight different proenvironmental attitudes in three convenience samples from the United States (N = 1,234; 1,000) and the United Kingdom (N = 538). Results: All five trait domains were related to at least one proenvironmental attitude across all three samples. Seven of eight proenvironmental attitudes could be predicted by one or more traits in all three samples. We also found evidence that the Openness aspect of Openness to Experience was a more consistent predictor of proenvironmental attitudes than the Intellect aspect. In contrast, there was little benefit in distinguishing between the aspects of other trait domains. We did not find evidence that age or political orientation moderated the associations between proenvironmental attitudes and personality. Conclusion: Results point to the need for more fine-grained research on individual differences in proenvironmental attitudes and behavior.


Correlations of normal-range personality trait domains and aspects with compassion for animals.
Correlations of maladaptive personality trait domains with compassion for animals.
Correlations of interpersonal values and problems with compassion for animals.
Personality and Compassion for Animals
  • Article
  • Full-text available

June 2022

·

307 Reads

·

16 Citations

People vary in their compassion for animals, likely due in part to more variation in more basic personality and interpersonal behavior attributes. Previous research has generally suggested that more communal and agreeable people also tend to be more compassionate to animals. However, this research is limited regarding the range and depth of individual differences used to examine this issue. The goal of this preregistered study was to extend previous research by examining associations between compassion for animals and a wider range of variables than has been previously examined. In a representative sample of American adults (n = 992), we tested associations between compassion for animals and (a) Big Five personality trait domains, (b) Big Five trait aspects, (c) maladaptive Big Five trait domains, (d) interpersonal values, and (e) interpersonal problems. Results supported our hypothesis that compassion for animals is related to communion/agreeableness and openness to experience. Consistent with our hypotheses, the compassionate aspect of agreeableness drove correlations with that trait. Contrary to our hypotheses, maladaptive antagonism was not more strongly related to compassion for animals than normal-range agreeableness. The results provide a fuller portrait of the personological foundation of compassion for animals. Specifically, people who are more communal/agreeable and open tend to be more compassionate toward animals. This suggests that personality-related patterns of behavior among humans extend to human-animal interactions. Results also provide a basis for future work examining the mechanisms underlying human compassion for animals.

Download

Primal world beliefs correlate strongly but differentially with character strengths

May 2022

·

262 Reads

·

13 Citations

Primal world beliefs–primals–are a category of beliefs about the overall character of the world (e.g., the world is a safe place). Theory suggests that such beliefs drive personality development–or at least reflect personality differences, such as character strengths. We examined the relationships of primals with character strengths among 1122 German-speaking adults. The primary primal good explained the most variance in most character strengths, especially hope, spirituality, zest, gratitude, curiosity, and leadership. Including specific secondary (e.g., safe, enticing, alive) and tertiary primals (e.g., beautiful, needs me, funny) often yielded better predictions, but, with few exceptions, increments were typically smaller than that of the primary primal. We recommend including these primals in positive psychology interventions and describe three couplings of primals and character strengths that may prove especially fruitful for future research and practice.


Figure 2
Summary of Model Selection Indices when Arousal is Modeled as a Function of Valence
Results of the Multilevel Extension of Model 5
On the Relationship Between Valence and Arousal in Samples Across the Globe

April 2022

·

2,531 Reads

·

34 Citations

Emotion

·

Chiel Mues

·

·

[...]

·

Affect is involved in many psychological phenomena, but a descriptive structure, long sought, has been elusive. Valence and arousal are fundamental, and a key question-the focus of the present study-is the relationship between them. Valence is sometimes thought to be independent of arousal, but, in some studies (representing too few societies in the world) arousal was found to vary with valence. One common finding is that arousal is lowest at neutral valence and increases with both positive and negative valence: a symmetric V-shaped relationship. In the study reported here of self-reported affect during a remembered moment (N = 8,590), we tested the valence-arousal relationship in 33 societies with 25 different languages. The two most common hypotheses in the literature-independence and a symmetric V-shaped relationship-were not supported. With data of all samples pooled, arousal increased with positive but not negative valence. Valence accounted for between 5% (Finland) and 43% (China Beijing) of the variance in arousal. Although there is evidence for a structural relationship between the two, there is also a large amount of variability in this relation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Primal world beliefs correlate strongly but differentially with character strengths

August 2021

·

44 Reads

Primal world beliefs–primals–are a category of beliefs about the overall character of the world (e.g., the world is a safe place). Theory suggests that such beliefs drive personality development–or at least reflect personality differences, such as character strengths. We examined the relationships of primals with character strengths among 1122 German-speaking adults. The primary primal good explained the most variance in the majority of character strengths, especially hope, spirituality, zest, gratitude, curiosity, and leadership. Including specific secondary (e.g., safe, enticing, alive) and tertiary primals (e.g., beautiful, needs me, funny) often yielded better predictions, but, with few exceptions, increments were typically smaller than that of the primary primal. We recommend including these primals in positive psychology interventions and describe three couplings of primals and character strengths that may prove especially fruitful for future research and practice.



Improving Cross-Cultural Equivalence Through Scale-Specific Translation Guides: An Example Case of the Primals Inventory

February 2021

·

51 Reads

Despite sophisticated scale-generic guidance for adapting/translating self-report scales and widespread adherence to guidance, low invariance remains a problem in cross-cultural clinical research. This may be due to scale-specific translation challenges, and original scale-creation papers provide little information about item-writing choices. It is typically unexplained, for example, why important repeated terms are selected over synonyms, leaving translators in a lurch. To supplement scale-generic guidance, we specify conditions warranting the creation of scale-specific translation guides and offer a full exemplar guide concerning the Primals Inventory, a new measure of beliefs about the world’s general character (e.g., the world is dangerous) presumed to influence a variety of clinical outcomes. Primals Inventory-specific recommendations include how best to evoke the object of belief (e.g., world versus universe) and lessons learned from initial translation efforts (German and Italian). If this scale-specific guide proves useful, similar guides could be created for other scales, aiding cross-cultural research generally.


Means and 95% confidence intervals (with Bonferroni correction) of the Character Strengths’ Moral Evaluation Task’s (CS-MET) ratings as functions of the 24 VIA character strengths and the three Dark Triad traits. The panels depict the ratings for the different trial types: (A) stories without consequences, (B) stories with positive consequences, (C) stories with mixed consequences, and (D) stories with negative consequences. N = 230.
(A) Histograms of process dissociation scores (PDS) for consequentialism and deontology. (B) Results of the linear regression analysis of the PDS on the mean moral evaluation in the Character Strength’s Moral Evaluation Task’s (CS-MET) stories without consequences: Mean moral evaluation = –2.15 + 4.87 × PDS consequentialism + 5.39 × PDS deontology – 5.11 × PDS consequentialism × PDS deontology. R²Adjusted = 0.60; N = 230.
Correlations of character strengths (as measured by the VIA-IS) with their respective moral evaluations in the CS-MET, split across the four trial types.
Scrutinizing the Criteria for Character Strengths: Laypersons Assert That Every Strength Is Positively Morally Valued, Even in the Absence of Tangible Outcomes

September 2020

·

153 Reads

·

36 Citations

This study examines Peterson and Seligman’s (2004, p. 19) claim that every VIA character strength “(…) is morally valued in its own right, even in the absence of obvious beneficial outcomes”. Although this criterion assumes a pivotal role in distinguishing character from personality, no previous study has investigated its validity. Based on what Peterson and Seligman (2004) have provided us with, we describe how we built our study around indirectly testing every strength’s assumed moral evaluation, in which inclinations toward deontology (e.g., “torture is wrong regardless of tangible positive outcomes”) and consequentialism (e.g., “torture can be good if it accounts for more positive than negative outcomes”) may play a critical role. We used Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) handbook to construct four ultra-short stories for every strength: the stories depict various agents engaging in strength-related behavior (e.g., a young student courageously stepping up against school bullies). We prompted participants to rate these and twelve anchor stories multiple times as to whether the agents acted morally correct: In the first block, the actions’ consequences were undetermined while in the second block, the actions had either positive, negative, or mixed consequences, which we used to compute proxies of participants’ inclinations toward deontology and consequentialism. The ratings of N = 230 German-speaking laypersons suggest that the criterion stands: participants perceived every strength as positively morally valued when consequences were undetermined, and positive consequences did not account for or increase this effect. However, moral value seems to come in degrees, and some strengths were valued more strongly than others (top five: judgment, honesty, kindness, fairness, and hope). Furthermore, specific character strengths (measured by self-report) were connected with more positive evaluations (e.g., endorsing spirituality was connected with rating spirituality as more positively valued). Both deontology and consequentialism were connected with more positive evaluations, and we suggest two hypotheses to explain how such inclinations can lead to perceiving character strengths as positively valued. Our findings highlight the importance of scrutinizing the criteria for character strengths, and our experimental paradigm can offer a template to further investigate character strengths’ moral evaluation and other fundamental assumptions in upcoming studies.


Temporally aggregated trajectories (i.e., across participants) of the three-cluster-solutions for life and job satisfaction, respectively
Two-cluster and three-cluster solutions for life satisfaction (left) and job satisfaction (right), including contemporaneous mean levels (i.e., aggregated across measurements) and point-biserial correlations with gelotophobia. All correlation coefficients are significant with p ≤ .01 (adjusted via Holm’s correction)
Toward a dynamic model of Gelotophobia: Social support, workplace bullying and stress are connected with diverging trajectories of life and job satisfaction among Gelotophobes

September 2020

·

201 Reads

·

11 Citations

Current Psychology

Recent theoretical advances have grounded gelotophobia (Greek: gelos = laughter, phobos = fear) in a dynamic framework of causes, moderating factors, and consequences of the fear of being laughed at. This understanding corresponds to that of vulnerability and translates gelotophobia into a distinguishable pattern of lacking resources (i.e., misinterpretation of joy and laughter) that can result in negative consequences (e.g., reduced well-being and performance) if individuals have no access to further resources (e.g., social support) or are exposed to severe stressors (e.g., workplace bullying). Based on the panel data provided by the Swiss National Centre of Competence in Research LIVES (N = 2469 across six measurement intervals), this study takes the first step toward empirically testing this model’s assumptions: First, we computed exemplary zero-order correlations and showed that gelotophobia was negatively connected with social support (resource) and life and job satisfaction (consequences) and positively connected with perceived stress, work stress, and workplace bullying (stressors). Second, we used longitudinal cluster analyses (KmL; k-means-longitudinal) and showed that the panel data can be clustered into three stable patterns of life and job satisfaction and that gelotophobia is primarily related to the two clusters marked by lower levels of satisfaction. Third, we computed partial correlations and showed that social support, perceived stress, and work stress (but not workplace bullying) can weaken or completely resolve gelotophobia’s relationships with such diverging trajectories of life and job satisfaction. We concluded that seeing gelotophobia through the lens of vulnerability is useful and that such research warrants further attention using more dedicated, theoretically grounded projects.


Citations (15)


... This distinction is crucial because personality traits are difficult to influence or consider in individual or official communications, as discussed by several studies and reviews on the stability of personality traits in adulthood [14,15]. Personality traits have been associated with a range of preventive health behaviours, including vaccination [16]. While both personality traits and the 7C antecedents are important concepts that have been related to health behaviours and vaccination, no study to date has explored the how the 7C model may be tied to personality traits. ...

Reference:

Are psychological attitudes towards vaccination an expression of personality? A cross-sectional study on COVID-19 vaccination in France
Big Five Personality Traits and Vaccination: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Health Psychology

... In addition, people are attracted to conspiracy narratives e.g. when the social status of their group is perceived as low [5]. As we will explain below, there is evidence that the big five have (in part) effects on vaccination intention [6] and conspiracy beliefs, while at the same time there is an effect of conspiracy beliefs on vaccination intention. This suggests that there is also a mediating effect from at least some of the big five via conspiracy beliefs on the vaccination intention. ...

Big Five personality traits and vaccination: A systematic review and meta-analysis
  • Citing Preprint
  • April 2024

... This study was established by two relatively recent and related literature reviews on the character strengths (CS) model of personality traits in the workplace (Freidlin & Littman-Ovadia, 2020;Miglianico et al., 2020). According to Stahlmann et al. (2024), CS is the term used to describe the individual differences that lead to personal fulfilment, happiness, and satisfaction and are reflected in particular emotions, thoughts, and behaviours. It also refers to the ability to elevate people who observe these differences and inspire admiration rather than caution. ...

Definitional ambiguities in character strengths: A comparative analysis with personality and other psychobiological attributes
  • Citing Article
  • February 2024

Journal of Research in Personality

... Situational affordances enclosed in parentheses may be relevant to the expression of the character strength under certain conditions or at certain times They found significant differences among character strengths, such as social intelligence, judgment, and love (broader), compared to vitality, forgiveness, and gratitude (narrower). Stahlmann and Arbenz (2023) corroborated these findings through psycho-lexical analysis, revealing that narrower strengths often have comprehensive lexical representation-words that encapsulate the entire concept-whereas broader strengths lack this comprehensiveness. For instance, the adjectives 'vital,' 'zestful,' and 'vigorous' comprehensively capture the consensual definition of the character strength vitality. ...

Expressing Virtue in Natural Language: The Character Strengths Adjective Lists
  • Citing Preprint
  • January 2023

... Research suggests a complex relationship between personality traits and civic engagement (Dušanić et al., 2019;Stahlmann et al., 2023). Individuals high in agreeableness, extraversion and openness tend to participate more in civic activities (Stahlmann et al., 2023). ...

Big Five personality traits predict small but robust differences in civic engagement
  • Citing Article
  • April 2023

Journal of Personality

... Lastly, no work has comparatively examined model fit using an English instrument (the original scale's language). This is an important gap, as scale translations (Clifton et al. 2022) and cultural differences in mental health beliefs (Choudhry et al. 2016) may result in variability in factor structures and fit across diverse linguistic and cultural samples. ...

Improving Scale Equivalence by Increasing Access to Scale-Specific Information

Perspectives on Psychological Science

... In a recent study, compassion for animals was found to be related to the communion/agreeableness and openness to experience personality traits (Hopwood, Stahlmann, & Bleidorn, 2023). In the absence of a specific validated measure of compassion for animals, the researchers used a mixture of measures of attitudes towards animals, solidarity with animals, similarity to animals, and speciesism. ...

Personality and Compassion for Animals

... For the donation measure, none of the lower-level primals emerged as significant pre dictors when good world belief was accounted for. This observation is consistent with previous findings suggesting that the overall belief in a good world accounts for a large amount of the shared variance between specific primals and some traits (Stahlmann & Ruch, 2023). A certain amount of this redundancy is perhaps unsurprising, given that five of these six primals are considered lower-level facets of good world belief (with which they covary strongly), and that cooperative and harmless belief, in particular, share several items with this broader construct (Clifton et al., 2019). ...

Primal world beliefs correlate strongly but differentially with character strengths

... Affective salience should not be equated with arousal. Although in some stimulus sets, normative ratings of valence appear to have a v-shaped relationship with those of arousal: i.e. arousal increases linearly with the valence distance in both the positive and negative direction (Haj-Ali et al. 2020;Kron et al. 2015), there is much variation between people, circumstances (Kuppens et al. 2013), and cultures (Yik et al. 2023), indicative of a complex relationship. For instance, individuals appear to differ in the degree to which they focus on valence or arousal in constructing their conscious affective experience (Barrett 1998). ...

On the Relationship Between Valence and Arousal in Samples Across the Globe

Emotion