January 2024
·
6 Reads
Sleep Health
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
January 2024
·
6 Reads
Sleep Health
December 2023
·
204 Reads
·
5 Citations
Clinical and Translational Neuroscience
The two-process model of sleep regulation has served as a conceptual framework in the last four decades for understanding sleep physiology. In the 1970s, long-term recordings of sleep in rats were obtained thanks to EEG telemetry. NonREM sleep and REM sleep were found to differ in their time course and response to light-dark protocols. There were indications for their coupling to the circadian system, in particular the light-dark and the dark-light transitions. With the advent of quantitative EEG analysis, slow-wave activity in nonREM sleep was recognized as a sleep-wake-dependent variable. The term “sleep homeostasis” was coined to specify the regulated balance between sleep and waking. The regulatory homeostatic process was designated as “Process S”. In the two-process model, its interaction with the circadian pacemaker “Process C” can account for sleep duration under various experimental protocols. Local, use-dependent slow-wave activity changes were demonstrated in both humans and rats by the selective, unilateral activation of a cortical region prior to sleep. Finding that rest in invertebrates has sleep-like regulatory properties opened a new realm of animal studies. Comparative sleep studies in a broad variety of animal species confirmed the validity of the basic concepts of the two-process model. Recent studies have addressed sleep-related changes of brain temperature as an indicator of brain metabolism; the application of the model to Drosophila; the divergence of cortical and subcortical states; and sleep in an increasing number of species and taxa.
May 2022
·
490 Reads
·
83 Citations
Journal of Sleep Research
The two-process model serves as a major conceptual framework in sleep science. Although dating back more than four decades, it has not lost its relevance for research today. Retracing its origins, I describe how animal experiments aimed at exploring the oscillators driving the circadian sleep-wake rhythm led to the recognition of gradients of sleep states within the daily sleep period. Advances in signal analysis revealed that the level of slow-wave activity in non-rapid eye movement sleep electroencephalogram is high at the beginning of the 12-light period and then declines. After sleep deprivation, the level of slow-wave activity is enhanced. By scheduling recovery sleep to the animal's activity period, the conflict between the sleep-wake-dependent and the circadian influence resulted in a two-stage recovery pattern. These experiments provided the basis for the first version of the two-process model. Sleep deprivation experiments in humans showed that the decline of slow-wave activity during sleep is exponential. The two-process model posits that a sleep-wake-dependent homeostatic process (Process S) interacts with a process controlled by the circadian pacemaker (Process C). At present, homeostatic and circadian facets of sleep regulation are being investigated at the synaptic level as well as in the transcriptome and proteome domains. The notion of sleep has been extended from a global phenomenon to local representations, while the master circadian pacemaker has been supplemented by multiple peripheral oscillators. The original interpretation that the emergence of sleep may be viewed as an escape from the rigid control imposed by the circadian pacemaker is still upheld.
September 2020
·
335 Reads
·
16 Citations
eLife
In 2016 we reported evidence for associative learning in plants (Gagliano et al., 2016). In view of the far-reaching implications of this finding we welcome the attempt made by Markel to replicate our study (Markel, 2020). However, as we discuss here, the protocol employed by Markel was unsuitable for testing for associative learning.
January 2017
·
36 Reads
·
15 Citations
Journal of Sleep Research
Motor activity recording by a wrist-worn device is a common method to monitor the rest-activity cycle. The first author wore an actimeter continuously for more than three decades, starting in 1982 at the age of 43.5 years. Until November 2006 analysis was performed on a 15-min time base, and subsequently on a 2-min time base. The timing of night-time sleep was determined from the cessation and re-occurrence of daytime-level activity. Sleep duration declined from an initial 6.8 to 6 h in 2004. The declining trend was reversed upon retirement, whereas the variance of sleep duration declined throughout the recording period. Before retirement, a dominant 7-day rhythm of sleep duration as well as an annual periodicity was revealed by spectral analysis. These variations were attenuated or vanished during the years after retirement. We demonstrate the feasibility of continuous long-term motor activity recordings to study age-related variations of the rest-activity cycle. Here we show that the embeddedness in a professional environment imparts a temporal structure to sleep duration.
January 2017
·
142 Reads
·
161 Citations
December 2016
·
99 Reads
December 2016
·
94 Reads
December 2016
·
3,109 Reads
·
217 Citations
In complex and ever-changing environments, resources such as food are often scarce and unevenly distributed in space and time. Therefore, utilizing external cues to locate and remember high-quality sources allows more efficient foraging, thus increasing chances for survival. Associations between environmental cues and food are readily formed because of the tangible benefits they confer. While examples of the key role they play in shaping foraging behaviours are widespread in the animal world, the possibility that plants are also able to acquire learned associations to guide their foraging behaviour has never been demonstrated. Here we show that this type of learning occurs in the garden pea, Pisum sativum. By using a Y-maze task, we show that the position of a neutral cue, predicting the location of a light source, affected the direction of plant growth. This learned behaviour prevailed over innate phototropism. Notably, learning was successful only when it occurred during the subjective day, suggesting that behavioural performance is regulated by metabolic demands. Our results show that associative learning is an essential component of plant behaviour. We conclude that associative learning represents a universal adaptive mechanism shared by both animals and plants.
December 2016
·
80 Reads
... A later chronotype was only associated with increased variability in sleep timing, mirroring its well-known association with social jetlag 42 . More frequent napping was associated with more variable sleep composition, likely reflecting variable sleep pressure during nightly sleep 43 . Of these nominally significant (p < 0.01) correlations, only age effects survived a stringent control for multiple testing, a finding replicated using the two alternative sleep variability metrics as well. ...
December 2023
Clinical and Translational Neuroscience
... Disruptions in adenosine signaling can contribute to difficulties falling asleep and maintaining restful sleep, which are hallmarks of insomnia. 27,28,33 Molecular disruptions affecting circadian rhythms, homeostatic drive, and neurotransmitter systems, such as orexin, exacerbate insomnia by increasing arousal and reducing sleep efficiency. [29][30][31][32][33][34][35] The aim of this review is to provide a comprehensive analysis of psychosomatic influences on insomnia, examining the neurobiological, psychological, and somatic mechanisms involved. ...
May 2022
Journal of Sleep Research
... It has been proven that plants were able to perceive the stimulus (light) both in space and time and to build both positive and negative associations. Although Markel 134 attempted to refute Gagliano et al. 133 results in a repeated experiment, Gagliano et al. 135 138 Bhandawat et al.'s 139 study was the first and the only one to date with transcriptomic evidences for the associative use of the memory in plants. In their study, sound (green music, 50 dB) was the indicator (UCS) that associatively evoked the response to abiotic stress, the heat (CS) in Arabidopsis. ...
September 2020
eLife
... According to the homeostatic theory of sleep regulation, a decrease in sleep time should be followed by a subsequent compensatory increase 18 . In male dusky antechinuses a high activity level continued after the reproductive period ended, suggesting no evidence for 'sleep rebound'. ...
Reference:
Sleep: Giving it up to get it on
January 2017
... These observations suggest that PTG is also a master regulator of astrocyte glycogen, a central element of glycogen regulation in vivo. Furthermore, changes in PTG expression in the brain have been shown to be associated with different behavioral conditions related to glycogen mobilization, such as sleep deprivation and learning and memory [60][61][62]. Astrocytes are the most widely distributed type of cells in the mammalian brain and the largest type of glial cells. It is also the main storage site of glycogen in the brain. ...
July 2010
Sleep
... In recent decades, actigraphy has become more popular in infant research as an option to collect ecologically valid and non-biased data. The small, watch-like device may be worn at home for several consecutive days, weeks and even decades (Borbély, Rusterholz, & Achermann, 2017), assessing real-life sleep-wake patterns and circadian rhythms that are not distorted by laboratory settings. Actigraphy refers to a continuous measurement of human activity and rest periods using a tri-axial accelerometer. ...
January 2017
Journal of Sleep Research
... The tested biofeedback strategies are to optimize energy and water consumption and reduce the growth time via the optimization of spectral light and irrigation. In terms of AI research, we demonstrate a complex exploration of biological organisms, and in particular the adaptive mechanisms of circadian clocks [23,24]. The tested plant species include different microgreens (e.g., wheat and pea), productive plants (e.g., tomato and tobacco) and room plants such as dracena. ...
December 2016
... Se han descrito diversos componentes subcorticales involucrados en la promoción de la vigilia (sistemas activadores) o en la promoción del sueño (sistemas somnogénicos), incluso algunos específicamente relacionados con el sueño no-rem o con el rem (Torterolo y Vanini, 2010). Los sistemas activadores y somnogénicos se relacionan por inhibiciones recíprocas, de las que resulta un predominio de uno u otro en distintos momentos del día (Borbély, Dijk, Achermann y Tobler, 2001). ...
January 2001
... In the COVID-19 setting, two aspects can directly influence melatonin: the effects of lockdown and the dynamics of working from home. It is known that the circadian rhythm of sleep/wakefulness is regulated by two main components: a circadian (24-h) component and a homeostatic component (Borbély et al. 2016). The circadian rhythm is controlled by daylight exposure and daily routines, including waking up at specific times, meals, and social activities. ...
January 2016
Journal of Sleep Research
... Sleep has been defined as a reversible behavioral state, during which the sensory input is reduced, coordinated behavior is abolished, and cognitive activities are suspended (Borbely and Tononi 1998). Importantly, sleep is not only a state but is also a process, as it is manifested in specific patterns of brain activity that unfold in time and space in a highly complex manner. ...
Reference:
Sleep and Synaptic Homeostasis
March 1998
Daedalus