Alejandro Torrado Pacheco’s research while affiliated with Oregon Health & Science University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (5)


Reductions of Grin2a in adolescent dopamine neurons confers aberrant salience and related psychosis phenotype
  • Preprint

October 2024

·

1 Read

Michelle L Kielhold

·

·

Alejandro Torrado Pacheco

·

[...]

·

Bita Moghaddam

Psychosis is a hallmark of schizophrenia. It typically emerges in late adolescence and is associated with dopamine abnormalities and aberrant salience. Most genes associated with schizophrenia risk involve ubiquitous targets that may not explain delayed emergence of dopaminergic disruptions. This includes GRIN2A, the gene encoding the GluN2A subunit of the NMDA receptor. Both common and rare variants of GRIN2A are considered genetic risk factors for schizophrenia diagnosis. We find that Grin2a knockout in dopamine neurons during adolescence is sufficient to produce a behavioral phenotype that mirrors aspects of psychosis. These include disruptions in effort optimization, salience attribution, and ability to utilize feedback to guide behavior. We also find a selective effect of this manipulation on dopamine release during prediction error signaling. These data provide mechanistic insight into how variants of GRIN2A may lead to the latent presentation of aberrant salience and abnormalities in dopamine dynamics. This etiologically relevant model may aid future discovery of course altering treatments for schizophrenia.


Licit use of illicit drugs for treating depression: the pill and the process
  • Article
  • Full-text available

June 2024

·

52 Reads

The Journal of clinical investigation

Download

Experimental methods. (A) Experimental design of the cued response inhibition task (CRIT). Sessions begin with simultaneous presentation of an inhibit action cue (variable time 5–30 s), and an action cue which remained lit for 10 s following cessation of the inhibit action cue. Created with Biorender.com. (B) Schematic of chemogenetic methods. DREADDs receptors were expressed in DMS projecting OFC neurons by injecting CAV/2 Cre (green) bilaterally in the DMS and inhibitory DREADDs receptors (red) in the OFC. (C) Robust expression of both viruses in their respective brain regions is observed. (D) Administration of CNO (10 mg/kg) suppresses firing rate (Z-scored) in a spontaneously active population of OFC neurons (not limited to DREADDs expressing units) under isoflurane anesthesia. (E) Compared to saline control (light grey), CNO injection (dark grey) produced a significant and sustained modulation of OFC firing rate.
Chemogenetic manipulation of corticostriatal circuits. (A) Timeline of CRIT showing animals’ age at each stage for adolescents (top) and adults (bottom). Arrows represent days animals were injected with either saline or CNO. Created with Biorender.com. (B) CNO (10 mg/kg) treatment resulted in increased premature actions in both age groups during later sessions. (C) Number of premature actions is similar between adults and adolescents treated with CNO, but adolescent premature actions differ from adults across sessions following saline treatment. (D) CNO treatment resulted in reduced correct actions in both age groups. (E) Both adolescents and adults increase the number of correct actions across session. Data are presented as mean + SEM. *p < 0.05 main effect.
Simultaneous recording from OFC and DMS in adults and adolescents as they perform CRIT (A) Schematic of the operant chamber where simultaneous recordings in the OFC and DMS were performed. Created with Biorender.com. (B) Baseline firing rate in adolescent (orange) and adult (blue) putative pyramidal cells (C), and medium spiny neurons (MSNs;2). Numbers reflect total number of cells recorded across all sessions. There were no age differences baseline firing rate. (D, E) Behavioral data for both age groups performing CRIT. (D) Adolescents make more premature responses during CRIT. (E) Adolescents and adults made similar number of correct responses. Data are presented as mean + SEM. *p < 0.05, main effect of age.
Single unit electrophysiology recordings in OFC and DMS during cue presentation, premature action and correct actions in adults and adolescents. (A–F) OFC heatmaps for unit firing and corresponding mean firing rate (G–L). DMS heatmaps for unit firing and corresponding mean firing rate. The bars on the heatmaps in the left column depict the 500 ms change in firing rate following the task event (cue on, or action). Age differences in line graphs on the right (Z-score responses) are depicted as adults in blue and adolescents in orange. The panel events (tone on, premature or correct action) occurred at time 0 s. Adults (blue) but not adolescents (orange) show a suppression of OFC cell firing rate after premature responding (A). In the DMS, MSNs show age differences in response to premature responding (B). OFC cell firing rate was not different between adults and adolescents following correct responses (C). Adolescent DMS MSNs show a larger response to reward compared to adults (D). Numbers at the top of y-axis reflect cell counts for each group. Data are presented as mean + SEM. Black significant bars reflect permutation testing between age groups, p < 0.05.
Simultaneous local field potential recordings in OFC and DMS in adults and adolescents during CRIT performance. (A) Schematic of LFP methods for determining age differences in LFP measures. Theta power, aperiodic exponent (power spectral density slope) and theta phase synchrony during task performance in the OFC and DMS were isolated from the power spectrum density in adults and adolescents (please see methods for detail). (B, C) Compared to baseline, OFC theta power is reduced following correct actions. Adolescents exhibit greater DMS theta power than adults at baseline and following premature actions. (D, E) The aperiodic exponent in OFC and DMS. The exponent was larger in adults than adolescents in the OFC during premature actions. In the DMS, the aperiodic exponent was increased following correct actions and was larger in adults compared to adolescents for all events. (F, H) Schematic and results of phase synchrony between the OFC and DMS. Phase synchrony between the OFC and DMS was stronger in adolescents compared to adults at baseline and during premature actions (G). Synchrony during premature actions in both age groups was also correlated with the total number of premature responses (H). Data are presented as mean + SEM. #p < 0.05, Tukey Post hoc, *p < 0.05 Bonferroni post hoc, p < 0.05, main effect of age, $p < 0.05, Welch’s two sample t-test.
Adolescent rats engage the orbitofrontal-striatal pathway differently than adults during impulsive actions

April 2024

·

39 Reads

Adolescence is characterized by increased impulsive and risk-taking behaviors. To better understand the neural networks that subserves impulsivity in adolescents, we used a reward-guided behavioral model that quantifies age differences in impulsive actions in adult and adolescent rats of both sexes. Using chemogenetics, we identified orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) projections to the dorsomedial striatum (DMS) as a critical pathway for age-related execution of impulsive actions. Simultaneous recording of single units and local field potentials in the OFC and DMS during task performance revealed an overall muted response in adolescents during impulsive actions as well as age-specific differences in theta power and OFC–DMS functional connectivity. Collectively, these data reveal that the OFC–DMS pathway is critical for age-differences in reward-guided impulsive actions and provide a network mechanism to enhance our understanding of how adolescent and adult brains coordinate behavioral inhibition.


Fig. 2 Psilocybin improves performance in the set-shifting task. a Average trials-to-criterion across all sets in the baseline period (PRE, open gray circles) and in the acute psilocybin condition (PSI, blue circles, n = 15). ***p = 0.0005, paired t-test. b Number of completed sets in baseline and psilocybin conditions, p = 0.546, Wilcoxon sign-rank test. c Response time in baseline and psilocybin conditions. **p = 0.008, Wilcoxon sign-rank test. d Fraction of correct responses across all sets in baseline and psilocybin conditions. e Number of streaks of consecutive correct responses averaged across all sets, in baseline and psilocybin conditions. *p = 0.025, Wilcoxon sign-rank test. f Average length of streaks averaged across all sets, in baseline and psilocybin conditions. *p = 0.034, paired ttest. g Average trials-to-criterion for baseline (PRE, gray open circles) and saline conditions (SAL, brown circles, n = 14). h Number of completed sets in baseline and saline conditions. i Fraction of correct responses across all sets in baseline and saline conditions.
Fig. 5 Pre-treatment with the 5HT2A antagonist ketanserin blocks psilocybin's effect on cognitive flexibility. a Average trials-to-criterion for baseline (PRE, gray open circles) and ketanserin-psilocybin condition (KET + PSI, orange circles, n = 14). b Number of completed sets in baseline and ketanserin-psilocybin conditions. c Fraction of correct responses averaged across all sets in baseline and ketanserin-psilocybin conditions. d Average trials-to-criterion for baseline (PRE, gray open circles) and ketanserin-saline conditions (KET + SAL, pink circles, n = 11). *p = 0.026, paired t-test. e Number of completed sets in baseline and ketanserin-saline conditions. f Fraction of correct responses averaged across all sets in baseline and ketanserin-saline conditions. *p = 0.05, paired t-test. g Average trials-to-criterion for baseline (PRE, gray open circles) and SB242084-psilocybin conditions (SB + PSI, green circles, n = 11). *p = 0.032, Wilcoxon sign-rank test. h Number of completed sets in baseline and SB242084-psilocybin conditions. i Fraction of correct responses averaged across all sets in baseline and SB242084-psilocybin conditions. j Average trials-to-criterion for baseline (PRE, gray open circles) and SB242084-saline conditions (SB + SAL, teal circles, n = 11). k Number of completed sets in baseline and SB242084-saline conditions. l Fraction of correct responses averaged across all sets in baseline and SB242084-saline conditions. *p = 0.031, paired t-test.
Acute psilocybin enhances cognitive flexibility in rats

February 2023

·

149 Reads

·

32 Citations

Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology

Psilocybin has been shown to improve symptoms of depression and anxiety when combined with psychotherapy or other clinician-guided interventions. To understand the neural basis for this pattern of clinical efficacy, experimental and conceptual approaches that are different than traditional laboratory models of anxiety and depression are needed. A potential novel mechanism is that acute psilocybin improves cognitive flexibility, which then enhances the impact of clinician-assisted interventions. Consistent with this idea, we find that acute psilocybin robustly improves cognitive flexibility in male and female rats using a task where animals switched between previously learned strategies in response to uncued changes in the environment. Psilocybin did not influence Pavlovian reversal learning, suggesting that its cognitive effects are selective to enhanced switching between previously learned behavioral strategies. The serotonin (5HT) 2 A receptor antagonist ketanserin blocked psilocybin’s effect on set-shifting, while a 5HT2C-selective antagonist did not. Ketanserin alone also improved set-shifting performance, suggesting a complex relationship between psilocybin’s pharmacology and its impact on flexibility. Further, the psychedelic drug 2,5-Dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine (DOI) impaired cognitive flexibility in the same task, suggesting that this effect of psilocybin does not generalize to all other serotonergic psychedelics. We conclude that the acute impact of psilocybin on cognitive flexibility provides a useful behavioral model to investigate its neuronal effects relevant to its positive clinical outcome.


Acute psilocybin enhances cognitive flexibility in rats

January 2023

·

75 Reads

·

6 Citations

Psilocybin has been shown to improve symptoms of depression and anxiety when combined with psychotherapy or other clinician-guided interventions. To understand the neural basis for this pattern of clinical efficacy, experimental and conceptual approaches that are different than traditional laboratory models of anxiety and depression are needed. A potential novel mechanism is that acute psilocybin improves cognitive flexibility, which then enhances the impact of clinician-assisted interventions. Consistent with this idea, we find that acute psilocybin robustly improves cognitive flexibility in male and female rats using a task where animals switched between previously learned strategies in response to uncued changes in the environment. Psilocybin did not influence Pavlovian reversal learning, suggesting that its cognitive effects are selective to enhanced switching between previously learned behavioral strategies. The serotonin (5HT) 2A receptor antagonist ketanserin blocked psilocybin's effect on set-shifting, while a 5HT2C-selective antagonist did not. Ketanserin alone also improved set-shifting performance, suggesting a complex relationship between psilocybin's pharmacology and its impact on flexibility. Further, the psychedelic drug 2,5-Dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine (DOI) impaired cognitive flexibility in the same task, suggesting that this effect of psilocybin does not generalize to all other serotonergic psychedelics. We conclude that the acute impact of psilocybin on cognitive flexibility provides a useful behavioral model to investigate its neuronal effects relevant to its positive clinical outcome.

Citations (2)


... Previous studies on psilocybin and decision-making indicate that psilocybin may improve cognitive flexibility [72][73][74]; a further study found that psilocybin may reduce punishment by changing one's concerns for the outcome of their game partner in a social decision-making task [73]. The current findings add to a growing literature in this area highlighting a range of effects that emerge in different task contexts, and which perhaps have similar underlying mechanisms. ...

Reference:

Psilocybin increases optimistic engagement over time: computational modelling of behaviour in rats
Acute psilocybin enhances cognitive flexibility in rats

Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology

... Classic psychedelic Serotonin 5-HT 2A, 5-HT 1A and 5-HT 2C receptor agonist Altered perception of time, space, and reality 31,42 ; changes in mood and affect; spiritual or mystical experiences; 48,49 enhanced empathy 50 ; increased cognitive flexibility 51,52 Increased blood pressure and heart rate, transient anxiety, panic or paranoia, headache Depression, anxiety, substance use disorders ...

Acute psilocybin enhances cognitive flexibility in rats