Adelyn Brecher’s research while affiliated with Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (21)


Learning from errors: Exploration of the monitoring learning effect
  • Article

July 2022

·

98 Reads

·

7 Citations

Cognition

·

·

Gary S. Dell

·

Adelyn Brecher

The present study examined spontaneous detection and repair of naming errors in people with aphasia to advance a theoretical understanding of how monitoring impacts learning in lexical access. Prior work in aphasia has found that spontaneous repair, but not mere detection without repair, of semantic naming errors leads to improved naming on those same items in the future when other factors are accounted for. The present study sought to replicate this finding in a new, larger sample of participants and to examine the critical role of self-generated repair in this monitoring learning effect. Twenty-four participants with chronic aphasia with naming impairment provided naming responses to a 660-item corpus of common, everyday objects at two timepoints. At the first timepoint, a randomly selected subset of trials ended in experimenter-provided corrective feedback. Each naming trial was coded for accuracy, error type, and for any monitoring behavior that occurred, specifically detection with repair (i.e., correction), detection without repair, and no detection. Focusing on semantic errors, the original monitoring learning effect was replicated, with enhanced accuracy at a future timepoint when the first trial with that item involved detection with repair, compared to error trials that were not detected. This enhanced accuracy resulted from learning that arose from the first trial rather than the presence of repair simply signifying easier items. A second analysis compared learning from trials of self-corrected errors to that of trials ending in feedback that were detected but not self-corrected and found enhanced learning after self-generated repair. Implications for theories of lexical access and monitoring are discussed.


Mapping articulatory and grammatical subcomponents of fluency deficits in post-stroke aphasia

June 2019

·

90 Reads

·

26 Citations

Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience

·

·

Denise Y. Harvey

·

[...]

·

Fluent speech production is a critical aspect of language processing and is central to aphasia diagnosis and treatment. Multiple cognitive processes and neural subsystems must be coordinated to produce fluent narrative speech. To refine the understanding of these systems, measures that minimize the influence of other cognitive processes were defined for articulatory deficits and grammatical deficits. Articulatory deficits were measured by the proportion of phonetic errors (articulatory and prosodic) in a word repetition task in 115 participants with aphasia following left hemisphere stroke. Grammatical deficits were assessed in 46 participants based on two measures—proportion of closed class words and proportion of words in sentences—generated during semistructured narrative speech production (telling the Cinderella story). These measures were used to identify brain regions critical for articulatory and grammatical aspects of speech production using a multivariate lesion-symptom mapping approach based on support vector regression. Phonetic error proportion was associated with damage to the postcentral gyrus and the inferior parietal lobule (particularly the supramarginal gyrus). Proportion of closed class words in narrative speech did not have consistent lesion correlates. Proportion of words in sentences was strongly associated with frontal lobe damage, particularly the inferior and middle frontal gyri. Grammatical sentence structuring relies on frontal regions, particularly the inferior and middle frontal gyri, whereas phonetic-articulatory planning and execution relies on parietal regions, particularly the postcentral and supramarginal gyri. These results clarify and extend current understanding of the functional components of the frontoparietal speech production system.


Learning from Errors: Further Exploration of the Monitoring Learning Effect

January 2018

·

51 Reads


Does Naming Accuracy Improve Through Self-Monitoring of Errors?

February 2016

·

181 Reads

·

21 Citations

Neuropsychologia

This study examined spontaneous self-monitoring of picture naming in people with aphasia. Of primary interest was whether spontaneous detection or repair of an error constitutes an error signal or other feedback that tunes the production system to the desired outcome. In other words, do acts of monitoring cause adaptive change in the language system? A second possibility, not incompatible with the first, is that monitoring is indicative of an item’s representational strength, and strength is a causal factor in language change. Twelve PWA performed a 615-item naming test twice, in separate sessions, without extrinsic feedback. At each timepoint, we scored the first complete response for accuracy and error type and the remainder of the trial for verbalizations consistent with detection (e.g., “no, not that”) and successful repair (i.e., correction). Data analysis centered on: (a) how often an item that was misnamed at one timepoint changed to correct at the other timepoint, as a function of monitoring; and (b) how monitoring impacted change scores in the Forward (Time 1 to Time 2) compared to Backward (Time 2 to Time 1) direction. The Strength hypothesis predicts significant effects of monitoring in both directions. The Learning hypothesis predicts greater effects in the Forward direction. These predictions were evaluated for three types of errors -- Semantic errors, Phonological errors, and Fragments – using mixed-effects regression modeling with crossed random effects. Support for the Strength hypothesis was found for all three error types. Support for the Learning hypothesis was found for Semantic errors. All effects were due to error repair, not error detection. We discuss the theoretical and clinical implications of these novel findings.


A Large, Searchable, Web-based Database of Aphasic Performance on Picture Naming and Other Tests of Cognitive Function
  • Article
  • Full-text available

June 2011

·

296 Reads

·

92 Citations

Cognitive Neuropsychology

Many research questions in aphasia can only be answered through access to substantial numbers of patients and to their responses on individual test items. Since such data are often unavailable to individual researchers and institutions, we have developed and made available the Moss Aphasia Psycholinguistics Project Database: a large, searchable, web-based database of patient performance on psycholinguistic and neuropsychological tests. The database contains data from over 240 patients covering a wide range of aphasia subtypes and severity, some of whom were tested multiple times. The core of the archive consists of a detailed record of individual-trial performance on the Philadelphia (picture) Naming Test. The database also contains basic demographic information about the patients and patients' overall performance on neuropsychological assessments as well as tests of speech perception, semantics, short-term memory, and sentence comprehension. The database is available at http://www.mappd.org/ .

Download

Fig. 1. Maps of the reliability ( t statistic) of the difference in ResidTaxon between patients with and without lesions in each voxel. Voxels exceeding the false discovery rate threshold ( q = 0.02) are rendered in a red ( t = 3.58) to yellow ( t = 5.44) scale, whereas nonsigni fi cant values are rendered on a scale from green ( t just below threshold) to blue ( t ≤ 0). Statistical maps are superimposed on the MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) space Colin27 template. In all panels, a large cluster of signi fi cant voxels is visible in the anterior temporal lobe. A – D show sagittal slices at x = − 60, x = − 56, x = − 52, and x = − 48, respectively. The region of peak effect is marked with crosshairs. E shows an axial slice at z = − 31, selected to intersect the peak region. 
Fig. S1. Maps depicting lesion overlap of the 86 patients (left hemisphere stroke) on the MNI space Colin27 template. Regions of high overlap (bright yellow), maximal at the crosshairs, are in the peri-Sylvian region. A – D show sagittal slices at x = − 60, x = − 56, x = − 52, and x = − 48, respectively. E shows a single axial slice at z = 25. Only voxels lesioned in at least fi ve subjects were included, yielding 504,228 total voxels to be tested. 
Fig. S2. Maps of the reliability ( t statistic) of the difference in ResidNonNoun between patients with and without lesions in each voxel. Voxels exceeding the false discovery rate threshold ( q = 0.02) are rendered in a red ( t = 3.03) to yellow ( t = 6.66) scale, whereas nonsigni fi cant values are rendered on a scale from green ( t just below threshold) to blue ( t ≤ 0). Statistical maps are superimposed on the MNI space Colin27 template. In all panels, a large cluster of signi fi cant voxels is visible in the anterior temporal lobe, while very few signi fi cant voxels appear in the temporoparietal junction. A – D show sagittal slices at x = − 60, x = − 56, x = − 52, and x = − 48, respectively. E shows an axial slice at z = − 21, selected to intersect the peak region. 
Neuroanatomical dissociation for taxonomic and thematic knowledge in the human brain

May 2011

·

241 Reads

·

251 Citations

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

It is thought that semantic memory represents taxonomic information differently from thematic information. This study investigated the neural basis for the taxonomic-thematic distinction in a unique way. We gathered picture-naming errors from 86 individuals with poststroke language impairment (aphasia). Error rates were determined separately for taxonomic errors ("pear" in response to apple) and thematic errors ("worm" in response to apple), and their shared variance was regressed out of each measure. With the segmented lesions normalized to a common template, we carried out voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping on each error type separately. We found that taxonomic errors localized to the left anterior temporal lobe and thematic errors localized to the left temporoparietal junction. This is an indication that the contribution of these regions to semantic memory cleaves along taxonomic-thematic lines. Our findings show that a distinction long recognized in the psychological sciences is grounded in the structure and function of the human brain.



Support for Anterior Temporal Involvement in Semantic Error Production in Aphasia: New Evidence from VLSM

October 2010

·

142 Reads

·

110 Citations

Brain and Language

Semantic errors in aphasia (e.g., naming a horse as "dog") frequently arise from faulty mapping of concepts onto lexical items. A recent study by our group used voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM) methods with 64 patients with chronic aphasia to identify voxels that carry an association with semantic errors. The strongest associations were found in the left anterior temporal lobe (L-ATL), in the mid- to anterior MTG region. The absence of findings in Wernicke's area was surprising, as were indications that ATL voxels made an essential contribution to the post-semantic stage of lexical access. In this follow-up study, we sought to validate these results by re-defining semantic errors in a manner that was less theory dependent and more consistent with prior lesion studies. As this change also increased the robustness of the dependent variable, it made it possible to perform additional statistical analyses that further refined the interpretation. The results strengthen the evidence for a causal relationship between ATL damage and lexically-based semantic errors in naming and lend confidence to the conclusion that chronic lesions in Wernicke's area are not causally implicated in semantic error production.


Anterior temporal involvement in semantic word retrieval: Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping evidence from aphasia

November 2009

·

195 Reads

·

334 Citations

Brain

Analysis of error types provides useful information about the stages and processes involved in normal and aphasic word production. In picture naming, semantic errors (horse for goat) generally result from something having gone awry in lexical access such that the right concept was mapped to the wrong word. This study used the new lesion analysis technique known as voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping to investigate the locus of lesions that give rise to semantic naming errors. Semantic errors were obtained from 64 individuals with post-stroke aphasia, who also underwent high-resolution structural brain scans. Whole brain voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping was carried out to determine where lesion status predicted semantic error rate. The strongest associations were found in the left anterior to mid middle temporal gyrus. This area also showed strong and significant effects in further analyses that statistically controlled for deficits in pre-lexical, conceptualization processes that might have contributed to semantic error production. This study is the first to demonstrate a specific and necessary role for the left anterior temporal lobe in mapping concepts to words in production. We hypothesize that this role consists in the conveyance of fine-grained semantic distinctions to the lexical system. Our results line up with evidence from semantic dementia, the convergence zone framework and meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies on word production. At the same time, they cast doubt on the classical linkage of semantic error production to lesions in and around Wernicke's area.



Citations (18)


... Thereafter, the adaption stage at the specific high air temperature began, during which subjects could practice these tasks on the computer in the waiting room to eliminate the effects of the 'learning effect' [52]. In the 150-min formal experiment, subjects completed these five tasks in approximately 20 min, and then they would finish the scales in 5 min. ...

Reference:

Cognitive Performance in Hot-Humid Environments of Non-Air-Conditioned Buildings: A Subjective Evaluation
Learning from errors: Exploration of the monitoring learning effect
  • Citing Article
  • July 2022

Cognition

... In the set of reviewed studies (Ash et al., , 2015Borovsky et al., 2007;DeLeon et al., 2012;Grossman et al., 2013;Matchin et al., 2020Matchin et al., , 2022Mirman et al., 2019;den Ouden et al., 2019;Wilson et al., 2010a;Henseler et al., 2014;Matchin et al., 2021;Rogalski et al., 2011;Sapolsky et al., 2010), morphosyntactic impairment correlated with damage to left prefrontal, parietal and, less consistently, temporal regions. In the meta-analysis including all the 16 studies that considered morphosyntactic processes (either exclusively or in association with thematic role assignment), the inferior frontal gyrus and the middle frontal gyrus were involved more extensively than temporal and parietal regions. ...

Mapping articulatory and grammatical subcomponents of fluency deficits in post-stroke aphasia
  • Citing Article
  • June 2019

Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience

... Es ist zwar zu vermuten, dass sich in den Ergebnissen auch zum Teil die Eff ekte des gesamten 7-w ö chigen Aufenthalts auf der Aachener Aphasiestation widerspiegeln, die neben dem Benenntraining aus der t ä glichen Intensivtherapie, Lehrbehandlungen, Gruppentherapien und weiteren Angeboten bestanden. Auch das allgemein sprachf ö rdernde Milieu der Aphasiestation kann sprachanregend gewirkt haben [18] . Diese Konfundierung wird sich aber vermutlich eher in der Verringerung der Omissionen manifestiert haben als in der Beeinfl ussung lexikalischer Parameter. ...

Variation in naming response profiles attributable to severity and recovery: In search of an explanation
  • Citing Article
  • October 1999

Brain and Language

... Participants had the opportunity to detect and correct their errors, but they were not explicitly directed to judge the accuracy of their response on each trial. Much of the prior research on error monitoring, particularly research on the relationship between error monitoring and clinical outcomes, uses this approach van der Stelt et al., 2021;Mandal et al., 2020;Schwartz, Middleton, Brecher, Gagliardi, & Garvey, 2016;Nozari et al., 2011;Marshall, Neuburger, & Phillips, 1994). In Experiment 1, detections were scored by trained researchers, and afterward, detection scores were verified by a separate researcher to ensure consistency with the scoring protocol. ...

Does Naming Accuracy Improve Through Self-Monitoring of Errors?
  • Citing Article
  • February 2016

Neuropsychologia

... These semantic interference effects have been shown to be increased in patients with aphasia, and this particularly for patients with prefrontal lesions (Biegler et al., 2008;Damian et al., 2001;Jefferies et al., 2007;Schnur et al., 2006Schnur et al., , 2009Thompson et al., 2017). While phonological variants of this paradigm (the pictures to be named refer to phonologically similar names) have also been developed and shown to lead to increased interference effects in patients with aphasia (Hodgson et al., 2005), there are no direct comparisons so far between phonological and semantic interference build-up conditions of this task. ...

Facilitation and interference in phonological blocked-cyclic naming

Brain and Language

... In aphasic speakers, the BCNP served to investigate the neuroanatomical correlates of word production and the underlying mechanisms of anomia. Schnur and colleagues tested eighteen individuals with the BCNP (Schnur, 2004;Schnur et al., 2006Schnur et al., , 2009Schnur et al., , 2005 and found semantic interference on naming accuracy especially at later cycles in participants with Broca aphasia and on RTs in participants with other types of aphasia (NonBroca). Semantic interference effects on accuracy were larger in participants with a temporal (vs frontal) lesion, but participants with a damaged left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) showed a linear increase of errors (independently of the overall magnitude of semantic interference) over the cycles. ...

Errors of lexical selection during high and low semantic competition
  • Citing Article
  • October 2004

Brain and Language

... Moreover, some studies used an innovative computer-based approach to apply semantic treatment in patients with svPPA. Among these, Jokel et al. [95] investigated the effectiveness of a computer-based treatment for anomia based on the principles of errorless learning in one svPPA patient to relearn lost words (MossTalk Words) [114][115][116]. The authors reported improvements in oral naming for trained and untrained items after treatment, with gains for trained items maintained up to 12 weeks. ...

Computer-assisted treatment of word retrieval deficits in aphasia
  • Citing Article
  • November 2005

... Most studies targeted nouns, although some studies included verbs (n = 9) [33][34][35][36][37][38][39][40][41], proper nouns (n = 3) [14,42,43], or adjectives (n = 1) [41]. Words were most commonly selected from vocabulary databases (n = 15) [34][35][36][37]40,[43][44][45][46][47][48][49][50][51][52], although some studies focused on personally relevant words, chosen by the PWA based on their interests or communication needs (n = 9) [12,14,33,38,39,41,42,53,54]. The therapies were delivered through a variety of programs or software (specialized or non-specialized). ...

A computer-implemented protocol for treatment of naming disorders: Evaluation of clinician-guided and partially self-guided instruction
  • Citing Article
  • October 2002

... This differential pattern was interpreted as increased difficulty in overcoming lexical competition in the LPFC group, due to a cognitive control mechanism relying on the LPFC. Several case studies using the BCNP also reported semantic interference in aphasic individuals with frontal or unspecified lesions in terms of naming accuracy (McCarthy and Kartsounis, 2000;Wilshire and McCarthy, 2002;Hodgson et al., 2003;Schwartz and Hodgson, 2002;Scott and Wilshire, 2010) or naming latencies (Biegler et al., 2008). These case reports generally merged the participant's results over the cycles, even if interference materialized only in later cycles. ...

Effects of relatedness, repetition, and rate: Further investigations of context-sensitive naming
  • Citing Article
  • October 2003

Brain and Language

... The occurrence of non-naming responses and fragments (omissions in terms of Foygel & Dell, 2000) as well as self-corrections previously were considered to indicate language monitoring (Schwartz & Brecher, 2000). Moreover, executive processing can be taken to be involved in the inhibition of automated speech (Martin et al., 1998), just as it dissolves interference in naming (e.g., inhibits the production of the distractor word instead of the target name). Automated speech (automatisms, perseverations) and unintended distractor repetition therefore are subsumed under the term "formulaic speech" here. ...

Lexical retrieval mechanism underlying whole-word perseveration errors in anomic aphasia
  • Citing Article
  • April 1998