ArticlePDF Available

Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI Study of Social Exclusion

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

A neuroimaging study examined the neural correlates of social exclusion and tested the hypothesis that the brain bases of social pain are similar to those of physical pain. Participants were scanned while playing a virtual ball-tossing game in which they were ultimately excluded. Paralleling results from physical pain studies, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) was more active during exclusion than during inclusion and correlated positively with self-reported distress. Right ventral prefrontal cortex (RVPFC) was active during exclusion and correlated negatively with self-reported distress. ACC changes mediated the RVPFC-distress correlation, suggesting that RVPFC regulates the distress of social exclusion by disrupting ACC activity.
Content may be subject to copyright.
DOI: 10.1126/science.1089134
, 290 (2003); 302Science et al.Naomi I. Eisenberger,
Exclusion
Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI Study of Social
www.sciencemag.org (this information is current as of September 30, 2008 ):
The following resources related to this article are available online at
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/302/5643/290
version of this article at: including high-resolution figures, can be found in the onlineUpdated information and services,
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/302/5643/290/DC1
can be found at: Supporting Online Material
found at: can berelated to this articleA list of selected additional articles on the Science Web sites
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/302/5643/290#related-content
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/302/5643/290#otherarticles
, 7 of which can be accessed for free: cites 17 articlesThis article
220 article(s) on the ISI Web of Science. cited byThis article has been
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/302/5643/290#otherarticles
37 articles hosted by HighWire Press; see: cited byThis article has been
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/neuroscience
Neuroscience : subject collectionsThis article appears in the following
http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl
in whole or in part can be found at: this article permission to reproduce of this article or about obtaining reprintsInformation about obtaining
registered trademark of AAAS. is aScience2003 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title CopyrightAmerican Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005.
(print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by theScience
on September 30, 2008 www.sciencemag.orgDownloaded from
subtilis (fig. S2). The RLP of B. subtilis
includes both those amino acid residues of
RuBisCO that are responsible for binding
the phosphate on C1 of RuBP and those
required for activation by CO
2
. However,
the residues of RuBisCO that are responsi-
ble for binding the other phosphate group
of RuBP and the residues of loop 6, which
are essential for RuBisCO activity (2,3),
are replaced by different amino acids in
RLP (Fig. 1B). The reaction catalyzed by
RuBisCO consists of three sequential, par-
tial reactions: enolization, carboxylation or
oxygenation, and hydrolysis (2,3,26). De-
letion of loop 6 from RuBisCO prevents it
from catalyzing the carboxylation/oxygen-
ation reactions (27). However, it retains the
ability to catalyze the enolization reaction
(27). This observation supports the hypoth-
esis that the RLP-catalyzed enolization of
DK-MTP-1-P does not require the amino
acid residues that bind the phosphate group
on C5 of RuBP and the loop 6. Moreover,
the structure of DK-MTP-1-P is very sim-
ilar to that of RuBP. In photosynthetic
RuBisCO, these additional structures may
hinder the DK-MTP-1-P enolase reaction,
and they may also explain the slow growth
of ykrW
/rbcL
cells (Fig. 4C). In this
context, our results with the RLP of B.
subtilis suggest that RLPs of other bacteria
may also catalyze a reaction similar to one
of the partial reactions of RuBisCO in a
bacterial metabolic pathway.
Our analysis shows that RLP of B. sub-
tilis functions as a DK-MTP-1-P enolase,
which has no RuBP-carboxylation activity,
in the methionine salvage pathway. More-
over, this function of RLP is conserved in
the RuBisCO from a photosynthetic bacte-
rium. In a standard phylogenetic tree of the
large subunits of RuBisCO, the RLP from
B. subtilis is not included on any branches
that include RuBisCO or on branches that
include other RLPs with RuBP-carboxyla-
tion activity (Fig. 1A). The codon usage
and the G C content of the gene for RLP
are typical of the organism. The literature
(28) suggests that genes such as the gene
for RLP were probably not derived by lat-
eral transfer of a gene for a RuBP-carbox-
ylating enzyme from another unrelated or-
ganism, for example, in this case, an ar-
chaeon or photosynthetic bacterium. Thus,
it is possible that the gene for RLP, which
in B. subtilis is part of the methionine
salvage pathway, and the gene for photo-
synthetic RuBisCO originated from a com-
mon ancestral gene (supporting online
text). However, bacteria and Archaea that
have RLPs first appeared on Earth (29)
long before the Calvin cycle developed in
photosynthetic bacteria (30), thus we sug-
gest that RLPs may be the ancestral en-
zymes of photosynthetic RuBisCO.
References and Notes
1. R. J. Ellis, Trend. Biochem. Sci. 4, 241 (1979).
2. T. J. Andrews, G. H. Lorimer, in Biochemistry of Plants,
vol. 10, M. D. Hatch, N. K. Boardman, Eds. (Academic
Press, New York, 1987), pp. 131–218.
3. H. Roy, T. J. Andrews, in Photosynthesis, vol. 9, R. C.
Leegood, T. D. Sharkey, S. von Caemmerer, Eds. (Klu-
wer, Dordrecht, Netherlands, 2000), pp. 53–83.
4. G. M. Watson, F. R. Tabita, FEMS Microbiol. Lett.146,
13 (1997).
5. G. M. Watson, J. Yu, F. R. Tabita, J. Bacteriol.181,
1569 (1999).
6. S. Ezaki, N. Maeda, T. Kishimoto, H. Atomi, T.
Imanaka, J. Biol. Chem.274, 5078 (1999).
7. F. Kunst et al., Nature 390, 249 (1997).
8. T. E. Hanson, F. R. Tabita, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
98, 4397 (2001).
9. J. A. Eisen et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.U.S.A.99, 9509
(2002).
10. H. P. Klenk et al., Nature 390, 364 (1997).
11. J. F. Grundy, M. T. Henkin, in Bacillus subtilis and Its
Closest Relatives: from Genes to Cells, A. L. Sonen-
shein et al., Eds. (ASM Press, Washington, DC, 2002),
pp. 245–254.
12. B. A. Murphy, F. J. Grundy, T. M. Henkin, J. Bacteriol.
184, 2314 (2002).
13. A. Sekowska, A. Danchin, BMC Microbiol.2, 8 (2002).
14. E. S. Furfine, R. H. Abeles, J. Biol. Chem. 263, 9598
(1988).
15. R. W. Myers, J. W. Wray, S. Fish, R. H. Abeles, J. Biol.
Chem.268, 24785 (1993).
16. J. W. Wray, R. H. Abeles, J. Biol. Chem. 270, 3147
(1995).
17. Y. Dai, T. C. Pochapsky, R. H. Abeles, Biochemistry 40,
6379 (2001).
18. J. Heilbronn, J. Wilson, B. J. Berger, J. Bacteriol. 181,
1739 (1999).
19. Materials and Methods are available as supporting
material on Science Online.
20. A. Sekowska, L. Mulard, S. Krogh, J. K. Tse, A. Danchin,
BMC Microbiol.1, 15 (2001).
21. G. Avigad, Methods Enzymol.41, 27 (1975).
22. N. C. Kyrpides, C. R. Woese, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
U.S.A.95, 224 (1998).
23. Y. R. Chen, F. C. Hartman, J. Biol. Chem.270, 11741
(1995).
24. H. Ashida et al., unpublished observations.
25. V. Vagner, E. Dervyn, S. D. Ehrlich, Microbiology 144,
3097 (1998).
26. W. W. Cleland, T. J. Andrews, S. Gutteridge, F.C.
Hartman, G. H. Lorimer, Chem. Rev. 98, 549 (1998).
27. E. M. Larson, F. W. Larimer, F. C. Hartman, Biochem-
istry 34, 4531 (1995).
28. I. Moszer, E. P. Rogha, A. Danchin, Curr. Opin. Micro-
biol.2, 524 (1999).
29. C. R. Woese, O. Kandler, M. L. Wheelis, Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 87, 4576 (1990).
30. H. Hartman, Origins Life Evol. Biosphere 28, 515
(1998).
31. We thank W. L. Ogren and A. R. Portis Jr., for review-
ing the manuscript. We also thank M. Inui, RITE, for
providing the plasmid pRR2119, and J. Tsukamoto for
assistance with mass analysis. This study was sup-
ported by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (no.
10460043) from the Ministry of Education, Science,
Sports and Culture of Japan, and by the “Research for
the Future” programs (JSPS-RFTF97R16001 and
JSPS-00L01604) of the Japan Society for the Promo-
tion of Science.
Supporting Online Material
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/302/5643/286/
DC1
Materials and Methods
SOM Text
Figs. S1 and S2
References
19 May 2003; accepted 26 August 2003
Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI
Study of Social Exclusion
Naomi I. Eisenberger,
1
* Matthew D. Lieberman,
1
Kipling D. Williams
2
A neuroimaging study examined the neural correlates of social exclusion and
tested the hypothesis that the brain bases of social pain are similar to those
of physical pain. Participants were scanned while playing a virtual ball-
tossing game in which they were ultimately excluded. Paralleling results
from physical pain studies, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) was more
active during exclusion than during inclusion and correlated positively with
self-reported distress. Right ventral prefrontal cortex (RVPFC) was active
during exclusion and correlated negatively with self-reported distress.
ACC changes mediated the RVPFC-distress correlation, suggesting that
RVPFC regulates the distress of social exclusion by disrupting ACC
activity.
It is a basic feature of human experience to
feel soothed in the presence of close others
and to feel distressed when left behind.
Many languages reflect this experience in
the assignment of physical pain words
(“hurt feelings”) to describe experiences of
social separation (1). However, the notion
that the pain associated with losing some-
one is similar to the pain experienced upon
physical injury seems more metaphorical
than real. Nonetheless, evidence suggests
that some of the same neural machinery
recruited in the experience of physical pain
may also be involved in the experience of
pain associated with social separation or
1
Department of Psychology, Franz Hall, University of
California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095–1563,
USA.
2
Department of Psychology, Macquarie Univer-
sity, Sydney NSW 2109, Australia.
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-
mail: neisenbe@ucla.edu
REPORTS
10 OCTOBER 2003 VOL 302 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org290
on September 30, 2008 www.sciencemag.orgDownloaded from
rejection (2). Because of the adaptive value
of mammalian social bonds, the social at-
tachment system, which keeps young near
caregivers, may have piggybacked onto the
physical pain system to promote survival
(3). We conducted a functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) study of social
exclusion to determine whether the regions
activated by social pain are similar to those
found in studies of physical pain.
The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is
believed to act as a neural alarm system
or conflict monitor, detecting when an au-
tomatic response is inappropriate or in con-
flict with current goals (46). Not surpris-
ingly, pain, the most primitive signal that
something is wrong,activates the ACC
(7,8). More specifically, dorsal ACC ac-
tivity is primarily associated with the affec-
tively distressing rather than the sensory
component of pain (79).
Because of the importance of social
bonds for the survival of most mammalian
species, the social attachment system may
have adopted the neural computations of
the ACC, involved in pain and conflict
detection processes, to promote the goal of
social connectedness. Ablating the cingu-
late in hamster mothers disrupts maternal
behavior aimed at keeping pups near (10),
and ablating the cingulate in squirrel mon-
keys eliminates the spontaneous production
of the separation cry, emitted to reestablish
contact with the social group (11). In hu-
man mothers, the ACC is activated by the
sound of infant cries (12). However, to
date, no studies have examined whether the
ACC is also activated upon social separa-
tion or social rejection in human subjects.
Right ventral prefrontal cortex (RVPFC)
has been implicated in the regulation or
inhibition of pain distress and negative af-
fect (1316). The primate homolog of
VPFC has efferent connections to the re-
gion of the ACC associated with pain dis-
tress (17,18), suggesting that RVPFC may
partially regulate the ACC. Additionally,
electrical stimulation of VPFC in rats di-
minishes pain behavior in response to pain-
ful stimulation (19). More recently in hu-
mans, heightened RVPFC activation has
been associated with improvement of pain
symptoms in a placebo-pain study (16).
Given that even the mildest forms of
social exclusion can generate social pain
(20), we investigated the neural response
during two types of social exclusion: (i)
explicit social exclusion (ESE), in which
individuals were prevented from participat-
ing in a social activity by other players
engaged in the social activity, and (ii) im-
plicit social exclusion (ISE), in which par-
ticipants, because of extenuating circum-
stances, were not able to join in a social
activity with other players.
fMRI scans were acquired while partic-
ipants played a virtual ball-tossing game
(CyberBall) with what they believed to
be two other players, also in fMRI scan-
ners, during which the players eventually
excluded the participant (21). In reality,
there were no other players; participants
were playing with a preset computer pro-
gram and were given a cover story to en-
sure that they believed the other players
were real (22).
In the first scan (ISE), the participant
watched the other playersplay Cyber-
Ball. Participants were told that, because of
technical difficulties, the link to the other two
scanners could not yet be made and thus, at
first, they would be able to watch but not play
with the other two players. This cover story was
intended to allow participants to view a
scene visually identical to ESE without par-
ticipants believing they were being exclud-
ed. In the second scan (inclusion), partici-
pants played with the other two players. In
the final scan (ESE), participants received
seven throws and were then excluded when
the two players stopped throwing partici-
pants the ball for the remainder of the scan
(45 throws). Afterward, participants
filled out questionnaires assessing how ex-
cluded they felt and their level of social
distress during the ESE scan (22).
Behavioral results indicated that partic-
ipants felt ignored and excluded during
ESE (t5.33, P0.05). As predicted,
group analysis of the fMRI data indicated
that dorsal ACC (Fig. 1A) (x8, y20,
z40) was more active during ESE than
during inclusion (t3.36, r0.71, P
0.005) (23,24). Self-reported distress was
positively correlated with ACC activity in
this contrast (Fig. 2A) (x6, y8, z
45, r0.88, P0.005; x4, y31,
z41, r0.75, P0.005), suggesting
that dorsal ACC activation during ESE was
associated with emotional distress parallel-
ing previous studies of physical pain (7,8).
The anterior insula (x42, y16, z1)
was also active in this comparison (t
4.07, r0.78, P0.005); however, it was
not associated with self-reported distress.
Two regions of RVPFC were more ac-
tive during ESE than during inclusion (Fig.
1B) (x42, y27, z11, t4.26, r
0.79, P0.005; x37, y50, z1, t
4.96, r0.83, P0.005). Self-reported
distress was negatively correlated with
RVPFC activity during ESE, relative to
inclusion (Fig. 2B) (x30, y34, z3,
r0.68, P0.005). Additionally,
RVPFC activation (x34, y36, z3)
was negatively correlated with ACC activ-
ity (x6, y8, z45) during ESE,
relative to inclusion (r0.81, P0.005)
(Fig. 2C), suggesting that RVPFC may play
a self-regulatory role in mitigating the dis-
tressing effects of social exclusion.
ACC activity (x6, y8, z45)
mediated the direct path from RVPFC (x
34, y36, z3) to distress (Sobel test,
Z3.16, P0.005). After controlling for
ACC activity, the remaining path from
RVPFC to distress was no longer signifi-
cant (␤⫽0.17, P0.5). This mediation-
al model is nearly identical to the results
from previous research on the self-regula-
tion of physical pain (16).
ISE, relative to inclusion, also produced
significant activation of ACC (x6, y
21, z41; (z41, t4.34, I 0.78, P
0.005). To preserve the cover story, self-
reported distress was not assessed after this
condition, and thus we could not assess the
relation between ACC activity during ISE
and perceived distress. However, no
RVPFC activity was found in this compar-
ison, even at a P.05 significance level,
suggesting that the ACC registered this ISE
but did not generate a self-regula-
tory response.
In summary, a pattern of activations very
similar to those found in studies of physical
pain emerged during social exclusion, pro-
viding evidence that the experience and reg-
ulation of social and physical pain share a
common neuroanatomical basis. Activity in
dorsal ACC, previously linked to the experi-
ence of pain distress, was associated with
increased distress after social exclusion. Fur-
thermore, activity in RVPFC, previously
linked to the regulation of pain distress, was
associated with diminished distress after so-
cial exclusion.
The neural correlates of social pain were
also activated by the mere visual appear-
Fig. 1. (A) Increased
activity in anterior cin-
gulate cortex (ACC)
during exclusion rela-
tive to inclusion. (B) In-
creased activity in
right ventral prefron-
tal cortex (RVPFC)
during exclusion rela-
tive to inclusion.
REPORTS
www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 302 10 OCTOBER 2003 291
on September 30, 2008 www.sciencemag.orgDownloaded from
ance of exclusion in the absence of actual
exclusion. The pattern of neural activity
associated with ISE and ESE provides
some challenges to the way we currently
understand exclusion and its consequences.
Although the neural correlates of distress
were observed in both ISE and ESE, the
self-regulation of this distress only oc-
curred in response to ESE. Explicit aware-
ness of exclusion may be required before
individuals can make appropriate attribu-
tions and regulate the associated distress.
Dorsal ACC activation during ESE
could reflect enhanced attentional process-
ing, previously associated with ACC activ-
ity (4,5), rather than an underlying distress
due to exclusion. Two pieces of evidence
make this possibility unlikely. First, ACC
activity was strongly correlated with per-
ceived distress after exclusion, indicating
that the ACC activity was associated with
changes in participantsself-reported feel-
ing states. Second, although inclusion is
likely to require greater attentional process-
ing than does ISE to facilitate participation
in the game, there was greater ACC activity
during ISE than during inclusion, indicat-
ing that the ACC activity was not fully
attributable to heightened attention.
Because of the need to maintain a realistic
situation in which participants would genu-
inely feel excluded, the study did not contain
some of the controls typical of most neuro-
imaging studies. For instance, the conditions
were always implemented in the same order
so as to keep expectations consistent from
one scan to the next across participants. It
was especially critical that ESE came last to
prevent expectations of possible exclusion
from contaminating the other conditions.
There was only a single ESE period to pre-
serve ecological validity. This modification,
however, diminishes, rather than increases,
the likelihood of Type I errors.
This study suggests that social pain is
analogous in its neurocognitive function to
physical pain, alerting us when we have
sustained injury to our social connections,
allowing restorative measures to be taken.
Understanding the underlying commonalities
between physical and social pain unearths
new perspectives on issues such as why phys-
ical and social pain are affected similarly by
both social support and neurochemical inter-
ventions (2,3,25), and why it hurtsto lose
someone we love (1).
References and Notes
1. M. R. Leary, C. A. Springer, in Behaving Badly: Aversive
Behaviors in Interpersonal Relationships, R. Kowalski,
Ed. (American Psychological Association, Washing-
ton, DC, 2001), pp. 151–175.
2. J. Panksepp, Affective Neuroscience (Oxford Univer-
sity Press, New York, 1998).
3. E. Nelson, J. Panksepp, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 22,
437 (1998).
4. G. Bush, P. Luu, M. I. Posner, Trends Cognit. Sci. 4, 215
(2000).
5. C. S. Carter et al.,Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97,
1944 (2000).
6. T. A. Kimbrell et al.,Biol. Psychol. 46, 454 (1999).
7. P. Rainville, G. H. Duncan, D. D. Price, B. Carrier, M. C.
Bushnell, Science 277, 968 (1997).
8. N. Sawamoto et al.,J. Neurosci. 20, 7438 (2000).
9. E. L. Foltz, E. W. White, J. Neurosurg. 19, 89 (1962).
10. M. R. Murphy, P. D. MacLean, S. C. Hamilton, Science
213, 459 (1981).
11. P. D. MacLean, J. D. Newman, Brain Res. 450, 111
(1988).
12. J. P. Lorberbaum et al.,Depress. Anxiety 10,99
(1999).
13. P. Petrovic, E. Kalso, K. M. Petersson, M. Ingvar,
Science 295, 1737 (2002).
14. A. R. Hariri, S. Y. Bookheimer, J. C. Mazziotta, Neuro-
report 11, 43 (2000).
15. D. M. Small, R. J. Zatorre, A. Dagher, A. C. Evans, M.
Jones-Gotman, Brain 124, 1720 (2001).
16. M. D. Lieberman et al., in preparation.
17. S. T. Carmichael, J. L. Price, J. Comp. Neurol. 363, 615
(1995).
18. C. Cavada, T. Company, J. Tejedor, R. J. Cruz-Rizzolo,
F. Reinoso-Suarez, Cerebr. Cort. 10, 220 (2000).
19. Y. Q. Zhang, J. S. Tang, B. Yuan, H. Jia, Pain 72, 127
(1997).
20. L. Zadro, K. D. Williams, reported in K. D. Williams,
T. I. Case, C. L. Govan, in Responding to the Social
World: Implicit and Explicit Processes in Social Judg-
ments and Decisions, J. Forgas, K. Williams, W. von
Hippel, Eds. (Cambridge University Press, New York,
in press).
21. K. D. Williams, C. Cheung, W. Choi, J. Personal. Soc.
Psychol. 79, 748 (2000).
22. Materials and methods are available as supportive
material on Science Online.
23. The correction for multiple comparisons was carried
out with an uncorrected Pvalue of 0.005 and a
cluster size threshold of 10, corresponding to a per-
voxel false positive probability of less than 0.000001
(24).
24. S. D. Forman et al.,Magn. Reson. Med. 33, 636
(1995).
25. J. L. Brown, D. Sheffield, M. R. Leary, M. E. Robinson,
Psychosom. Med. 65, 276 (2003).
26. We thank A. Satpute and the staff of the University
of California, Los Angeles, Brain Mapping Center
for their assistance. This research was funded by a
grant from the National Institutes of Mental
Health (R21MH66709-01) to M.D.L.
Supporting Online Material
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/302/5643/290/
DC1
Materials and Methods
14 July 2003; accepted 15 August 2003
Fig. 2. Scatterplots showing the
relation during exclusion, rela-
tive to inclusion, between (A)
ACC activity and self-reported
distress, (B) RVPFC and self-
reported distress, and (C) ACC
and RVPFC activity. Each point
represents the data from a single
participant.
REPORTS
10 OCTOBER 2003 VOL 302 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org292
on September 30, 2008 www.sciencemag.orgDownloaded from
... Several paradigms have been developed to study social pain using fMRI, each providing unique insights into its neural underpinnings. Examples include trust games [1], social feedback paradigms [2,3], rejection simulations [4,5], and passive exposure to loss [6]. ...
... In the third and final phase, the participant is unexpectedly excluded from play from the players (exclusion condition). This paradigm became notorious after Eisenberger et al. [5] showed in a functional imaging study the activation of the dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC, an area associated with pain in previous studies, [8,9]). This finding led to the proposal of the "social pain" hypothesis, which suggests that social and physical pain share common neural mechanisms/activations [5,10]. ...
... This paradigm became notorious after Eisenberger et al. [5] showed in a functional imaging study the activation of the dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC, an area associated with pain in previous studies, [8,9]). This finding led to the proposal of the "social pain" hypothesis, which suggests that social and physical pain share common neural mechanisms/activations [5,10]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background/Objectives: The cyberball paradigm has been used in numerous neuroimaging studies to elicit activation in neural substrates of social exclusion, which have been interpreted in terms of activity associated with “social pain”. The objectives of the study were to assess not only the replicability but also the specificity of the areas activated by this paradigm. Methods: Functional imaging with arterial spin labeling, an approach to image longer mental states. Results: We replicated findings of previous meta-analyses of this paradigm in the inferior frontal gyrus and ventral cingular cortex. However, these areas were also active in a watch condition (in which participants were not excluded), although less so. Conclusions: These findings relativize a simple and specific interpretation of these areas as the neural substrates of social exclusion and social pain, as in previous studies. In a broader experimental context, similar activations have been reported by neuroimaging studies when semantic disambiguation and evaluation of action goals are required, an interpretation that may also apply to the effects elicited by this paradigm.
... In line with this, chronically rejected adolescents show increased neural activation in the anterior cingulate cortex during ostracism (e.g., , an area that is also activated during other experiences that threaten belonging (e.g., Eisenberger, 2012;Eisenberger et al., 2003), indicating more threat perception. ...
... Ostracism may be a specific, yet more subtle, form of discrimination against sexual minorities (e.g., DeSouza et al., 2017). Even though targets of ostracism are not physically harmed, ostracism causes the affected individuals a tremendous amount of psychological pain that is neurologically similar to experiencing physical pain (e.g., Eisenberger, 2012;Eisenberger et al., 2003). As a consequence, ostracism bears immense individual and societal costs such as lowering well-being, reducing productivity, and increasing burnout, mental illness, and suicidality (e.g., Chen et al., 2020;Qian et al., 2019;Rudert et al., 2021). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This thesis leverages experience sampling as an innovative approach to investigate real-life experiences of ostracism (i.e., being ignored and excluded), filling gaps left by traditional survey and experimental approaches. In three projects, I explore the frequency, behavioral responses, risk factors, and perceived reasons for everyday ostracism. First, I discuss how Büttner, Ren et al. (2024) use event-contingent and time-contingent experience sampling to quantify ostracism frequency in daily life. Further, this project investigates the behaviors that follow ostracism. Büttner, Ren et al. (2024) propose and find support for a framework suggesting that the severity of need threat after ostracism influences whether individuals approach, avoid, or behave antisocially towards others. In the second project, Büttner, Rudert, & Kachel (2024) identify sexual minorities as a group that is particularly at risk for experiencing frequent ostracism. A complementing experiment reveals that the reason why sexual minorities frequently face ostracism is their deviation from gender-role expectations. Finally, Büttner & Greifeneder (2024) investigate how depression critically shapes targets’ frequency, experience, and attribution of ostracism. Depressed individuals not only experience more frequent everyday ostracism and exhibit heightened need threat responses; their depression also leads to maladaptive attributions of ostracism. Together, the three presented projects underscore the value of experience sampling in providing nuanced insights into the psychological impact of real-life ostracism. Traditional survey and experimental approaches alone are insufficient to capture the dynamics and the pervasive, immediate impact of ostracism in everyday life, but experience sampling bridges this gap. The presented projects not only deepen the empirical understanding of ostracism but also set a precedent for future research and practical applications to mitigate the effects of ostracism through targeted interventions.
... Although shared pathways in the brain for emotional and pain processing are not specifically connected to the idea of social pain, many studies have investigated the physical-social pain overlap [4]. The most important evidence is provided by neuroimaging studies, which identified the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the anterior insula as key areas for the physicalsocial pain overlap [6,7]. It should be noted, that the modern view of how the brain works is moving away from specific mind-brain aspects. ...
... The circuit network model of somatosensory describes enhanced neuronal pain processing activity in the ACC and the insular cortex [21]. This fact is particularly interesting because parts of the ACC and the insula play a key role in terms of the physical-social pain overlap [6]. Another noteworthy fact is that patients with somatoform pain disorder are frequently stigmatized and devaluated by their social environment [22,23]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives The overlapping neural networks of social and physical pain have been investigated intensively in recent years. It was postulated that triggering social pain might result in greater physical pain. Nonetheless, how this affects somatoform pain disorder has not yet been considered. Since an increased pain processing activity is reported in these patients, the investigation of social exclusion and its effect on this group seems interesting. Hence, the aim of this study was to compare the influence of social exclusion on healthy controls and patients with somatoform pain disorder. Methods Nineteen patients with somatoform pain disorder and 19 healthy controls were examined. Cyberball, a virtual ball-tossing paradigm, was used to experimentally induce social exclusion and inclusion. To measure effects on pain perception, pressure pain thresholds and heart rate variability (HRV) were recorded after each round of cyberball. Demographic data, pain medication, and potential psychosocial moderators were collected by questionnaires. Results After social exclusion, pressure pain thresholds were significantly reduced in healthy controls (p < 0.01) as well as somatoform pain patients (p < 0.05), while HRV increased only in patients with somatoform pain disorder (p < 0.05) indicating increased parasympathetic activity. Conclusion This study is the first to analyse the effects of social exclusion on pain perception in somatoform pain disorder. While the reduction in pressure pain thresholds is in line with the social pain literature, the effects on HRV could be interpreted as a form of pain regulation mechanism. However, further research is needed to investigate the role of parasympathetic activity in socially excluded somatoform pain patients.
... Paradigms involving social exclusion have been used to experimentally probe these processes. Early studies of social exclusion showed that ostracization leads to activations in the dACC, insula and mPFC 105 . These regions are also part of the acute threat circuit (Fig. 2a, left) and the 'pain matrix' (the network responsible for processing physical pain) 106 , which suggests that the brain may interpret social exclusion as a form of 'social pain', underscoring the aversive nature of ostracization. ...
Article
This study develops and employs a novel paradigm called “Moneyball” – a modification of the “Cyberball” framework with monetary incentives – to simultaneously examine the consequences of ostracism on both targets and sources in an interactive and deception-free environment ( N = 504 university students). Validating the paradigm, we observe comparable detrimental impacts on targets’ need satisfaction and mood to those observed in nonincentivized studies. Sources experienced some positive effects on belonging, meaningful existence, and control but suffered a decline in self-esteem and mood. Both sources and targets allocated less money to group members than inclusive participants, even when financial equality was maintained. Thus, Moneyball offers a viable method to examine voluntary social exclusion in a unified framework.
Article
Social rejection hurts, but it can also be informative: Through experiences of acceptance and rejection, people identify partners interested in connecting with them and choose which ties to cement or to sever. What is it that people actually learn from rejection? In social interactions, people can learn from two kinds of information. First, people generally learn from rewarding outcomes, which may include concrete opportunities for interaction. Second, people track the “relational value” others ascribe to them—an internal model of how much others value them. Here, we used computational neuroimaging to dissociate these forms of learning. Participants repeatedly tried to match with others in a social game. Feedback revealed whether they successfully matched (a rewarding outcome) and how much the other person wanted to play with them (relational value). A Bayesian cognitive model revealed that participants chose partners who provided rewarding outcomes and partners who valued them. Whereas learning from outcomes was linked to brain regions involved in reward-based reinforcement, learning about relational value was linked to brain regions previously associated with social rejection. These findings identify precise computations underlying brain responses to rejection and support a neurocomputational model of social affiliation in which people build an internal model of relational value and learn from rewarding outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
Three studies (N = 860) examined whether pleasure of revenge predicts both vengeful and benevolent motivations toward the provocateur. Across all studies, self-reported positive affect was higher, whereas self-reported negative affect was lower after an instance of revenge. Furthermore, the revenge-related positive affect predicted greater subsequent vengeful and benevolent motivations toward the provocateur (but more consistently the former). These findings replicate and extend previous research on affect-improving qualities of revenge. They allow for a more nuanced understanding of the reinforcing nature of vengeful pleasure. Moreover, they suggest that the pleasure of revenge might be harnessed to promote forgiveness.
Article
Full-text available
Ostracism is such a widely used and powerful tactic that the authors tested whether people would be affected by it even under remote and artificial circumstances. In Study 1, 1,486 participants from 62 countries accessed the authors' on-line experiment on the Internet. They were asked to use mental visualization while playing a virtual tossing game with two others (who were actually computer generated and controlled). Despite the minimal nature of their experience, the more participants were ostracized, the more they reported feeling bad, having less control, and losing a sense of belonging. In Study 2, ostracized participants were more likely to conform on a subsequent task. The results are discussed in terms of supporting K. D. Williams's (1997) need threat theory of ostracism. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2009 APA, all rights reserved). (from the journal abstract)
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies have shown that the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex (OMPFC) is extensively connected with medial temporal and cingulate limbic structures. In this study, the organization of these projections was defined in relation to architectonic areas within the OMPFC. All of the limbic structures were substantially connected with the following posterior and medial orbital areas: the posteromedial, medial, intermediate, and lateral agranular insular areas (Iapm, Iam, Iai, and Ial, respectively) and areas 11m, 13a, 13b, 14c, and 14r. In contrast, lateral orbital areas 12o, 12m, and [12] and medial wall areas 24a, b and 32 were primarily connected with the amygdala, the temporal pole, and the cingulate cortex. Data were not obtained on the poateroventral medial wall. Three distinct projections were recognized from the basal amygdaloid nucleus: (1) The dorsal part projected to area 121; (2) the ventromedial part projected to most areas in the posterior and medial orbital cortex except for areas Iai, 12o, 13a, and 14c; and (3) the ventrolateral part projected to orbital areas 12o, Iai, 13a, 14c, and to the medial wall areas. The accessory basal and lateral amygdaloid nuclei projected most strongly to areas in the posterior and medial orbital cortex. The medial, anterior cortical, and central amygdaloid nuclei and the periamygdaloid cortex were connected with the posterior orbital areas. The projection from the hippocampus originated from the rostral subiculum and terminated in the medial orbital areas. The same region was reciprocally connected with the anterornedial nucleus of the thalamus, which received input from the rostral subiculum. The parahippocampal cortical areas (including the temporal polar, entorhinal, perirhinal, and posterior parahippocampal cortices) were primarily connected with posterior and medial orbital areas, with some projections to the dorsal parr of the medial wall. The rostral cingulate cortex sent fibers to the medial wall, to the medial orbital areas, and to lateral areas 12o, 12r, and Iai. The posterior cingulate gyrus, including the caudomedial lobule, was especially strongly connected with area 11 m. © 1995 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.
Book
Some investigators have argued that emotions, especially animal emotions, are illusory concepts outside the realm of scientific inquiry. With advances in neurobiology and neuroscience, however, researchers are proving this position wrong while moving closer to understanding the biology and psychology of emotion. In Affective Neuroscience, Jaak Panksepp argues that emotional systems in humans, as well as other animals, are necessarily combinations of innate and learned tendencies; there are no routine and credible ways to really separate the influences of nature and nurture in the control of behavior. The book shows how to move toward a new understanding by taking a psychobiological approach to the subject, examining how the neurobiology and neurochemistry of the mammalian brain shape the psychological experience of emotion. It includes chapters on sleep and arousal, pleasure and pain systems, the sources of rage and anger, and the neural control of sexuality. The book will appeal to researchers and professors in the field of emotion.
Article
Abundant proteins found within the spore core (the small, acid-soluble proteins, or SASP) as well as surrounding the spore (the coat proteins) help to protect the spore from these assaults such as extremes of temperature, radiation, desiccation, and attack by a wide variety of toxic molecules. The SASP and coat proteins have been studied for many years, and much is known about their roles in resistance. The predominant proteins of the spore core are the SASP, making up as much as 20% of total spore protein. YrbB, which has been localized to the cortex and to the inner coat, and SspG , which is synthesized in the mother cell, may also be coat proteins. Measuring the effects of the loss of a single protein might require highly sensitive and specific assays. Given their unique roles in spore dormancy and survival, it will be of great interest to compare SASP and coat protein genes of the various spore-forming bacteria as their genome sequences become available. At the moment, relatively little information is available, making a detailed comparative analysis difficult. Homologues of CotE are encoded in three of the genomes of endospore-forming bacteria (Bacillus anthracis, B. stearothermophilus, B. subtilis) for which data are available, and homologues of SpoIVA are encoded by these genomes as well as those of two Clostridium species (C. acetobutylicum and C. difficile). Therefore, it is tempting to speculate that the formation of a basement layer by SpoIVA is a universal early step in coat assembly.
Article
Examines the dark side of relating, an inevitable part of interacting with others. How is it that we need others so much, indeed rely on them for our survival and well-being, yet often find it so difficult to maintain satisfying relationships? How can the loved one who raises your spirits by leaving flowers for you one day be the same individual who the next day acts like an insensitive jerk? Relationships provide us with meaning and psychological well-being, but are the source of many, perhaps most, of life's greatest frustrations. Some the most commonly experienced aversive phenomena are explored in this book, including teasing, swearing, gossip, and betrayal. Rich in research and vivid examples, the chapters of this volume explore these behaviors through the eyes of both victims and perpetrators, often revealing the hidden benefits of aversive behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Provides an overview of what is currently known about hurt feelings and speculates regarding its causes and functions. The chapter begins with an examination of the status of hurt feelings as an emotion. Given that the emotions of hurt feelings have not been widely studied and do not appear in most taxonomies of emotion, the authors have only partial answers regarding the characteristics of hurt feelings and how they relate to other emotions. After discussing the features of hurt feelings, the authors offer a theoretical perspective that attempts to explain why people's feelings are hurt, and then they review the sparse empirical findings that bear on this theory. Common behavioral reactions to being hurt are introduced, followed by a discussion of why people hurt one another's feelings in the 1st place. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The typical functional magnetic resonance (fMRI) study presents a formidable problem of multiple statistical comparisons (i.e, > 10,000 in a 128 x 128 image). To protect against false positives, investigators have typically relied on decreasing the per pixel false positive probability. This approach incurs an inevitable loss of power to detect statistically significant activity. An alternative approach, which relies on the assumption that areas of true neural activity will tend to stimulate signal changes over contiguous pixels, is presented. If one knows the probability distribution of such cluster sizes as a function of per pixel false positive probability, one can use cluster-size thresholds independently to reject false positives. Both Monte Carlo simulations and fMRI studies of human subjects have been used to verify that this approach can improve statistical power by as much as fivefold over techniques that rely solely on adjusting per pixel false positive probabilities.
Article
Since the separation of mammals serves to maintain (1) mother-offspring contanct and (2) contact between members of a group, it probably ranks as a basic mammalian vocalization. The present study is part of an investigation concerned with identifying the cerebral representation of the separation call in squirrel monkeys. For this purpose, monkeys are tested for their ability to produce spontaneous calls in isolation before and after ablations of different parts of the brain. Because of the subject's auditory and visual isolation, the call emitted during testing is referred to as the isolation call. In a preceding study, it was shown that lesions at the thalamomidbrain junction and in the ventral central gray interfere with the structure and/or production of the call. The present study focuses on the rostral midline limbic cortex, known to be one of the two cortical areas where stimulation elicits vocalization in monkeys. Evidence derived by the process of elimination indicates that the spontaneous call depends on the concerted action of a continuous band of rostral limbic cortex comprising parts of areas 24, 25, and 12. The midline frontal neocortex peripheral to this limbic zone does not appear to be essential for the call.
Article
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of California, Los Angeles, 2001. Typescript (photocopy). Vita. Includes bibliographical references.
Article
Hamsters deprived from birth of the neocortex developed normally and displayed the usual hamster-typical behavioral patterns. With the additional concurrent destruction of midline limbic convolutions (cingulate and underlying dorsal hippocampal), there were deficits in maternal behavior and a lack of development of play behavior. These findings demonstrate in a rodent (i) that the striatal complex and limbic system, along with the remaining neuraxis, are sufficient for giving expression to a wide range of unlearned forms of species-typical behavior and (ii) that midline limbic structures are required for the expression of play behavior and the integrated performance of maternal behavior.