ArticlePublisher preview available

Complementary Justice: Effects of "Poor but Happy" and "Poor but Honest" Stereotype Exemplars on System Justification and Implicit Activation of the Justice Motive

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

It was hypothesized that exposure to complementary representations of the poor as happier and more honest than the rich would lead to increased support for the status quo. In Study 1, exposure to "poor but happy" and "rich but miserable" stereotype exemplars led people to score higher on a general measure of system justification, compared with people who were exposed to noncomplementary exemplars. Study 2 replicated this effect with "poor but honest" and "rich but dishonest" complementary stereotypes. In Studies 3 and 4, exposure to noncomplementary stereotype exemplars implicitly activated justice concerns, as indicated by faster reaction times to justice-related than neutral words in a lexical decision task. Evidence also suggested that the Protestant work ethic may moderate the effects of stereotype exposure on explicit system justification (but not implicit activation).
Complementary Justice: Effects of “Poor but Happy” and “Poor but
Honest” Stereotype Exemplars on System Justification and Implicit
Activation of the Justice Motive
Aaron C. Kay and John T. Jost
Stanford University
It was hypothesized that exposure to complementary representations of the poor as happier and more
honest than the rich would lead to increased support for the status quo. In Study 1, exposure to “poor but
happy” and “rich but miserable” stereotype exemplars led people to score higher on a general measure
of system justification, compared with people who were exposed to noncomplementary exemplars. Study
2 replicated this effect with “poor but honest” and “rich but dishonest” complementary stereotypes. In
Studies 3 and 4, exposure to noncomplementary stereotype exemplars implicitly activated justice
concerns, as indicated by faster reaction times to justice-related than neutral words in a lexical decision
task. Evidence also suggested that the Protestant work ethic may moderate the effects of stereotype
exposure on explicit system justification (but not implicit activation).
Lower income and status is more tolerable when one can believe that
the rich are not receiving a happiness income commensurate with their
money income. (Robert E. Lane, 1959, pp. 3940)
It is virtually a cliche´ in our culture to consider the poverty-stricken,
or even the relatively deprived, as having their own compensating
rewards. They are actually happy in their own way—carefree, happy-
go-lucky, in touch with and able to enjoy the “simple pleasures of
life”. . . Some systems of religious belief see virtue in suffering, and
assume restitution in later life. (Melvin Lerner, 1980, pp. 20–21)
Cultural depictions of the rich and poor in numerous works of
literature, religion, and the mass media reflect a leveling tendency
to ascribe virtues such as happiness and morality to the underpriv-
ileged and, conversely, vices such as misery, loneliness, and dis-
honesty to those who are blessed with material abundance. Cele-
brated novels, plays, and films that reinforce such complementary,
offsetting stereotypes in which each group possesses its unique
benefits and burdens include Dickens’ Great Expectations, Mo-
liere’s The Miser, Fitzgerald’s The Great Gatsby, Orson Welles’s
Citizen Kane, Herman Hesse’s Siddhartha, and even Steve Mar-
tin’s The Jerk. The sentimental fiction movement that dominated
British literature of the 18th century churned out best sellers in
which peasants were portrayed as relentlessly joyful and virtuous
(e.g., Mackenzie, 1771/1967). An impoverished old man in
Sterne’s (1768) A Sentimental Journey Through France and Italy,
for instance, declares that “a chearful [sic] and contented mind was
the best sort of thanks to heaven that an illiterate peasant could
pay” (p. 120). This literary form, which sometimes parodied its
readers for their romanticization of the poor, culminated with
Charles Dickens. In one of his best-known examples, Dickens
(1843/1971) contrasts the rich, miserable Ebenezer Scrooge with
the insolvent but cheery Cratchit family in A Christmas Carol:
They were not a handsome family; they were not well dressed; their
shoes were far from being water proof; their clothes were scanty and
Peter might have known, and very likely did, the inside of a pawn-
broker’s. But they were happy, grateful, pleased with one another and
contented with the time. (p. 99)
It has been a familiar cultural theme for centuries, then, that
poverty has its rewards and affluence its drawbacks. These specific
characterizations may be conspicuously Western, but general pref-
erences to perceive balance and complementarity in the social
world are present also in Taoist notions, most especially the
dialectical relationship between elements of yin and yang and the
“reconciliation of opposites.”
Public opinion is another carrier of popular culture that often
assumes that there is “an inverse relationship between satisfaction
and standard of living” (Hunyady, 1998, p. 85). Theorists have
occasionally speculated about the social and psychological func-
tions of “poor but happy,” “rich but miserable,” “poor but honest,”
and “rich but dishonest” stereotypes (Jost, Burgess, & Mosso,
2001; Lane, 1959; Lerner, 1980). A provocative suggestion that
Aaron C. Kay, Department of Psychology, Stanford University; John T.
Jost, Graduate School of Business, Stanford University.
Portions of this research were presented at the following conferences:
International Society for Justice Research, Sko¨vde, Sweden, June 2002;
Person Memory Interest Group, Salt Fork, Ohio, October 2002; and the
Nags Head Conference on the Justice Motive, Highland Beach, Florida,
December 2002. This work was supported by the Graduate School of
Business at Stanford University and the Radcliffe Institute for Advanced
Study at Harvard University. We acknowledge Susan Andersen, Mahzarin
Banaji, Monica Biernat, Ramona Bobocel, Ted Coons, Grainne Fitzsi-
mons, Peter Glick, Carolyn Hafer, E. Tory Higgins, Orsolya Hunyady, Ian
Kay, Rod Kramer, Arie Kruglanski, Mel Lerner, Hazel Markus, Lee Ross,
Hal Sigall, Jim Sherman, Claude Steele, and Tom Tyler for extremely
helpful suggestions concerning the ideas contained in this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to either
Aaron C. Kay, Department of Psychology, Jordan Hall, Building 420,
Stanford University, Stanford, California 94305, or John T. Jost, who is
now at the Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washing-
ton Place, New York, New York 10003-6634. E-mail: aaronk@
psych.stanford.edu or john.jost@nyu.edu
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
2003, Vol. 85, No. 5, 823–837 0022-3514/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.823
823
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
... One such stimulus is exposure to rationalizing or legitimizing myths-cultural narratives that justify the status quo between an ingroup and outgroup(s). These myths, which include the Protestant Work Ethnic and the American Dream, provide elaborate explanations for the unequal stratification of racial groups in the United States (Sidanius & Pratto, 2000; see also Jost, 2020;Jost, Federico, & Napier, 2013;Kay & Jost, 2003), with exposure to these myths often catalyzing system justification's effects (Jost 2020). For example, find that affirming the myth of the American Dream heightens system justification levels among Latino adults, which then steers them toward greater support for nativist policies that harm some of their co-ethnics. ...
... After completing a factual manipulation check (true/false) about the content of one's assigned article, all respondents completed three (3) items from a previously validated 8-item scale of system justification levels (Kay & Jost, 2003). Specifically, Asian American participants used a scale from 1-strongly disagree to 7-strongly agree to complete the following statements: a) "In general, I find society to be fair"; b) "Society is set up so that people usually get what they deserve"; and c) "Everyone has a fair shot at wealth and happiness." ...
... The minimum sample size to detect an effect of this size at this more relaxed alpha level is N=620. 5 We only administered three (3) of the original eight (8) items in Kay and Jost (2003) to minimize respondent fatigue. 6 In the long run, system justification is associated with depression and maladjustment among disadvantaged individuals because they internalize their alleged inferiority (Jost 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Why is solidarity between people of color (PoC) so difficult to achieve? New evidence suggests solidarity can be activated through a sense of shared discrimination between PoC. Yet other research highlights many real-world obstacles to this solidarity, including recurring inter-minority conflicts. We consider system justification as one possible mechanism that undercuts PoC solidarity. System justification is a human motive to bolster the status quo. System justifiers who are PoC condone racial inequalities as stable, predictable, and just—which alleviates mental stressors associated with their own racially stigmatized status. We investigate system justification ’s impacts on Asian Americans: a key party to many coalitions and conflicts with Black and Latino people. Using national survey data, we find that system justification is significantly associated with Asian opposition to solidarity with Black Lives Matter, net of racial resentment, and other key covariates. We then refine this result experimentally by exposing Asian adults to the model minority myth—a system-legitimizing ideology. Exposure to this myth triggers system justification , which then increases Asian opposition to pro-Black and pro-Latino policies, among other solidarity-based outcomes. Both results are primarily driven by conservative Asian Americans, highlighting a need to better appreciate Asian Americans’ ideological diversity in U.S. racial politics.
... To measure system rejection tendencies, participants answered the System Justification Scale (Kay & Jost, 2003) involving 8 statements (e.g., "In general, I find society to be fair"). Items were answered on a 9-point scale from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 9 (Strongly Agree), were reverse-coded so that higher scores indicate feelings of greater system rejection, and averaged into a scale (Cronbach's α = .83). 12 ...
... Other manipulations to examine experiences with police injustice (such as an accumulation of injustices; see De Graaf, 2011;Della Porta, 1995), or other systemic forms of injustice could be examined in future research (see Jansma et al., 2024b). We also note that we measured protesters' rejection of the prevailing societal system by reverse-coding the System Justification Scale developed by Kay and Jost (2003), such that higher scores indicated greater tendencies to reject the system. After all, lower scores on the scale typically suggest a potential rejection of the status quo (Liekefett & Becker, 2022; see also Jost et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Based on significance-quest theory and research on procedural justice, we propose that climate protesters’ support for ecotage (i.e., the tactic of property damage to prevent environmental harm), is affected by their need for significance and perceived police injustice. To test this assumption, we surveyed climate protesters in the United States (Study 1, N = 253) and the Netherlands (Study 2, N = 333). In these studies, we manipulated whether participants were reminded about experiences of unfair police treatment. We measured protesters’ support for climate actions involving property damage, such as arson, slashing SUV tires, and sabotaging pipelines. Both studies showed that need for significance was positively related to support for ecotage when protesters were exposed to police injustice, but not in daily life situations, providing evidence for the situational activation of significance quest. Furthermore, we found that the more protesters perceived unfair police treat­ment, the more they experienced feelings of personal humiliation and disrespect, which was related to greater support for damaging climate actions.
... System justification was measured using four items from Kay and Jost (2003). Items were rated on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) scale. ...
Article
Full-text available
Research warns of a tokenistic combination of two colonial ideologies that recognises Indigenous culture as part of the nation's identity (low symbolic exclusion) yet denies the relevance of colonisation to contemporary inequities (high historical negation). Because symbolic exclusion and historical negation respectively reinforce symbolic and material inequalities, this Moral Credentialer profile may mask intolerance with superficial support for equality. Using latent profile analysis, we investigate the presence of this asymmetric response pattern among a nationwide random sample of Pākehā (New Zealand Europeans; N = 13,877) and Māori (N = 2004). As expected, we identify heterogeneity among both ethnic groups, yet find a Moral Credentialer profile (high historical negation and low symbolic exclusion) among only Pākehā. Sociopolitical attitudes predicted profile membership, which, in turn, predicted bicultural political attitudes. Results thus corroborate a tokenistic subgroup of settler colonisers who endorse less costly forms of symbolic equality whilst upholding material inequities.
Article
Research on (un)ethical behavior in organizations has a long tradition, yet the role of individual morality in career development remains underexamined. Insights from both career and behavioral ethics research can help address this issue. While career research so far lacks systematic investigation into the morality-career relationship, behavioral ethics research overlooks the developmental and career-related outcomes of individual morality. This points to a critical need for dialogue between these two fields. An initial analysis of core theories and concepts in career research reveals the scant inclusion of moral variables. Through our systematic review of a final sample of 43 articles, we identify gaps in understanding the interplay between morality and career dynamics, particularly concerning career options (including decisions and opportunities) and both subjective and objective career success. By leveraging the specific insights extracted from the literature review, our aim is to bridge the morality and career research domains. We highlight discrepancies and untapped areas in this field, proposing future research directions concerning (1) the role of morality in career decisions and opportunities, (2) the nexus between morality and career success, and (3) employees' implicit beliefs of this relationship. Furthermore, we consider implications for measurement and career counseling. Altogether, we advocate for a fresh research focus on exploring the impact of individual morality on career development.
Article
Full-text available
Prosocial behavior research is usually based on emotional and cognitive approaches that shed light on factors that facilitate the readiness to help. Encountering groups that need help, e.g., socially disadvantaged, might cause dissonance among people who perceive themselves as part of the social system that induced such problems. To deal with this conflict, people might endorse system-justifying beliefs that might affect the readiness to help. The aim of the research was to test the Expanded Dual Chamber Model of Collective Action that incorporates system-justifying beliefs as the moral dimension of the model and personal responsibility as an additional key predictor of prosocial collective actions (e.g., donations, signing petitions, conversations, social movements) to help socially disadvantaged groups, namely, men suffering from domestic violence, the unemployed, migrants, people with physical disabilities, alcohol addiction, mental disorders, and drug addiction. In total, 2029 individuals (962 men and 1067 women) took part in online surveys, completing questionnaires on system-justifying beliefs, identity of a helping group, anger, personal and collective efficacy, personal responsibility, and intentions to participate in prosocial collective action. The results of partial least squares structural equation modeling demonstrated that identity enhances the intentions to participate in prosocial collective action, whereas the contributions of responsibility and efficacy differentiated across target groups. System-justifying beliefs directly mitigated only the intention to help men suffering from domestic violence but were positively indirectly associated with helping other social groups through efficacy and responsibility. The findings are discussed in line with social identity models of collective action and system justification theory.
Article
In the news media, fiction, and conversations we are often presented with utopic and dystopic versions of the future related to the environment or technology. In this research, we ask how thinking about ecological or technological utopias—or dystopias—as potential future societies alters one’s motivation to change and justification of one’s current society. In Study 1a ( n = 121) and 1b ( n = 174), thinking about ecological, but not technological, utopias enhanced motivation to change one’s current society, whereas neither utopia changed justification with one’ current society. Study 2 ( n = 196), which included ecological and technological dystopia conditions, showed that ecological and technological utopias and dystopias increased motivation to change one’s current society, whereas none of them changed justification with one’s current society. Moreover, thinking about an ecological utopia showed added effects: compared to the technological utopia, it was more positively evaluated, functioned more to bring about change, and further increased motivation to change one’s current society. All together these results show that thinking about possible future societies, especially ecological utopias, is a powerful way to motivate change in our present time.
Article
Socioeconomic status has long been understood as a key factor in individuals’ attitudes, cognition, and well-being. However, less attention has been given to the determinants of subjective socioeconomic status with consideration for the disparity between objective socioeconomic status and subjective socioeconomic status. Drawing on a nation-wide survey in China, this study explores how authoritarianism, system justification, and subjective well-being impact the formation of individuals’ subjective socioeconomic status. Results indicate that system justification and subjective well-being positively affect people’s perception of social identity, while authoritarianism adversely influences this perception. In addition, Internet use is found to significantly inhibit the effects of authoritarianism and subjective well-being on subjective socioeconomic status, which mitigates Chinese people’s cognitive bias between objective socioeconomic status and subjective socioeconomic status. The research findings shed light on the relationship between political ideology, well-being, Internet use, and subjective socioeconomic status, as well as contribute to ongoing theoretical debates about how Internet use could influence the public positively.
Article
Full-text available
Three studies examine the effects of stress on the accessibility of proximity-related thoughts. In all the studies, participants reported on their attachment style, and the accessibility of proximity themes and worries in a lexical decision task was assessed upon the priming of a stress or neutral word. In Study 2, the primed stress word was semantically related to attachment themes. In Study 3, lexical decisions were made under low or high cognitive load conditions. Overall, the priming of a stress word led to increased accessibility of proximity themes, regardless of attachment style. Anxious–ambivalent people also showed high accessibility to proximity themes and worries in both neutral and stress contexts. In most conditions, avoidant persons' reactions were similar to those of secure persons. However, they showed no accessibility to proximity worries even after the priming of a semantically related word and reacted with high accessibility to these worries upon the addition of cognitive load.
Article
Full-text available
Prejudice against fat people was compared with symbolic racism. An anti-fat attitudes questionnaire was developed and used in several studies testing the notion that antipathy toward fat people is part of an “ideology of blame.” Three commonalities between antifat attitudes and racism were explored: (a) the association between values, beliefs, and the rejection of a stigmatized group, (b) the old-fashioned antipathy toward deviance of many sorts, and (c) the lack of self-interest in out-group antipathy. Parallels were found on all 3 dimensions. No in-group bias was shown by fat people. Fatism appears to behave much like symbolic racism, but with less of the negative social desirability of racism.
Chapter
This book contains essays in honour of Melvin J. Lerner, a pioneer in the psychological study of justice. The contributors to this volume are internationally renowned scholars from psychology, business, and law. They examine the role of justice motivation in a wide variety of contexts, including workplace violence, affirmative action programs, helping or harming innocent victims and how people react to their own fate. Contributors explore fundamental issues such as whether people's interest in justice is motivated by self-interest or a genuine concern for the welfare of others, when and why people feel a need to punish transgressors, how a concern for justice emerges during the development of societies and individuals, and the relation of justice motivation to moral motivation. How an understanding of justice motivation can contribute to the amelioration of major social problems is also examined.
Chapter
This book contains essays in honour of Melvin J. Lerner, a pioneer in the psychological study of justice. The contributors to this volume are internationally renowned scholars from psychology, business, and law. They examine the role of justice motivation in a wide variety of contexts, including workplace violence, affirmative action programs, helping or harming innocent victims and how people react to their own fate. Contributors explore fundamental issues such as whether people's interest in justice is motivated by self-interest or a genuine concern for the welfare of others, when and why people feel a need to punish transgressors, how a concern for justice emerges during the development of societies and individuals, and the relation of justice motivation to moral motivation. How an understanding of justice motivation can contribute to the amelioration of major social problems is also examined.
Book
Motivated by the desire to explain how Americans perceive and evaluate inequality and related programs and policies, the authors conducted a national survey of beliefs about social and economic inequality in America. Here they present the results of their research on the structure, determinants, and certain political and personal consequences of these beliefs. The presentations serve two major goals; to describe and explain the central features of Americans' images of inequality. Beliefs About Inequality begins with a focus on people's perceptions of the most basic elements of inequality: the availability of opportunity in society, the causes of economic achievements, and the benefits and costs of equality and inequality. The book's analysis of the public's beliefs on these key issues is based on fundamental theories of social psychology and lays the groundwork for understanding how Americans evaluate inequality-related policies. The authors discuss the ultimate determinants of beliefs and the implications of their findings for social policies related to inequality. They propose that attitudes toward economic inequality and related policy are influenced by three major aspects of the current American social, economic, and political environment: a stable "dominant ideology" about economic inequality; individuals' social and economic status; and specific beliefs and attitudes, often reflecting "social liberalism" shaped by recent political debates and events. © 1986 by James R. Kluegel and Eliot R. Smith. All rights reserved.